Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 176, Issues 2–3, 30 April 2010, Pages 213-218
Psychiatry Research

Hyper-priming in cannabis users: A naturalistic study of the effects of cannabis on semantic memory function

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.09.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Psychotic symptoms have theoretically been linked to semantic memory impairments in patients with schizophrenia. Little is known of the effects of cannabis, the world's most popular illicit drug, on semantic memory and whether they are linked to the psychotomimetic states elicited by the drug. Thirty-six cannabis users were tested whilst under the influence of cannabis. They were then tested again when not intoxicated and compared with 38 non-drug using controls. Semantic memory was assessed using a semantic priming task with a long and short stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) to differentiate automatic and controlled processing. Under the influence of cannabis, users showed increases in both automatic semantic priming and schizotypal symptoms compared with controls. When abstinent, cannabis users exhibited hyper-priming at long SOAs. Cannabis users did not differ from controls in either trait schizotypy or state schizotypy when not intoxicated. Acute cannabis use increases schizotypyal symptoms and may increase automatic semantic priming in recreational users of this drug. When drug-free, cannabis users did not differ from controls in schizotypy but did show hyper-priming at the long SOA. The acute increase in automatic semantic priming may be one factor contributing to the psychotomimetic effects of cannabis.

Introduction

Cannabis is the most widely used illegal recreational drug: amongst all recreational drugs its use is second only to that of alcohol. The British Crime Survey reported that amongst young people (aged 16–24), 30% of men and 18% of women said they had used cannabis during 2004/5 (British Crime Survey, 2006). There have been growing concerns recently over the consequences of long-term cannabis use for cognition and mental health, particularly in light of claims that the drug may induce psychosis in some susceptible individuals (e.g. Di Forti et al., 2007). Acute cannabis intoxication is known to produce memory impairments (for a review, see Ranganathan and D'Souza, 2006). There are many chemical compound constituents of the cannabis plant, the most psychoactively potent of which has been identified as Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). The memory-impairing effects of an acute dose of cannabis are thought to relate to the action of THC at the Cannabinoid 1 (CB1) receptor, which is found at particularly high densities in the hippocampus, cerebellum and basal ganglia. Evidence of memory deficits following long-term cannabis use is less convincing, with verbal memory deficits found in some studies (Dafters et al., 2004, Messinis et al., 14-3-2006, Block and Ghoneim, 1993) but not others (Pope et al., 2001, Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2005, Fried et al., 2005). Reasons that have been suggested to account for these discrepancies are time since abstinence, age of onset and extent of use (Di Forti et al., 2007).

Despite numerous investigations of the effect of acute and chronic cannabis administration on what is termed episodic memory, or ‘time-locked’ memory for events and occurrences, another area of memory has been largely neglected: semantic memory. Semantic memory refers to our memory for facts and general knowledge of the world, including memory for meanings and language. Semantic memory in cannabis users is particularly interesting as semantic memory impairments are suggested to underlie some of the symptoms observed in psychosis such as thought disorder (Spitzer et al., 1993). A recent study found no impairment in speed of semantic processing in cannabis users (Wadsworth et al., 2006), although some previous studies have found evidence for an impairment in category fluency, where participants are required to generate as many category exemplars as they can within a given interval (e.g. Messinis et al., 2006), which is suggestive of a semantic memory impairment.

A more valid method of assessing semantic processing than category fluency is the semantic priming paradigm, a lexical-decision task in which participants must respond to a target word that is preceded by a prime word that is either related to the target or not (Meyer and Schvanevedlt, 1971). Semantic priming is a facilitated by responding to concepts that are semantically related. When a prime word is related to the target word (e.g. chair, table), people generally respond faster than when they are unrelated (e.g. chair, fish), and this decrease in reaction time is the ‘priming effect’. The mechanistic explanation for this describes the semantic system as organised in a map-like network of nodes, representing concepts (Neely and Keefe, 1989). When one node is activated, spreading of activation occurs so that connected nodes are also activated. This spreading of activation facilitates faster responding to related nodes in the network.

Semantic priming is a popular methodology for investigating semantic memory as the standard priming task can be manipulated to investigate different semantic mechanisms. Stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA, the time between the onset of the prime and the target) manipulations are used to assess effects on either automatic or controlled processes. SOAs shorter than 250 ms are considered to tap into automatic, pre-attentional processing, whereas controlled processing, which is slower and thought to involve attention, is tapped by SOAs longer than 700 ms.

Recent claims have related cannabis use to psychosis, with implications that there may be a causal link between the two (Ferdinand et al., 2005, Fergusson et al., 2005). Disturbance in the organisation and processing of knowledge has long been considered a central feature of schizophrenia and, more broadly, psychosis (Bleuler, 1911/1950). As a result of this, semantic processing tasks are at the forefront of much research on cognitive deficits associated with psychosis and schizophrenia. Whilst normal semantic priming is associated with normal language processing, it is thought that overactive priming (faster and further spreading of activation) may relate to schizophrenic thought disorder and language disruptions. A faster spreading of activation in semantic networks in schizophrenia may result in spurious nodes becoming activated and interfering with the normal train of thoughts. In patients with schizophrenia, research has supported this hypothesis with increased automatic (i.e. at a short SOA) priming or 'hyper-priming' found in some studies (Moritz et al., 2001, Moritz et al., 2003). However, the results of research on semantic priming in schizophrenia have been far from consistent. A review of research in this field concluded that hyper-priming was more robustly found in conditions of ‘indirect’ priming (Minzenberg et al., 2002). Indirect priming refers to priming where, unlike ‘direct’ priming in which the words are directly semantically related (e.g. lemon-sour), the two words presented have a mediator word that is related to both of them but not presented (e.g. lemon-sweet, not presented mediator = sour). We therefore included an indirect as well as a direct priming condition in the current study, which aimed to examine the effects of cannabis, both acutely and sub-acutely, on semantic memory.

The present study set out to compare a naturalistic sample of cannabis users with controls on semantic processing and schizophrenia-like symptoms, both under the influence of the drug and then again when not intoxicated. We investigated semantic priming with a computer-based, lexical-decision task, which manipulated SOA. State schizotypy was assessed using a novel state schizotypy questionnaire (Mason et al., 2008). As little research has investigated the long-term effects of cannabis on semantic memory, this aspect of the study was exploratory. We hypothesised that intoxicated cannabis users would show a pattern of semantic priming similar to one that has been shown in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients, i.e. hyper-priming at a short SOA, and that this would be related to schizophrenia-like symptoms.

Section snippets

Design and participants

An independent groups, repeated measures design was used to compare a sample of 38 non-cannabis-using controls with 36 recreational cannabis users on two separate test occasions 3–5 days apart. Inclusion criteria required that participants were at least 18 years old, had English as a native language, were non-dyslexic and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no personal history of mental illness. The cannabis-using group were required to be recreational cannabis users (at least

Demographics

One-way ANOVAs showed no significant group differences in age, anxiety or depression (Table 1). There were no group differences in total scores on the trait schizotypy measure (SPQ), but the cannabis users did score more highly on one of the nine sub-scales: ‘loss of concentration’ (P = 0.003). Chi-square analysis showed no significant group differences of gender (Control group: 22 males, 15 females; Cannabis group 21 males, 14 females). Most participants were university students or graduates and

Discussion

This study investigated semantic priming in cannabis users both under the influence of the drug and when they had abstained for 3 to 5 days. The main finding was of an increase in priming, or ‘hyper-priming’ in cannabis users at the short SOA when they were under the influence of the drug versus when they were drug-free. In addition, un-intoxicated cannabis users showed a pattern of greater priming at the long SOA compared with non-cannabis-using controls. Although the two groups did not differ

Acknowledgements

We thank Chief Inspector Alan Macfarlane, Home Office UK, for his support in this research. The research was funded by the ESRC (UK RES).

References (42)

  • BarkusE.J. et al.

    Cannabis-induced psychosis-like experiences are associated with high schizotypy

    Psychopathology

    (2006)
  • BattigW.F. et al.

    Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories. A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1969)
  • BleulerE.

    Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias (translated by J.Zinkin, 1950), International Universities Press, New York.

    (1911)
  • BlockR.I. et al.

    Effects of chronic marijuana use on human cognition

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (1993)
  • British Crime Survey (2006).Drug Misuse: one in three young men use cannabis...
  • CurranH.V. et al.

