Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 190, Issue 1, 30 November 2011, Pages 43-48
Psychiatry Research

Effect of alexithymia on the process and outcome of psychotherapy: A programmatic review

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.026Get rights and content

Abstract

Most psychotherapeutic approaches assume that individuals have some access to their emotions. Thus, patients who are unable to identify, differentiate, and articulate their emotions present therapists with a difficult challenge. Such patients may suffer from alexithymia. Despite much attention in the clinical literature, research on alexithymia in the treatment setting has been sparse. Thus, many of the assumptions about psychotherapeutic treatment of alexithymic patients remain untested. This article summarizes findings from a series of studies that examined the effect of alexithymia on various aspects of the psychotherapeutic enterprise. Findings indicated that alexithymia has little effect on patients' treatment preferences, yet there was some tendency for alexithymic patients to prefer group therapy. However, alexithymia was associated with poor outcome in both traditional psychodynamic psychotherapy and supportive therapy. This negative effect was found in individual and group psychotherapies. In the context of group therapy, higher levels of alexithymic features elicited negative reactions from one's therapist, which partially contributed to the poor outcome experienced by such patients. Finally, the negative reaction that therapists had toward patients with high alexithymia appeared to be in response to the lack of positive emotion expressed by these patients. Clinical implications and ideas for future research are considered.

Introduction

“The assumption that other people's emotional responses are like our own is the basis of empathy and as such is basic to all human intercourse. The uncritical acceptance of this assumption, however, has led us to the false expectation that all patients have to them the affective functions necessary for the utilization of psychotherapy” (Krystal, 1982–1983, p. 353). Indeed, most psychotherapeutic approaches assume that individuals have at least some awareness of and access to their emotions. However, some patients have great difficulty identifying and expressing feelings or emotional aspects of social interaction. Such patients may suffer from alexithymia, a term coined by Sifneos (1973). Alexithymia is characterized by an impoverished fantasy life and limited imagination, an impaired capacity for empathy, a propensity for impulsive behaviour, a tendency to somatize emotions, and a penchant for offering undifferentiated descriptions of emotional experience (Warnes, 1986, Taylor, 2000). According to Taylor (2000), the core features of alexithymia include difficulty identifying feelings and distinguishing them from bodily sensations of emotional arousal, difficulty describing feelings to other people, and an externally oriented cognitive style.

Current conceptualizations describe alexithymia as a trait deficit in the cognitive processing of emotional experience, such that individuals have a limited capacity to symbolize emotions and elaborate upon emotional experience (Taylor and Bagby, 2004). Thus, the construct of alexithymia is relevant to mental state understanding (Jurist, 2005, Choi-Kan and Gunderson, 2008, Fortune et al., 2004). As the editors of this special of Psychiatry Research ("Meta-cognitive and social cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Focus on psychosocial issues and treatment") have pointed out, mental state understanding involves a variety of processes (e.g., emotional awareness) that influence a person's capacity to think about aspects of the self and how these interact with the world, a capacity variously referred to as mentalizing, theory of mind, meta-cognition, and self-reflection (Dimaggio et al., 2011, Dimaggio et al., 2009). The ability to perceive, reflect on, and communicate affectively laden mental states is crucial for regulating one's emotions (Mohaupt et al., 2006, Lane, 2008). Furthermore, the capacity to differentiate one's emotional experiences from those of another is necessary for managing interpersonal relations (Guttman and Laporte, 2002). Difficulties identifying, interpreting, and describing one's emotions and those of others, characteristics of alexithymia, would therefore impede a person's ability to manage affectively oriented mental contents and interactions with others (Lane, 2008). In the presence of such impairment, emotions remain global and undifferentiated, leading to a relative inability to use one's own emotions to guide adaptive behaviour. As such, alexithymia has been conceptualized as one form of impairment that is part of a larger network of dysfunctions that contribute to difficulties understanding mental states (Dimaggio et al., 2009). Indeed, in a unique study, Moriguchi et al. (2006) found that alexithymia was related to impaired mentalizing, which in turn was associated with hypoactivity in the medial prefrontal cortices. The deficit in mentalizing was associated with impairment in the higher cognitive ability to take a perspective different from the self, a skill that may be essential for the comprehension of the mental states of both self and others. As Swiller (1988) points out, it is important to keep in mind that alexithymia is not a psychiatric disorder, but rather a characterization of thinking, feeling, and relating processes among patients with a wide range of psychiatric diagnoses. Further, alexithymia is not a dichotomous construct (i.e., you have it or you don't), but rather a dimensional one (i.e., people exhibit different levels or degrees of alexithymia).

Patients who are unable to identify, differentiate, and articulate their emotions present psychotherapists with a difficult challenge (Taylor, 1984, Kleinberg, 1996). Alexithymia constricts a person's ability to elaborate on inner feelings, to engage in abstract thought, and to appreciate the possibility that somatic symptoms may be the manifestation of psychic distress (Sifneos, 1973). Patients with high levels of alexithymia know that they do not feel good, but do not know how to say or describe what they are feeling. They have difficulty presenting material spontaneously and fixate on their physical symptoms and minute details of external events. Alexithymia is believed to complicate the establishment and maintenance of a therapeutic alliance, as it may predispose an individual toward narcissistic rage, hyper-irritability, and impulsive behaviour (Tacon, 2001). It has been shown to be associated with the use of primitive and immature defences such as projection and denial (Parker et al., 1998), as well as acting-out, dissociation, and passive–aggressive behaviour (Taylor et al., 1997). The lack of integration between affect and cognition, the use of immature defences, and a proclivity toward impulsive behaviour have significant implications for treatment (Krystal, 1979, Taylor et al., 1997).

Despite the considerable attention it has received in the clinical literature, there has been very little research that has examined the effect of alexithymia on the process and outcome of psychotherapy. Most research on alexithymia has focused primarily on etiological factors and associated pathologies (e.g., Bailey and Henry, 2007). Thus, many of the assumptions and musings about the psychotherapeutic treatment of an alexithymic patient remain untested. The work of our research group may represent one of the more comprehensive and systematic investigations of alexithymia in the context of psychotherapy. This article reviews a series of studies that we have conducted which attempted to provide some clarity regarding the role that alexithymia plays in the treatment setting.

Section snippets

Does alexithymia affect patients' willingness to engage in psychotherapy?

Given that alexithymic patients appear to lack the very skills that psychotherapy requires, e.g., self-reflection, interest in internal events, and access to feelings, it would seem reasonable to assume that they would tend to avoid psychotherapeutic treatment and prefer other forms of treatment, e.g., pharmacotherapy. However, it is unknown whether alexithymic patients actually prefer pharmacotherapy over psychotherapy, as no previous study has actually tested this assumption. We recently

Does alexithymia affect the outcome of psychotherapy?

A widely held assumption is that alexithymic individuals are ill-suited for dynamic psychotherapy and that supportive psychotherapy is more appropriate (Sifneos, 1996). We used data from two clinical trials of individual (N = 144) and group (N = 107) psychotherapies to test this assumption (McCallum et al., 2003). Each trial involved randomly assigning patients to either interpretive or supportive psychotherapy (Piper et al., 1998, Piper et al., 2001). Briefly, the primary objective of interpretive

Is there other evidence of the clinical relevance of alexithymia?

We further investigated the clinical relevance of alexithymia by looking at whether it was predictive of residual symptoms of depression following psychotherapy (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2004a). Residual symptoms of depression refer to symptoms that persist despite apparent response or remission, and appear to increase the risk of relapse (Judd et al., 2002). Because alexithymic people have a poor capacity to regulate and resolve negative emotions, they may be susceptible to residual symptoms.

We

What might account for the poor outcome of alexithymic patients?

We began to wonder how alexithymia produces this negative effect on the outcome of psychotherapy. There have been several suggestions about possible mechanisms. One concerns the therapists' negative reaction to alexithymic patients (Krystal, 1979). In other words, alexithymia may exert some of its negative effect through the therapist. Patients with high levels of alexithymia are often perceived as dull, boring, and frustrating for therapists (Taylor, 1976). Further, these patients' inability

Discussion

Our research group is among the few that have systematically studied alexithymia in the context of a psychotherapeutic treatment setting. That so few have engaged in such research is surprising given the frequency with which alexithymia is discussed in the clinical literature. Thus, whilst it is not possible to unequivocally rule out the prospect that our findings are limited to the particular treatment settings involved in our studies, the generalisability of the results is supported by their

References (64)

  • Y. Moriguchi et al.

    Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: an fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • J.S. Ogrodniczuk et al.

    Residual symptoms in depressed patients who respond to short-term psychotherapy

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2004)
  • J.S. Ogrodniczuk et al.

    The negative effect of alexithymia on the outcome of group therapy for complicated grief: what role might the therapist play?

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • J.D.A. Parker et al.

    Alexithymia: relationship with ego defense and coping styles

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • M. Rufer et al.

    A prospective long-term follow-up study of alexithymia in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • M. Speranza et al.

    Predictive value of alexithymia in patients with eating disorders: a 3-year prospective study

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2007)
  • M. Stingl et al.

    Effects of inpatient psychotherapy on the stability of alexithymia characteristics

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2008)
  • R.M. Bagby et al.

    The development of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia: item selection, factor structure, reliability and concurrent validity

    Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics

    (2006)
  • R.M. Baron et al.

    The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • A.W. Bateman et al.

    Mentalization-based treatment of BPD

    Journal of Personality Disorders

    (2004)
  • J.L. Berdahl et al.

    Effects of power on emotion and expression during a controversial group discussion

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • M. Beresnevaite

    Exploring the benefits of group psychotherapy in reducing alexithymia in coronary heart disease patients: a preliminary study

    Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics

    (2000)
  • E.A. Butler et al.

    The social consequences of expressive suppression

    Emotion

    (2003)
  • L.W. Choi-Kan et al.

    Mentalization: ontogeny, assessment, and application in the treatment of borderline personality disorder

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • D.G. Fortune et al.

    Targeting cognitive-behaviour therapy to patients' implicit model of psoriasis: results from a patient preference controlled trial

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (2004)
  • H.J. Grabe et al.

    Alexithymia and outcome in psychotherapy

    Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics

    (2008)
  • E.L. Jurist

    Mentalized affectivity

    Psychoanalytic Psychology

    (2005)
  • J.L. Kleinberg

    Working with alexithymic patients in groups

    Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy

    (1996)
  • H. Krystal

    Alexithymia and psychotherapy

    American Journal of Psychotherapy

    (1979)
  • Krystal, H., 1982–1983. Alexithymia and the effectiveness of psychoanalytic treatment. International Journal of...
  • R.D. Lane

    Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: a unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (2008)
  • R.D. Lane et al.

    Levels of emotional awareness: implications for psychotherapeutic integration

    Journal of Psychotherapy Integration

    (1992)
  • Cited by (175)

    • Alexithymia in gaming addiction and engagement

      2024, Journal of Affective Disorders
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text