Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T08:07:40.685Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Attention as an organ system

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Michael I. Posner
Affiliation:
Professor Emeritus Psychology Department University of Oregon Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227
Jin Fan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry Icahn Medical Institute 1425 Madison Avenue, Room 20-82 Mount Sinai School of Medicine One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1228 New York, NY 10029
James R. Pomerantz
Affiliation:
Rice University, Houston
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Attention is relatively easy to define subjectively as in the classical definition of William James (1890) who said: “Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind in clear and vivid form of one out of what seem several simultaneous objects or trains of thought.”

However, this subjective definition does not provide hints that might lead to an understanding of attentional development or pathologies. The theme of our chapter is that it is now possible to view attention much more concretely as an organ system. We follow the Webster dictionary definition of an organ system: “An organ system may be defined as differentiated structures in animals and plants made up of various cell and tissues and adapted for the performance of some specific function and grouped with other structures into a system.”

We believe that viewing attention as an organ system aids in answering many perplexing issues raised in cognitive psychology, psychiatry, and neurology. Neuroimaging studies have systemically shown that a wide variety of cognitive tasks can be seen as activating a distributed set of neural areas, each of which can be identified with specific mental operations (Posner & Raichle, 1994, 1998). Perhaps the areas of activation have been more consistent for the study of attention than for any other cognitive system. We can view attention as involving specialized networks to carry out functions such as achieving and maintaining the alert state, orienting to sensory events, and controlling thoughts and feelings.