    Cognitive and subjective dose–response effects of acute oral Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (2002)
  • D’SouzaD.C.

    Cannibinoids and psychosis

    International reviews of neurobiology

    (2007)
  • DaftersR.I. et al.

    Contribution of cannabis and MDMA (“ecstasy”) to cognitive changes in long-term polydrug users

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (2004)
  • Di FortiM. et al.

    Cannabis use and psychiatric and cogitive disorders: the chicken or the egg?

    Current Opinion in Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • FerdinandR.F. et al.

    Cannabis—psychosis pathway independent of other types of psychopathology

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2005)
  • FergussonD.M. et al.

    Tests of causal linkages between cannabis use and psychotic symptoms

    Addiction

    (2005)
  • FletcherP.C. et al.

    Schizophrenia, ketamine and cannabis: evidence of overlapping memory deficits

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2006)
  • ForsterK.I. et al.

    DMDX: a Windows display program with millisecond accuracy

    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers

    (2003)
  • FriedP.A. et al.

    Neurocognitive consequences of marihuana—a comparison with pre-drug performance

    Neurotoxicology and Teratology

    (2005)
  • Gouzoulis-MayfrankE. et al.

    Semantic hyperpriming in thought-disordered patients with schizophrenia: state or trait?—a longitudinal investigation

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2003)
  • KischkaU. et al.

    Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation

    Neuropsychologia

    (1996)
  • LupicaC.R. et al.

    Endocannabinoid release from midbrain dopamine neurons: a potential substrate for cannabinoid receptor antagonist treatment of addiction

    Neuropharmacology

    (2005)
  • ManschreckT.C. et al.

    Semantic priming in thought disordered schizophrenic patients

    Schizophrenia Research

    (1988)
  • MasonO.J. et al.

    The Psychotomimetic States Inventory (PSI): measuring psychotic-type experiences from ketamine and cannabis

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2008)
  • MessinisL. et al.

    Neuropsychological deficits in long-term frequent cannabis users

    Neurology

    (14-3-2006)
  • MeyerD. et al.

    Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence on retrieval operations

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1971)
  • Cited by (24)

    • Head in the clouds? Cannabis users' creativity in new venture ideation depends on their entrepreneurial passion and experience

      2021, Journal of Business Venturing
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, a meta-analysis has found that creative people tend to be more impulsive (Feist, 1998), and reductions in inhibition increase the number and originality of ideas in divergent thinking tasks (Radel et al., 2015). Scholars have also found that cannabis users demonstrate a greater ability to connect seemingly disconnected words compared to non-users (LaFrance and Cuttler, 2017) and identify associations and patterns more easily (Morgan et al., 2010). Such pattern recognition has been identified as a means by which entrepreneurs identify entrepreneurial opportunities (Baron and Ensley, 2006), with highly original ideas often surfacing through creative combinations at the nexus of disparate domains or concepts (Grégoire et al., 2010; Johansson, 2004; Ward, 2004).

    • Do the effects of cannabis on the hippocampus and striatum increase risk for psychosis?

      2020, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      It exerts its effects by acting as a partial agonist on CB1 endocannabinoid receptors, which are distributed across several cortical and subcortical regions (Herkenham et al., 1990), including the hippocampus, amygdala, striatum and cerebellum (Malone et al., 2010). THC is also responsible for the adverse effects associated with smoking cannabis, including mediating the risk of addiction (Morgan et al., 2010a, b), cognitive deficits (Oomen et al., 2018) and producing psychotic and anxiogenic effects (Morrison et al., 2009). CBD is the other main chemical constituent of cannabis often studied in research.

    • Δ<sup>9</sup>-Tetrahydrocannabinol at Retrieval Drives False Recollection of Neutral and Emotional Memories

      2018, Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      One potential mechanism by which THC during retrieval could increase false recollections is by impairing prefrontal functions (13,14), including retrieval monitoring processes that help to suppress false recollections for words and pictures (15). For example, THC is known to enhance mental imagery (36,37) and processing fluency (38,39), which together can result in false recollections when retrieval monitoring fails (40). A broad impairment of prefrontal monitoring processes also may explain why THC increased false alarms for both critical lures and unrelated lures in the DRM task.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text