Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T22:56:12.825Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Culture and developmental pathways of relationship formation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Sevda Bekman
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Ayhan Aksu-Koç
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Get access

Summary

I met Çiğdem at various IACCP conferences over the last decades in various functions. The distant perspective of an intellectually much appreciated colleague turned over the years into a close and wonderful friendship. Çiğdem's enthusiasm and devotion for her work and her message is inspiring for students and colleagues. Her interpersonal warmth and her sense of humour make all the encounters with her special. I feel privileged to have had many great moments with her and I truly want many more to come.

Heidi Keller

m: No, I like it but I'm just thinking the child should not grow up only in the world of herself and the mother.

i: Okay.

m: You need to bring other people into her life because you cannot just grow up with your child. There are moments, for instance if she [the mother] dies now, the child will find it difficult getting closer to another person. Though the mother and the child need to have their own relationship, when you are making a relationship with the child [you must] allow her to interact with other people. So that, if you are not there, she can easily get closer to other people. Like in this situation when you are always there with the child, immediately you are not there she looks for that familiar face and when she does not find the face, no matter what any other person does it cannot really satisfy her.

(Excerpt from an interview with a Cameroonian Nso mother about the best care for an infant)

m: And sometimes they do need time away – uhm –, 'cause they get overstimulated if there are just too many people around.[…]

