Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T00:30:26.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time-dependent changes in positively biased self-perceptions of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A developmental psychopathology perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 April 2010

Betsy Hoza*
Affiliation:
University of Vermont
Dianna Murray-Close
Affiliation:
University of Vermont
L. Eugene Arnold
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Stephen P. Hinshaw
Affiliation:
University of California at Berkeley
Lily Hechtman
Affiliation:
Montreal Children's Hospital
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Betsy Hoza, Department of Psychology, University of Vermont, 2 Colchester Avenue, Burlington, VT 05405-0134; E-mail: bhoza@uvm.edu.

Abstract

This study examined changes in the degree of positive bias in self-perceptions of previously diagnosed 8- to 13-year-old children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; n = 513) and comparison peers (n = 284) over a 6-year period. The dynamic association between biased self-perceptions and dimensional indices of depressive symptoms and aggression also were considered. Across the 6-year time span, comparison children exhibited less bias than children with ADHD, although a normative bolstering of social self-views during early adolescence was observed. Decreases in positive biases regarding social and behavioral competence were associated with increases in depressive symptoms over time, whereas increases in levels of positively biased self-perceptions in the behavioral (but not social) domain were predictive of greater aggression over time. ADHD status moderated the dynamic association between biases and adjustment. Finally, evidence indicated that there was a bidirectional relationship between biases and aggression, whereas depressive symptoms appeared to inversely predict later bias.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 6594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berndt, T. J. (1996). Transitions in friendship and friends' influence. In Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Petersen, A. C. (Eds.), Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and contexts (pp. 5784). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bukowski, W. M., & Kramer, T. L. (1986). Judgments of the features of friendships among early adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Early Adolescence, 6, 331338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 11521162.Google ScholarPubMed
David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive self-perceptions have a “dark side”? Examination of the link between perceptual bias and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 327337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. S. (2002). Statistical methods for the analysis of repeated measurements. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2004). Measuring informant discrepancies in clinical child research. Psychological Assessment, 16, 330334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, M. B., & Milich, R. (1997). Effects of positive feedback on the social interactions of boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A test of the self-protective hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 256265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edens, J. F., Cavell, T. A., & Hughes, J. N. (1999). The self-systems of aggressive children: A cluster-analytic investigation. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 40, 441453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gresham, F. M., Lane, K. L., MacMillan, D. L., Bocian, K. M., & Ward, S. L. (2000). Effects of positive and negative illusory biases: Comparisons across social and academic self-concept domains. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 151175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harter, S. (1985). Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for Children. Unpublished manuscript, University of Denver.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1988). The Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. Unpublished manuscript, University of Denver.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of self: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Friendships and their developmental significance. In McGurk, H. (Ed.), Childhood and social development: Contemporary perspectives (pp. 175205). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P., March, J. S., Abikoff, H., Arnold, L. E., Cantwell, D. P., Conners, C. K., et al. (1997). Comprehensive assessment of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in the context of a multisite, multimodal clinical trial. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 217234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoza, B. (2007). Peer functioning in children with ADHD. Published jointly in Ambulatory Pediatrics, 7, 101–106, and Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 719727.Google Scholar
Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Pelham, W. E., Molina, B. S. G., et al. (2004). Self-perceptions of competence in children with ADHD and comparison children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 382391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoza, B., & Pelham, W. E. (1995). Social–cognitive predictors of treatment response in children with ADHD. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 2335.Google Scholar
Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Dobbs, J., Owens, J. S., & Pillow, D. R. (2002). Do boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 268278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, R. H., Reinecke, M. A., Gollan, J. K., & Kane, P. (2008). Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A cognitive science and developmental perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 759782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, N. M., Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., & Greiner, A. R. (2008). ADHD status and degree of positive illusions: Moderational and mediational relations with actual behavior. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 227238.Google ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M. (1985). The Children's Depression Inventory. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995998.Google ScholarPubMed
Lochman, J. E., Coie, J. D., Underwood, M. K., & Terry, R. (1993). Effectiveness of a social relations intervention program for aggressive and nonaggressive, rejected children. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 61, 10531058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeber, R., Green, S. M., & Lahey, B. B. (1990). Mental health professionals' perception of the utility of children, mothers, and teachers as informants on childhood psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 136143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. D., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2003). Studying change in dominance and bullying with Linear Mixed Models. School Psychology Review, 32, 401417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCulloch, C. E., & Searle, S. R. (2001). Generalized, linear, and mixed models. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescent-limited and life-course persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 10731086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, B. (1989). Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika, 54, 557585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2004). Mplus user's guide (4th ed.). Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Ohan, J. L., & Johnston, C. (2002). Are the performance overestimates given by boys with ADHD self-protective? Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 31, 230241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, J. S., Goldfine, M. E., Evangelista, N. M., Hoza, B., & Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 335351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and peer-perceived popularity: Two distinct dimensions of peer status. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Dulcan, M., Davies, M., Piacentini, J., Schwab-Stone, M., et al. (1996). The second version of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC-2). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 865877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 251268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. M. (1992). School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students. Irvine, CA: K.C. Publications.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Whitton, S. W., Larson, J. J., & Hauser, S. T. (2008). Depressive symptoms and bias in perceived social competence among young adults. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 791805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed