Skip to main content
Log in

Methodological Issues in the Direct Observation of Parent–Child Interaction: Do Observational Findings Reflect the Natural Behavior of Participants?

  • Published:
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This review examines evidence for the utility and validity of direct observational techniques for answering particular research and clinical questions. Observational techniques often involve recording behavior in settings that are relatively unnatural for families. However, it is argued that construct validity of observational methods depends partly on whether the findings are representative of participants' typical everyday behavior. Evidence is reviewed concerning whether observational findings are affected by the presence of the observer, and by two factors which have been neglected in the literature, namely the type of task imposed by the observer (e.g., directing parent and child to play rather than observing spontaneous interaction) and the location of the observations (e.g., clinic or laboratory rather than home). The review suggests that the presence of an observer does not necessarily distort the nature of interactions. However, the small number of studies in this area suggest that interactions in structured or artificial settings are not necessarily representative of those normally taking place at home.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Arnold, D., O'Leary, S., Wolff, L., & Acker, M. (1993). The Parenting Scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychological Assessment, 5, 137-144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. (2nd edn.) Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. (1963). The stream of behavior. NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1989). Hyperactive girls and boys: stimulant drug effects on mother-child interactions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 379-391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Karlsson, J., Pollard, S., & Murphy, J. (1985). Developmental changes in the mother-child interactions of hyperactive boys: effects of two dose levels of Ritalin. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 705-717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Befera, M. S., & Barkley, R. A. (1985). Hyperactive and normal girls and boys: mother-child interaction, parent psychiatric status and child psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 439-453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J. (1980). Mother-infant interaction at home and in the laboratory: a comparative study. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 37-47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernal, M. E., Gibson, D. M., William, D. E., & Pesses, D. I. (1971). A device for automatic audio tape recording. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4, 151-156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton-Jones (ed.) (1972). Ethological studies of child behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool children: a review of recent research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 113-149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capaldi, D., Chamberlain, P., & Patterson, G. (1997). Ineffective discipline and conduct problems in males: association, late adolescent outcomes, and prevention. Aggression and violent behavior, 2, 343-353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cone, J. D. (1982). Validity of direct observation assessment procedures. In D. P. Hartmann (Ed.), Using Observers to Study Behavior (pp. 67-79). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology, 26, 961-971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1996). Physical discipline among African American and European American mothers: Links to children's externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1065-1072.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dishion, T. J., Burraston, B., & Li, F. (in press). A multimethod and multitrait analysis of family management practices: Convergent and predictive validity. In B. Bukowski & Z. Amsel (Eds.) Handbook for drug abuse prevention theory, science, and practice. New York: Plenum.

  • Dishion, T. J., French, D. C., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). The development and ecology of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, pp. 421-470). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dowdney, L., Mrazek, D., Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1984). Observation of parent-child interaction with two-to-three year olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 379-409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dumas, J. E. (1987). INTERACT-A computer-based coding and data management system to assess family interactions. In R. J. Prinz (Ed.), Advances in Behavioral Assessment of Children and Families, 3, 177-203.

  • Dumas, J. E., LaFreniere, P. J., & Serketich, W. J. (1995). “Balance of power”: A transactional analysis of control in mother-child dyads involving socially competent aggressive, and anxious children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 104-113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Stocker, C., & Plomin, R. (1990). Assessing the relationship between young siblings: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 983-991.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1982). Siblings: Love, envy and understanding. London: Grant McIntyre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1980). The arrival of a sibling: changes in patterns of interaction between mother and first-born child. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 119-132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, J. M., Dishion, T., & Stoolmiller, M. (1998). The analysis of intervention change in children and families: Methodological and conceptual issues embedded in intervention studies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 53-71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, J. M., Leve, L., & Fagot, B. (2000). Coercive family processes: A replication and extension of Patterson's Coercion Model. Aggressive Behavior, in press.

  • Fergusson, D., Lynskey, M., & Horwood, L. (1993). The effect of maternal depression on maternal ratings of child behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 245-271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forehand, & McMahon (1981). Helping the noncompliant child. NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forehand, R., Wells, K. C., & Sturgis, E. T. (1978). Predictors of child noncompliant behavior in the home. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, F. (1987). Positive interaction between mothers and children with conduct problems: Is there training for harmony as well as fighting? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 283-293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, F. (1989). Inconsistent parenting: Is there evidence for a link with children's conduct problems? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 223-233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, F. (1994). The quality of joint activity between mothers and their children with behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 935-948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, F., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Sayal, K. (1999a). Parents anticipating misbehaviour: An observational study of strategies parents use to prevent conflict with behavior problem children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1185-1196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, F., Burton, J., Wilson, C. (1999b). Positive parenting style: How does it influence the early development of conduct problems? Paper presented at meeting of International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, Barcelona, June 1999.

  • Gardner, F., Burton, J., Wilson, C., & Ward, S. (2000). Parent-child interaction and pre-school conduct problems: how consistent is behavior observed in different settings in the home? Manuscript in preparation.

  • Harris, F. C., & Lahey, B. B. (1982). Subject reactivity in direct observational assessment: a review and critical analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 2, 523-538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, D. P., & Wood, D. D. (1990). Observational methods. In: A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. E. Kazdin, (Eds.), International handbook of behavior modification and therapy (pp. 107-138). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P., Simmel, C., & Heller, T. L. (1995). Multimethod assessment of covert antisocial behavior in children: laboratory observations, adult ratings, and child self-report. Psychological Assessment, 7, 209-219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holden, G. W. (1983). Avoiding conflict: Mothers as tacticians in the supermarket. Child Development, 54, 233-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hops, H., Davis, B., & Langoria, N. (1995). Methodological issues in direct observation: Illustrations with the Living in Familial Environments (LIFE) coding system.. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24, 193-203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hops, H., Biglan, A., Sherman, L., Arthur, J., Friedman, L., & Osteen, V. (1987). Home observations of family interactions of depressed women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 341-346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M., Carmichael, H., Pinkerton, G., & Tizard, B. (1979). Recording children's conversations at home and at nursery school: a technique and some methodological considerations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, 225-232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H. M., & Haynes, S. N. (1978) Structured laboratory observation in the behavioral assessment of parent-child interactions: a methodological critique. Behavior Therapy, 9, 428-447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutt, S. J., & Hutt, C. (1970). Direct observation and measurement of behavior. Springfield IL: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, T., Tennenbaum, D., Seilhamer, R. A., Bargiel, K. et al. (1994). Reactivity effects during naturalistic observation of distressed and nondistressed families. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 354-363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. M., & Bolstad, O. D. (1975). Reactivity to home observation: a comparison of audio recorded behavior with observers present or absent. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 181-185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. M., & Bolstad, O. D. (1973). Methodological issues in naturalistic observations: Some problems and solutions for field research. In L. A. Hamerlynck, L. C. Handy, & E. J. Mash (Eds.), Behavior change: Methodology, concepts and practice. (pp. 7-67). Champaign, IL: Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Observer effects: reactivity of direct observation. In D. P. Hartmann (Ed.), Using observers to study behavior. (pp. 5-19). San Francisco: Jossey

    Google Scholar 

  • Kier, C. (1996). How natural is “naturalistic” home observation? Observer reactivity in infant-sibling interaction. Proceedings of the British Psychological Society, 4, 79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kniskern, J. R., Robinson, E. A., & Mitchell, S. K. (1983). Mother-child interaction in home and laboratory settings. Child Study Journal, 13, 23-39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (1995). Mother-child mutually positive affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development, 66, 236-254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1989). Correspondence between mothers' self-reported and observed child rearing practices. Child Development, 60, 56-63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C., Kier, C., Hyder, C., Prenderville, N., Pullen, J., & Stephens, A. (1996). Observer influences on fathers and mothers: An experimental manipulation of the structure and function of parent-infant conversation. Early Development and Parenting, 5, 57-68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1994). Developmental approaches to aggression and conduct problems. In: M. L. Rutter & D. Hay (Eds.), Development through life: a handbook for clinicians (pp. 488-516). Oxford, UK: Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  • Margolin, G., Oliver, P. H., Gordis, E. B., O'Hearn, H. G., Medina, A. M., Ghosh, C. M., & Morland, L. (1998). The nuts and bolts of behavioral observation of marital and family interaction. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 195-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mash, E. J., & Barkley, R. (1986). Assessment of family interaction with the Response Class Matrix.. In R. J. Prinz (Ed.), Advances in Behavioral Assessment of Children and Families, 2, 29-67.

  • Mitchell, S. K. (1979). Interobserver agreement, reliability, and generalizability of data collected in observational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 36, 376-390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. L., & Foster, S. L. (1991). Assessing childhood noncompliance with parental requests: current status and future directions. In R. J. Prinz (Ed.), Advances in Behavioral Assessment of Children and Families, 5, 139-170.

  • Parten, M. (1932). Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 243-269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia. (Chapter 3: Observations of family process).

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson. G. R., & Bank, L. (1986). Bootstrapping your way in the nomological thicket. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 49-73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson. G. R., & Forgatch, M. S. (1995). Predicting future clinical adjustment from treatment outcome and process variables. Psychological Assessment, 7, 275-285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson. G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial Boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peed, S., Roberts, M., & Forehand, R. (1977). Evaluation of the effectiveness of a standardized parent training program in altering the interaction of mothers and their non-compliant children. Behavior Modification, 1, 323-350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1995). A peek behind the fence: naturalistic observations of aggressive children with remote audio-visual recording. Developmental Psychology, 31, 548-553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pett, M. A., Wampold, B. E., Vaughan-Cole, B., & East, T. D. (1992). Consistency of behaviors within a naturalistic setting: an examination of the impact of context and repeated observations on mother-child interactions. Behavioral Assessment, 14, 367-385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, A., Bank, L., Reid, J., Knutson, J., Burraston, B., & Eddy, M. (2000). The veridicality of punitive childhood experiences reported by adolescents and young adults. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, in press.

  • Rapoport, J. L., & Benoit, M. (1975). The relation of direct home observations to the clinic evaluation of hyperactive school age boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 141-147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, J. (1993). Prevention of conduct disorder before and after school entry: Relating interventions to developmental findings. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 243-262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richters, J. (1992). Depressed mothers as informants about their children: A critical review of the evidence for distortion. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 485-499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M. W., & Powers, S. W. (1988). The Compliance Test. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 375-398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, E. A., & Eyberg, S. M. (1981). The dyadic parent-child interaction coding system: standardization and validation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 245-250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, A., Russell, G., & Midwinter, D. (1992). Observer influences on mothers and fathers: self reported influence during a home observation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 263-283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, G. (Ed) (1978). Observing behavior (Vol. 2): Data collection and analysis methods. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. (1994b). Developmental precursors of externalizing behavior: ages 1 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 355-364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Cohn, J. F., & Bell, R. Q. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95-107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoolmiller, M., Eddy, J. M., & Reid, J. B. (2000). Detecting and describing preventive intervention effects in a universal school-based randomized trial targeting delinquent and violent behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 296-306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stormshak, E. A., Speltz, M. L., DeKlyen, M., & Greenberg, M. T. (1997). Observed family interaction during clinical interviews: a comparison of families containing preschool boys with and without disruptive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 345-357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wahler, R., House, A., & Stambaugh, E (1976). Ecological assessment of child problem behavior. NY: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasik, B. (1989). The systematic observation of children: Rediscovery and advances. Behavioral Assessment, 11, 201-217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C. (1985). Comparisons of behavior transactions between conduct-disordered children and their mothers in the clinic and at home. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 169-184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C., Kolpacoff, M., & Hollinsworth, T. (1989). The long-term effectiveness and clinical significance of three cost-effective training programs for families with conduct problem children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 550-553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C., & Spitzer, A. (1991). Development, reliability, and validity of the daily telephone discipline interview. Behavioral Assessment, 13, 221-239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C. (1994). Advancing videotape parent training: A comparison study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 583-593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in head start children: strengthening parenting competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, in press.

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Iannotti, R. J., Cummings, E. M., & Denham, S. (1990). Antecedents of problem behaviors in children of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 271-291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zangwill, W. M., & Kniskern, J. R. (1982). Comparison of problem families in the clinic and at home. Behavior Therapy, 13, 145-152.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gardner, F. Methodological Issues in the Direct Observation of Parent–Child Interaction: Do Observational Findings Reflect the Natural Behavior of Participants?. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 3, 185–198 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009503409699

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009503409699

Navigation