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Infancy in Uganda (Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1967).Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Maternal Sensitivity Scales (www.psychology.sunysb.edu/ ewaters/552/senscoop.htm, 1969).
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S., Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978).Google Scholar
Bard, K. A., Leavens, D., Custance, D., Vancatova, M., Keller, H., Benga, O., and Sousa, C., “Emotion cognition: Comparative perspectives on the social cognition of emotion,” Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 8 (2005), 3–4.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Konner, M., Bakeman, R., and Adamson, L., “Crying in Kung San infants: A test of the cultural specificity hypothesis,” Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33 (1991), 601–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., “Interactional and contextual determinants of attachment security,” in Cassidy, J. and Shaver, P. R. (eds.), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (New York: Guilford Press, 1999), pp. 249–64.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J., Attachment and Loss. Volume 1. Attachment (New York: Basic Books, 1969).Google Scholar
Bowlby, J., Attachment and Loss. Volume 3. Loss: Sadness and Depression (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1980).Google Scholar
Bowlby, J., A Secure Base: Parent–child Attachment and Healthy Human Development (New York: Basic Books, 1988).Google Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., and Main, M., “The origins of reciprocity: The early mother–infant interaction,” in Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), The Effect of the Infant on its Caregiver (New York: Wiley, 1974), pp. 49–76.Google Scholar
Bruner, J., Actual Minds, Possible World. (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1986).Google Scholar
Carlson, V. J. and Harwood, R. L., “Attachment, culture, and the caregiving system: The cultural patterning of everyday experiences among Anglo and Puerto Rican mother–infant pairs,” Infant Mental Health Journal, 24 (2003), 53–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, R. K., “Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training,” Child Development, 65 (1994), 1111–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, R. K., “Chinese and European American cultural models of the self reflected in mothers' childrearing beliefs,” Ethos, 23 (1995), 328–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockenberg, S. and Litman, C., “Autonomy as competence in 2-year olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance and self-assertion,” Developmental Psychology, 26 (1990), 961–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draper, P. and Harpending, H., “A sociobiological perspective on human reproductive strategies,” in MacDonald, K. B. (ed.), Sociobiological Perspectives on Human Development (New York: Springer, 1988), pp. 340–72.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P. and W.-L. Liu, “Parenting toddlers,” in Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Handbook of Parenting. Volume 1. Children and Parenting, Second edition (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2002), pp. 45–71.Google Scholar
Emde, R. N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R. B., and Oppenheim, D., “The moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowledge,” Developmental Review, 11 (1991), 251–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantz, R. L., “Pattern vision in newborn infants,” Science, 140 (1963), 296–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C., “Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment,” Psychological Bulletin, 126 (2000), 55–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M., “Culture as process: Empirical methods for cultural psychology,” in Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., and Pandey, J. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-cultural psychology. Volume1, Second edition (Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, 1997), pp. 301–46.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., and Maynard, A., “Cultural pathways through universal development,” Annual Review of Psychology, 54 (2003), 461–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harwood, R. L., Leyendecker, B., Carlson, V. J., Asencio, M., and Miller, A. M., “Parenting among Latino families in the US,” in Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Handbook of Parenting. Volume 4. Social Conditions and Applied Parenting, Second edition (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2002), pp. 21–46.Google Scholar
Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., and Irizarry, N. Lucca, Culture and Attachment: Perceptions of the Child in Context (New York: Guilford Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerich, A., Schulze, P. A., and Gonzalez, Z., “Cultural differences in maternal beliefs and behaviors: A study of middle-class Anglo and Puerto Rican mother–infant pairs in four everyday situations,” Child Development, 70 (1999), 1005–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinde, R. A., “Attachment: Some conceptual and biological issues,” in Stevenson-Hinde, J. and Parke, C. M.s (eds.), The Place of Attachment in Human Behavior (New York: Basic Books, 1982), pp. 60–76.Google Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “Family and socialization in cross-cultural perspective: A model of change,” in Berman, J. J. (ed.), Cross-cultural Perspectives: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1989 (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1990), pp. 135–200.Google Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “A critical appraisal of individualism and collectivism: Toward a new formulation,” in Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., Choi, S.-C., and Yoon, G. (eds.), Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method and Applications (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1994), pp. 52–65.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “Individualism and collectivism,” in Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., and Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç (eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology. Volume 3. Social Behavior and Applications, Second edition (Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, 1996), pp. 1–49.Google Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Implications for self and family,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36 (2005), 403–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kakar, S., The Inner World: A Psychoanalytic Study of Childhood and Society in India (New Delhi, India: Oxford University Press, 1978).Google Scholar
Kaye, K. and Fogel, A., “The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants,” Developmental Psychology, 16 (1980), 454–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., “Human parent–child relationships from an evolutionary perspective,” American Behavioral Scientist, Special Issue, 43 (2000), 957–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., “Ontogeny as the interface between biology and culture: Evolutionary considerations,” in Saraswathi, T. S. (ed.), Cross-cultural Perspectives in Human Development: Theory, Research and Application (New Delhi: Sage, 2003), pp. 102–27.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Cultures of Infancy (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2007).Google Scholar
