Skip to main content
Short Research Report

Buffer against Cumulative Stress

Positive Age Self-Stereotypes Predict Lower Cortisol across 30 Years

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000149

Abstract. Prolonged elevation of cortisol, the primary stress biomarker, is associated with impaired cognitive and physical health. Cortisol tends to increase in later life among most, but not all, older individuals. The current study considered whether this pattern could be explained by more-positive age stereotypes acting as a stress buffer. The 439 participants drawn from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging provided 1,789 cortisol measurements, from 24-h collections of urine, across 30 years. Among those aged 50 or greater, the cortisol of the more-negative age-stereotype group increased by 44%, whereas the more-positive age-stereotype group showed no increase. Also as expected, there was no association of age stereotypes and cortisol level among the younger participants, for whom the age stereotypes were self-irrelevant. The findings indicate the importance of considering the relationship between both positive and negative stereotypes and stress biomarkers over time.

References

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6. 1173 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bolignano, D., Mattace-Raso, F., Sijbrands, E. J., & Zoccali, C. (2014). The aging kidney revisited: A systematic review. Ageing Research Review, 14, 65–80. doi 10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. doi 10.1037/ 0033-2909.130.3.355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Esoterix Laboratory Services (2016). Retrieved from http://www. esoterix.com/endocrinology-services First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Evans, G. W. (2003). A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 924–933. doi 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferrucci, L. (2008). The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA): A 50-year-long journey and plans for the future. Journal of Gerontology: Biological and Medical Sciences, 63, 1416–1419. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hatcher, L. A. (1994). Step-by-step approach to using the SAS system for factor analysis and structural equation modeling. Cary, NC: SAS Institute. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hehman, J. A., & Bugental, D. B., (2015). Responses to patronizing communication and factors that attenuate those responses. Psychology and Aging, 30, 552–560. doi 10.1037/pag0000041. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Statham, J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: Do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychology and Aging, 19, 495–505. doi 10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.495 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horton, S., Baker, J., Pearce, G. W., & Deakin, J. M. (2008). On the malleability of performance: Implications for seniors. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 27, 446–465. doi 10.1177/0733464808315291 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jung, C., Greco, S., Nguyen, H. H. T., Ho, J. T., Lewis, J. G., Torpy, D. J., & Inder, W. J. (2014). Plasma, salivary and urinary cortisol levels following physiological and stress doses of hydrocortisone in normal volunteers. BMC Endocrine Disorders, 14, 91. doi 10.1186/1472-6823-14-91 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Juster, R. P., McEwen, B. S., & Lupien, S. J. (2010). Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 2–16. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1092–1107. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1092 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 332–336. doi 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Ashman, O., & Dror, I. (1999–2000). To be or not to be: The effects of aging stereotypes on the will to live. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 40, 409–420. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Ferrucci, L., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D., Troncoso, J., & Resnick, S. M. (2016). A culture–brain link: Negative age stereotypes predict Alzheimer’s-Disease biomarkers. Psychology and Aging, 31, 82–88. doi 10.1037/pag0000062 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Hausdorff, J. M., Hencke, R., & Wei, J. Y. (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55, 205–213. doi 10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Kasl, S. V., & Gill, T. M. (2004). Image of Aging Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 208–210. doi 10.2466/pms.99.1.208-210 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China and among the American Deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 989–997. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.989 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Pilver, C., Chung, P. H., & Slade, M. D. (2014). Subliminal strengthening: Improving older individuals’ physical function over time with an implicit-age-stereotype intervention. Psychological Science, 25, 2127–2135. doi 10.1177/0956797614551970 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Chung, M. D., & Gill, T. M. (2015). Resiliency over time of elders’ age stereotypes after encountering stressful events. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70, 886–890. doi 10.1093/geronb/gbu082? First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Murphy, T. E., & Gill, T. M. (2012). Association between positive age stereotypes and recovery from disability in older persons. JAMA, 308, 1972–1973. doi 10.1001/jama. 2012.14541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D., & Ferrucci, L. (2009). Age stereotypes held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological Science, 20, 296–298. doi 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02298.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lupien, S., Lecours, A., Schwartz, G., Sharma, S., Hauger, R. L., Meaney, M. J., & Nair, N. P. V. (1996). Longitudinal study of basal cortisol levels in healthy elderly subjects: Evidence for subgroups. Neurobiology of Aging, 17, 95–105. doi 10.1016/0197-4580(95)02005-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marques, S., Lima, M., Abrams, D., & Swift, H. (2014). Will to live in older people’s medical decisions: Immediate and delayed effects of aging stereotypes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44, 399–408. doi 10.1111/jasp.12231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McEwen, B. S., Bowles, N. P., Gray, J. D., Hill, M. N., Hunter, R. G., Karatsoreos, K. N., & Nasca, C. (2015). Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 1353–1363. doi 10.1038/nn.4086 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McEwen, B. S., & Morrison, J. H. (2013). The brain on stress: Vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron, 79, 16–29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meisner, B. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of positive and negative age stereotype priming effects on behavior among older adults. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67, 13–17. doi 10.1093/geronb/gbr062 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nicolson, N. A. (2008). Measurement of cortisol. In L. J. LueckenL. C. GalloEds., Handbook of physiological research methods in health psychology (pp. 37–74). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Otte, C., Hart, S., Neylan, T. C., Marmar, C. R., Yaffe, K., & Mohr, D. C. (2005). A meta-analysis of cortisol response to challenge in human aging: Importance of gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 80–91. doi 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.06.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Otte, C., Marmar, C. R., Pipkin, S. S., Moos, R., Browner, W. S., & Whooley, M. A. (2004). Depression and 24-h urinary cortisol in medical outpatients with coronary heart disease: The Heart and Soul Study. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 241–247. doi 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004. 06.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi 10.1177/014662167700100306 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robertson, D. A., Savva, G. M., King-Kallimanis, B. L., & Kenny, R. A. (2015). Negative perceptions of aging and decline in walking speed: A self-fulfilling prophecy. PLoS One, 10, e0123260. doi 10.1371/journal.pone.0123260 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rueggeberg, R., Wrosch, C., & Miller, G. E. (2012). Sleep duration buffers diurnal cortisol increases in older adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 1029–1038. doi 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sargent-Cox, K. A., Anstey, K. J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2012). The relationship between change in self-perceptions of aging and physical functioning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 27, 750–760. doi 10.1037/a0027578 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shock, N. W., Greulich, R. C., Andres, R., Arenberg, D., Costa, P. T., Lakatta, E. G., & Tobin, J. D. (1984). Normal human aging: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, C. J. (2012). A sociological overview of cortisol as a biomarker of response to the social environment. Sociology Compass, 6, 434–444. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tuckman, J., & Irving, L. (1953). Attitudes toward old people. The Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 249–260. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., & Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men. JAMA, 284, 861–868. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wester, V. L., Lamberts, S. W., & van Rossum, E. F. (2014). Advances in the assessment of cortisol exposure and sensitivity. Current Opinion in Endocrinology, Diabetes, and Obesity, 21, 306–311. doi 10.1097/MED.0000000000000077 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westerhof, G. J., Miche, M., Brothers, A. F., Barrett, A. E., Diehl, M., Montepare, J. M., ... Wurm, S. (2014). The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: A meta-analysis of longitudinal data. Psychology and Aging, 29, 793–802. doi 10.1037/a0038016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wurm, S., & Benyamini, Y. (2014). Optimism buffers the detrimental effect of negative self-perceptions of ageing on physical and mental health. Psychology and Health, 29, 832–848. doi 10.1080/08870446.2014.891737 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • . Yehuda, R., Halligan, S. L., Yang, R. K., Song Guo, L., Makotkinee, I., Singh, B., & Pickholtz, D. (2003). Relationship between 24-h urinary-free cortisol excretion and salivary cortisol levels sampled from awakening to bedtime in healthy subjects. Life Sciences, 73, 349–358. doi 10.1016/S0024-3205(03)00286-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar