Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 215, Issue 3, 30 March 2014, Pages 675-682
Psychiatry Research

Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: Pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041Get rights and content

Abstract

Dysfunctional decision-making in individuals with pathological gambling (PGs) may result from dominating reward-driven processes, indicated by higher impulsivity. In the current study we examined (1) if PGs show specific decision-making impairments related to dominating reward-driven processes rather than to strategic planning deficits and (2) whether these impairments are related to impulsivity. Nineteen PGs according to DSM-IV and 19 matched control subjects undertook the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) to assess decision-making. The delay discounting paradigm (DDP) as well as the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (measuring urgency, premeditation, perseverance and sensation seeking) were administered as multidimensional measures of impulsivity. Results revealed that (1) PGs exhibited higher risk seeking and an immediate reward focus in the CGT and, in contrast, comparable strategic planning to the control group. (2) Decision-making impairments were related to more severe delay discounting and, specifically, to increased urgency and less premeditation. Our findings suggest (1) the necessity to disentangle decision-making components in order to improve etiological models of PGs, and (2) that urgency and premeditation are specifically related to disadvantageous decision-making and should be tackled in intervention strategies focusing on emotion tolerance and control strategies.

Introduction

While it is well known, that individuals diagnosed with pathological gambling (PGs) display impaired performance in neuropsychological decision-making tasks (e.g. Petry, 2001b, Cavedini et al., 2002, Brand et al., 2005, Goudriaan et al., 2005, Linnet et al., 2006, Labudda et al., 2007, Forbush et al., 2008, Roca et al., 2008, Lawrence et al., 2009, Kertzman et al., 2011) and heightened impulsivity (Goudriaan et al., 2004, Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008, van Holst et al., 2010), the relation of both constructs in PGs remains unclear. Findings from previous studies on this issue in non-gambling samples were inconsistent (e.g. Monterosso et al., 2001, Jollant et al., 2005, Zermatten et al., 2005, Suhr and Tsanadis, 2007, Dolan et al., 2008, Franken et al., 2008, Sweitzer et al., 2008, Janis and Nock, 2009, Perales et al., 2009, Xiao et al., 2009, Billieux et al., 2010) which may result from a neglected consideration of the lower-order sub-components, for either or both constructs. Thus, the present study aims to assess the relation of impaired decision-making and impulsivity in PGs taking a multidimensional perspective.

In line with recent models on pathways of addictions (Bechara, 2005, Bühringer et al., 2008, Redish et al., 2008b, Goldstein and Volkow, 2011) and specifically of pathological gambling (Bechara, 2003, Evans and Coventry, 2006, van Holst et al., 2010), we assume an important role of an – either antecedent or resultant – imbalance of (more automatic) motivational and valuation brain networks and (more reflective) cognitive control networks. According to these models, the motivational and valuation systems in PGs may overestimate the value of immediate short-term rewards. This would explain, on the one hand, the heightened impulsivity found in PGs, because the tendency towards immediate rewards while disregarding negative consequences is frequently considered a central aspect of impulsivity (Moeller et al., 2001, Bechara, 2005, Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008). On the other hand, this imbalance may result in disadvantageous gambling-related decisions, as dominating reward-driven processes were found to be strongly related to impaired decision-making (Krawczyk, 2002, Fellows, 2004, Yechiam et al., 2005, Dunn et al., 2006, Rangel et al., 2008). Due to the lack of a consensus model of decision-making, a broad variety of tasks has been used to measure the construct (Fellows, 2004). However, a fractionating of lower-order decision-making components may be an advantageous approach to gain a deeper understanding of the complexity of underlying processing mechanisms (see e.g. Busemeyer and Stout, 2002, Krawczyk, 2002, Clark et al., 2004, Brand et al., 2005, Fellows and Farah, 2005, Yechiam et al., 2005, Diekhof et al., 2008). Following this approach, it can be expected that PGs will show specific impairments in decision-making sub-components associated with reward and valuation. These decision-components have often been related to processes in the ventromedial regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (e.g. Brand et al., 2006, Lawrence et al., 2008, Clark, 2010). Indeed, PGs display functional changes in these brain areas in tasks of reward processing and decision-making (Potenza et al., 2003, Reuter et al., 2005, Potenza, 2008, Clark, 2010). In contrast, decision-making components like reasoning or strategic planning – often related to processes in the dorsolateral PFC – may be unchanged in PGs (Lawrence et al., 2009, Clark, 2010). A neuropsychological task which differentiates between different components of decision-making behavior is the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) (Rogers et al., 1999). The CGT assesses parameters related to risk seeking (e.g. ‘risk taking’), an immediate reward focus (e.g. ‘delay aversion’) or strategic planning (e.g. ‘percentage of rational choices’). First studies using the CGT in gamblers and PGs revealed disorder-specific deficits in risk seeking rather than in strategic planning (Lawrence et al., 2008, Grant et al., 2011). Crucially, both studies did not explicitly report on other parameters related to reward-driven processes like delay aversion or chasing behavior within a task (i.e. betting larger sums of money or taking greater risks with an intention to recover prior losses immediately) (O’Connor and Dickerson, 2003, Linnet et al., 2006).