Keller, H. and Lamm, B., “Parenting as the expression of sociohistorical time: The case of German individualism,” International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29 (2005), 238–46.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Hentschel, E., Yovsi, R. D., Abels, M., Lamm, B., and Haas, V., “The psycho-linguistic embodiment of parental ethnotheories: A new avenue to understand cultural differences in parenting,” Culture and Psychology, 10 (2004a), 293–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Kärtner, J., Borke, J., Yovsi, R. D., and Kleis, A., “Parenting styles and the development of the categorial self: A longitudinal study on mirror self recognition in Cameroonian Nso farming and German families,” International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29 (2005), 496–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Cappenberg, M., and Chasiotis, A., “Temporal contingency as an independent component of parenting behavior,” Child Development, 70 (1999), 474–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Papaligoura, Z., Kuensemueller, P., Voelker, S., Papaliou, C., Lohaus, A., Kokkinaki, N., Chrysikou, L., and Mousouli, V., “Concepts of mother–infant interactions in Greece and Germany,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34 (2003), 677–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Voelker, S., and Yovsi, R. D., “Conceptions of parenting in different cultural communities: The case of West African Nso and Northern German women,” Social Development, 14 (2005), 158–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Yovsi, R. D., Borke, J., Kärtner, J., Jensen, H., and Papaligoura, Z., “Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: Self regulation and self recognition in three cultural communities,” Child Development, 75 (2004b), 1745–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H., Yovsi, R. D., and Voelker, S., “The role of motor stimulation in parental ethnotheories: The case of Cameroonian Nso and German women,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (2002), 398–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., “Cultural psychology of the self: A renewed look at independence and interdependence,” in Hofsten, C. and Bäckmnan, L. (eds.), Psychology of the Turn of the Millenium (Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 2000), pp. 305–22.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A., “Parental goals: A cross-cultural view,” Teachers College Record, 76 (1974), 226–39.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A., “Infant environments in psychoanalysis: A cross-cultural view,” in Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A., and Herdt, G. (eds.), Cultural Psychology. Essays on Comparative Human Development (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990), pp. 454–74.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A., Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1994).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeVine, R. A. and Norman, K., “The infant's acquisition of culture: Early attachment reexamined in anthropological perspective,” in Moore, C. C. and Methews, H. F. (eds.), The Psychology of Cultural Experience (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001), pp. 83–104.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J., Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Self (New York: Plenum Press, 1979).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K., “Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung [The inborn forms of potential experience],” Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5 (1943), 235–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R. and Kitayama, S., “Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation,” Psychological Review, 98 (1991), 224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard, A. E., “Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions,” Child Development, 73 (2002), 969–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, E., The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press, 1982).Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. Das, Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M., “Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding,” Child Development, 73 (2002), 1715–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. M. and Harwood, R. L., “The cultural organization of parenting: Change and stability of behavior patterns during feeding and social play across the first year of life,” Parenting: Science and Practice, 2 (2002), 241–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nsamenang, A. B. and Lamb, M. E., “Socialization of Nso children in the Bamenda grassfields of Northwest Cameroon,” in Greenfield, P. M. and Cocking, R. R. (eds.), Cross-cultural Roots of Minority Child Development (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1994), pp. 133–46.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., “Talking to children in Western Samoa,” Language in Society, 11 (1982), 77–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papoušek, H. and Papoušek, M., “Innate and cultural guidance of infants' integrative competencies: China, the United States, and Germany,” in Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Cultural Approaches to Parenting (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991), pp. 23–44.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D., “Chimpanzee theory of mind?,” in Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. K. (eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996), pp. 293–329.Google Scholar
Prechtl, H., Continuity of Neural Functions from Prenatal To Postnatal Life (London: Spastics International Medical Publications, 1984).Google Scholar
Rabain-Jamin, J. and Sabeau-Jouannet, E., “Maternal speech to 4-month-old infants in two cultures: Wolof and French,” International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20 (1997), 425–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V., Hay, D., Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C., “Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention,” in Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant Development: Recent Advances (Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1997), pp. 247–73.Google Scholar
Rochat, P., “Early development of the ecological self,” in C. Dent-Read and P. Zukow-Goldring (eds.), Evolving Explanations of Development. Ecological Approaches to Organism-environment Systems (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1997), pp. 91–121.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., and Weisz, J., “The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension,” Child Development, 71 (2000), 1121–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saraswathi, T. S. and Pai, S., “Socialization in the Indian context,” in Kao, H. S. R. and Sinha, D. (eds.), Asian Perspectives on Psychology (New Delhi: Sage, 1997), pp. 74–92.Google Scholar
Stewart, S. M. and Bond, M. H., “A critical look at parenting research from the mainstream: Problems uncovered while adapting Western research to non-Western cultures,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20 (2002), 379–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Super, C. M. and Harkness, S., “The cultural structuring of child development,” in Berry, J. W., Dasen, P. R., and Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology Voume 2. Basic Processes and Human Development, Second edition (Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, 1996).Google Scholar
Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y. H., and Davidson, D. H., Preschool in Three Cultures: Japan, China and the United States (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1989).Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., “Cultural transmission: A view from chimpanzees and human infants,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (2001), 135–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ijzendoorn, M. H. and Sagi, A., “Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions,” in Cassidy, J. and Shaver, P. R. (eds.), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (New York: Guilford Press, 1999), pp. 713–34.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. S., Matheson, C., and Bernheimer, L., “American cultural models of early influence and parent recognition of developmental delays: Is earlier always better than later?,” in Harkness, S., Super, C. M., and New, R. (eds.), Parents' Cultural Belief Systems (New York: Guilford Press, 1996), pp. 496–531.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B. (ed.), Six cultures: Studies of Child Rearing (New York: Wiley, 1963).
Whiting, B. B. and Edwards, C., Children of Different Worlds: The Formation Of Social Behavior (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O., Sociobiology (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1975).Google Scholar
Yovsi, R. D. and Keller, H., “Breastfeeding: An adaptive process,” Ethos, 31 (2003), 147–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×