Type
Chapter
Information
Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
From Cells to Cognition
, pp. 31 - 61
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdullaev, Y. G. and Posner, M. I. (1998). Event-related brain potential imaging of semantic encoding during processing single words. Neuroimage, 7, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allman, J. (2001). The anterior cingulate cortex: the evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. In Damasio, A.et al. (eds.), Unity of Knowledge. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 935, 107–17.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (2000). Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18(3), 281–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beane, M. and Marrocco, R. T. (2004). Cholinergic and noradrenergic inputs to the parietal cortex modulate the components of exogenous attention. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, New York: Guilford, pp. 313–28.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., and Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, RC165:1–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benes, F. (1999). Model generation and testing to probe neural circuitry in the cingulate cortex of postmortem schizophrenic brains. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 219–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., and Dolan, R. J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facila expression of sadness and anger. Brain, 1222, 883–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, G., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., et al. (2005). Effects of Catecol-O-Methyltransferase val158 met genotype on attentional control. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5038–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botwinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, G., Frazier, J. A., Rauch, S. L., et al. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting Stroop. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1542–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4/6, 215–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., et al. (1998). The counting Stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging – validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270–82.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M. and Moses, L. J. (2001). Individiual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R., Giedd, J., et al. (1997a). The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: a developmental neuroanatomical study. Developmental Psychobiology, 3, 61–9.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., et al. (1997b). A developmental function MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Development of the functional visual field. Acta Psychologica, 106, 51–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. D., Aston-Jones, G., and Gilzenrat, M. S. (2004). Guided activation, adaptive gating and conflict monitoring, and exploitation versus exploration. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, New York: Guilford, pp. 71–90.Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., and Shulman, G. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex, Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems?Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 831–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M. and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L., and Petersen, S. E. (1991). Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: Functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 2383–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coull, J. T., Nobre, A. C., and Frith, C. D. (2001). The noradrenergic alph2 agonist clonidine modulates behavioural and neuroanatomical correlates of human attentional orienting and alerting. Cerebral Cortex, 11(1), 73–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curran, T. and Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and non-attentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 189–202.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994) Descartes Error: Emotion, Reason and the Brain. New York: G. P. Putnam.Google Scholar
Davidson, M. C. and Marrocco, R. T. (2000). Local infusion of scopolamine into intraparietal cortex slows cover orienting in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 1536–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In Carey, S. and Gelman, R. (eds.), The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 67–110.Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (2004). Selective attention in distributed brain systems. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, New York: Guilford, pp. 105–13.Google Scholar
Duncan, J. and Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 475–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., et al. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, M. F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J. H., et al. (2003). The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell, 112, 257–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, B. A. and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everitt, B. J. and Robbins, T. W. (1997). Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 649–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage, 18, 42–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Fossella, J. A., Summer, T., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 7406–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., and Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26, 471–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, M., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(14), 340–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, J., Wu, Y., Fossella, J., and Posner, M. I. (2001). Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BioMed Central Neuroscience, 2, 14.Google ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Duque, D. and Posner, M. I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 74–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., et al. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedrich, F. J., Egly, R., Rafal, R. D., and Beck, D. (1998). Spatial attention deficits in humans: A comparison of superior parietal and temporal-parietal junction lesions. Neuropsychology, 12(2), 193–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, W. J., Gross, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., and Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24–36 months of age. Developmental Science, 3/4, 397–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, E. (2001). The Executive Brain. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Goldberg, T. E. and Weinberger, D. R. (2004). Genes and the parsing of cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 325–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, P. M., Sunderland, T., Friz, J. L., and Parasuraman, R. (2000). Genetics and visual attention: Selective deficits in healthy adult carriers of the epsilon 4 allele of the apolipoprotein E gene. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11661–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harman, C., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (1997). Distress and attention interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 27–43.Google Scholar
Hawkins, H. L., Hillyard, S. A., Luck, S. J., et al. (1990). Visual attention modulates signal detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, 802–11.Google Scholar
Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., Burchert, W., et al. (1994). Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature, 372, 543–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Jones, L., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of inhibitory control in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanwisher, N. and Duncan, J. (eds.) (2004). Attention and Performance XX, Oxford University Press, pp. 505–28.Google Scholar
Karnath, H.-O., Ferber, S., and Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature, 411, 95–953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastner, S., Pinsk, M. A., Weerd, P., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1999). Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron, 22, 751–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennard, M. A. (1955). The cingulate gyrus in relation to consciousness. Journal of Nervous Mental Disorders, 121, 34–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G. (1995). Children's temperament, mothers' discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., and Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internationalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Losier, B. J. W. and Klein, R. (2001). A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal damage. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luu, P., Collins, P., and Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality and self-monitoring: Negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error-detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129, 43–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., and Driver, J. (2000). Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. Science, 289, 1204–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marrocco, R. T. and Davidson, M. C. (1998). Neurochemistry of attention. In Parasuraman, R. (ed.), The Attentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 35–50.Google Scholar
Martinzez, A., DiRusso, F., Anllo-Vento, L., et al. (2001). Putting spatial attention on the map: Timing and localization of stimulus selection processing striate and extrastriate visual areas. Vision Research, 41, 1437–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 168–80.Google Scholar
Mesulam, M.-M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, 309–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nimchinsky, E. A., Gilissen, E., Allman, J. M., et al. (1999). A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 5268–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N., Kossyln, S. M., Cosgrove, G. R., et al. (2001). Deficits in visual cognition and attention following bilateral anterior cingulotomy. Neuropsychologia, 39, 219–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M., Haxby, J. B., and Grady, C. L. (1992). Visuospatial attention in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain, 115, 711–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M., Kumar, R., and Fossella, J. (2005). Beyond heritability: Neurotransmitter genes differentially modulate visuospatial attention and working memory. Psychological Science, 16, 200–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, R. J. and Zeki, S. (2000). The neurology of saccades and covert shifts of spatial attention. Brain, 123, 2273–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric Explorations of Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In Boll, T. and Bryant, B. (eds.), Master Lectures in Clinical Neuropsychology and Brain Function: Research, Measurement, and Practice, American Psychological Association, pp. 171–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. (ed.) (2004). Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Gilbert, C. D. (1999). Attention and primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 96/6, 2585–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of Mind. Scientific American Books.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Raichle, M. E. (eds.) (1998). Neuroimaging of Cognitive Processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, 763–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Rothbart, M. K. (2005). Influencing brain networks: Implications for education. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 99–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., and Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive functions in the human brain. Science, 240, 1627–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rafal, R. (1998). Neglect. In Parasuraman, R. (ed.), The Attentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 711–33.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., Fiez, J. A., Videen, T. O., et al. (1994). Practice-related changes in the human brain: Functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 8–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rob, H. J., Lubbe, V., and Keuss, P. J. G. (2001). Focused attention reduces the effect of lateral interference in multi-element arrays. Psychological Research, 65, 107–18.Google Scholar
Robertson, I. H., Tegnér, R., Tham, K., Lo, A., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1995). Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: Theoretical and rehabilitation implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(3), 416–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, I. H. (1999). Cognitive rehabilitation, attention and neglect. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 385–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., and Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71, 1113–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., Halparin, J., et al. (2004a). Development of attention during childhood, Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–40.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2004b). Attentional control and self-regulation. In Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D. (eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, New York: Guilford Press, 14, 283–300.Google Scholar
Sapir, A., Soroker, N., Berger, A., and Henik, A. (1999). Inhibition of return in spatial attention: Direct evidence for collicular generation. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1053–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohlberg, M. M., McLaughlin, K. A., Pavese, A., Heidrich, A., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Evaluation of attention process therapy training in persons with acquired brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 656–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, C., Burke, S., and Marrocco, R. (2001). Cholinergic modulation of covert attention in the rat. Psychopharmocology, 155(2), 210–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strum, W., Willmes, K., Orgass, B., and Hartje, W. (1997). Do specific attention effects need specific training. Neurological Rehabilitation, 81–103.Google Scholar
Swanson, J., Oosterlaan, J., Murias, M., et al. (2000). ADHD children with 7-repeat allele of the DRD4 gene have extreme behavior but normal performance on critical neuropsychological tests of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 4754–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turken, A. U. and Swick, D. (1999). Response selection in the human anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Neurosceince, 2(10), 920–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uttal, W. R. (2001). The New Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Voytko, M. L., Olton, D. S., Richardson, R. T., et al. (1994). Basal forebrain lesions in monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(1), 167–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Washburn, D. A. (1998). Stroop-like effects for monkeys and humans: Processing speed or strength of association?Psychological Science, 5, 375–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wojciulik, E., Kanwisher, N., and Driver, J. (1998). Covert visual attention modulates face-specific activity in the human fusiform gyrus: fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(3), 1574–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Attention as an organ system
    • By Michael I. Posner, Professor Emeritus Psychology Department University of Oregon Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227, Jin Fan, Department of Psychiatry Icahn Medical Institute 1425 Madison Avenue, Room 20-82 Mount Sinai School of Medicine One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1228 New York, NY 10029
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.005
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Attention as an organ system
    • By Michael I. Posner, Professor Emeritus Psychology Department University of Oregon Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227, Jin Fan, Department of Psychiatry Icahn Medical Institute 1425 Madison Avenue, Room 20-82 Mount Sinai School of Medicine One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1228 New York, NY 10029
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.005
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Attention as an organ system
    • By Michael I. Posner, Professor Emeritus Psychology Department University of Oregon Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227, Jin Fan, Department of Psychiatry Icahn Medical Institute 1425 Madison Avenue, Room 20-82 Mount Sinai School of Medicine One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1228 New York, NY 10029
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.005
Available formats
×