According to our present hypothesis, the impaired reward- and valuation-related components of decision-making are positively related to impulsivity, in contrast to the strategic components of decision-making (Lawrence et al., 2008). To our best knowledge, to date, there are no studies on the relation of impaired decision-making components and impulsivity in PGs. Studies on this relation in non-gambling samples have mainly been conducted without considering the aforementioned multidimensional nature of decision-making and of impulsivity itself (e.g. Reynolds et al., 2006, Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008, Broos et al., 2012). Especially, the behaviorally assessed impulsivity dimension delay discounting (Bechara, 2003, Franken et al., 2008) as well as self-reported dimensions concerning the tendency to act rashly in an emotional context (urgency) and the lack of forethought (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001) may be relevant mechanisms underlying impaired decision-making (Enticott and Ogloff, 2006). Hitherto, studies with non-gambling samples assessing the relation of delay discounting and decision-making yielded inconsistent results (Monterosso et al., 2001, Janis and Nock, 2009), whereas studies on urgency and premeditation underpin our hypothesis (Zermatten et al., 2005, Billieux et al., 2010).

In summary, we expected a specific pattern of impaired decision-making components in PGs which is related to an imbalance of reward- and valuation-related and reflection-related brain networks, as indicated by increased risk seeking and an immediate reward focus. Further, we hypothesized that impulsivity is an important indicator of this imbalance. Thus, we expect that the impaired decision-making components are specifically related to the impulsivity dimensions delay discounting, urgency and premeditation. As impaired decision-making is important in the development (e.g. starting to gamble regularly), maintenance (e.g. chasing behavior) and cessation (as well as relapse) of pathological gambling, it is of central importance to understand its components and underlying mechanisms. If a dominating reward- and valuation-system, indicated by an elevated impulsivity, turns out to be the core mechanism rather than problems in reasoning or strategic planning, this would be an important result for the adaptation of etiological models as well as therapeutic strategies.

Section snippets

Design

We used a cross-sectional design with matched-pairs to compare a pathological gambling group (PGG) and a paired control group (CG). In detail, we matched for each member of PGG a corresponding member of the control group. The matching was realized according to age (±2 years), gender and smoking status (daily smoking or not), because these variables were found to be associated with different decision-making and impulsivity measures (Mitchell, 2004, Reynolds et al., 2007, Fields et al., 2009).

Decision-making

Mean scores, standard deviation and test statistic of group differences between the PGG and CG on the decision-making parameters are displayed in Table 2. Regarding our hypotheses of specific decision-making differences, we found the following results:

Significant group differences as expected were found for risk taking (F(1,16)=5.71, p=0.03) and delay aversion (F(1,16)=5.74, p=0.03). These results and the interaction of group and condition in risk taking (F(1,16)=7.10, p=0.02) indicate, on the

Discussion

In the current study, we investigated the specificity of impaired decision-making sub-components in PGs and the relation of these impairments to multidimensionally assessed impulsivity. Our results showed that PGs displayed specific impairments of decision-components which are presumably related to dominating reward- and valuation-systems tuned to immediate short-term rewards. This was further supported by the finding that heightened impulsivity was specifically related to these decision-making

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported by the German Research Foundation (Grant GO-720/8-1 and SFB 940/1 2013). The authors wish to thank Andrea Horn for her assistance to carry out the assessments and her contribution to the data entry.

References (91)

  • I.H.A. Franken et al.

    Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision-making deficits

    Psychiatry Research

    (2008)
  • P. Gay et al.

    Heterogeneous inhibition processes involved in different facets of self-reported impulsivity: evidence from a community sample

    Acta Psychologica

    (2008)
  • A.E. Goudriaan et al.

    Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2004)
  • A.E. Goudriaan et al.

    Decision making in pathological gambling: a comparison between pathological gamblers, alcohol dependents, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal controls

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2005)
  • J.E. Grant et al.

    Selective decision-making deficits in at-risk gamblers

    Psychiatry Research

    (2011)
  • D.D. Holt et al.

    Is discounting impulsive? Evidence from temporal and probability discounting in gambling and non-gambling college students

    Behavioural Processes

    (2003)
  • K. Houben et al.

    Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2011)
  • I.B. Janis et al.

    Are self-injurers impulsive? Results from two behavioral laboratory studies

    Psychiatry Research

    (2009)
  • S. Kertzman et al.

    Risk-taking decisions in pathological gamblers is not a result of their impaired inhibition ability

    Psychiatry Research

    (2011)
  • D.C. Krawczyk

    Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision making

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2002)
  • K. Labudda et al.

    Decision-making and neuroendocrine responses in pathological gamblers

    Psychiatry Research

    (2007)
  • C.H. Lagorio et al.

    Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards: III. Steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards

    Behavioural Processes

    (2005)
  • N.M. Petry

    Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2001)
  • N.M. Petry et al.

    Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (1999)
  • S. Rahman et al.

    Decision making and neuropsychiatry

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2001)
  • B. Reynolds et al.

    Dimensions of impulsive behavior: personality and behavioral measures

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2006)
  • R.D. Rogers et al.

    Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (1999)
  • J.A. Suhr et al.

    Affect and personality correlates of the Iowa Gambling Task

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2007)
  • R.J. van Holst et al.

    Why gamblers fail to win: a review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2010)
  • A. Verdejo-Garcia et al.

    Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2008)
  • S.P. Whiteside et al.

    The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR

    (2000)
  • A. Bechara

    Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction

    Journal of Gambling Studies

    (2003)
  • A. Bechara

    Decision-making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • W.K. Bickel et al.

    Are executive function and impulsivity antipodes? A conceptual reconstruction with special reference to addiction

    Psychopharmacology

    (2012)
  • A. Blaszczynski et al.

    A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling

    Addiction

    (2002)
  • N. Broos et al.

    The relationship between impulsive choice and impulsive action: a cross-species translational study

    PLoS ONE

    (2012)
  • G. Bühringer et al.

    Universal characteristics and consequences of the addiction syndrome

  • G. Bühringer et al.

    Why people change? The role of cognitive control processes in the onset and cessation of substance abuse disorders

    International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research

    (2008)
  • J.R. Busemeyer et al.

    A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task

    Psychological Assessment

    (2002)
  • L. Clark

    Decision-making during gambling: an integration of cognitive and psychobiological approaches

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences

    (2010)
  • L. Clark et al.

    Social and emotional decision-making following frontal lobe injury

    Neurocase

    (2004)
  • J. Deakin et al.

    Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2004)
  • M.R. Dixon et al.

    Delay discounting by pathological gamblers

    Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis

    (2003)
  • S.L. Dolan et al.

    Executive dysfunction as a risk marker for substance abuse: the role of impulsive personality traits

    Behavioral Sciences & the Law

    (2008)
  • Cited by (64)

    • Neuropsychology in GD: old and new directions

      2020, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Nevertheless, not all GD studies report altered cognitive control on the Stroop [20–22], suggesting the existence of important GD subgroups, in which interference control is not the central feature. On risk-taking tasks, GD groups show riskier performance through greater bet proportions, higher chasing behaviour, and greater choices of high-loss probabilities, including staking more points following losses, suggesting a deficiency in using negative feedback [10,23–25]. Additionally, the greatest risk-taking is evident in those GD individuals with comorbid alcohol and nicotine use disorders, with heightened impulsivity in these individuals linked with more aggressive betting [26–28].

    • A computational model of the Cambridge gambling task with applications to substance use disorders

      2020, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, unlike QDM and IMP, Risk is often operationalized in different ways across studies. For example, some studies define Risk as the average betting ratio only on those trials where participants choose the optimal color (Risk+; e.g. Kräplin et al., 2014; Zois et al., 2014); others as the average betting ratio across all trials (Risk-Avg or Overall Proportion Bet; e.g. Bowden-Jones et al., 2005; Lawrence et al., 2009); and still others as the average betting ratio for trials in which participants choose the less optimal color (Risk-, e.g. Baldacchino et al., 2015). Based on our literature review, we chose to operationalize risk-taking as the Risk + definition above due to its more widespread use (which is typically called ‘Risk Taking’).

    • Long-term deficits in risky decision-making after traumatic brain injury on a rat analog of the Iowa gambling task

      2019, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Some of the most long-term, difficult to manage, and pervasive deficits associated with TBI are impairments revolving around cognition and executive function, including various memory deficits, poor impulse control, and reduced decision-making capacity. Often, impairments can result in symptoms resembling those found in psychiatric disorders such as gambling disorder or bipolar disorder (Kräplin et al., 2014; Zgaljardic et al., 2015). In particular, impairments in decision-making and impulsivity, are likely to contribute to poor quality of life, and may result in significant issues for both patients with TBI, and their caregivers (Marsh et al., 1998).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text