Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural activity predicts attitude change in cognitive dissonance

Abstract

When our actions conflict with our prior attitudes, we often change our attitudes to be more consistent with our actions. This phenomenon, known as cognitive dissonance, is considered to be one of the most influential theories in psychology. However, the neural basis of this phenomenon is unknown. Using a Solomon four-group design, we scanned participants with functional MRI while they argued that the uncomfortable scanner environment was nevertheless a pleasant experience. We found that cognitive dissonance engaged the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula; furthermore, we found that the activation of these regions tightly predicted participants' subsequent attitude change. These effects were not observed in a control group. Our findings elucidate the neural representation of cognitive dissonance, and support the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in detecting cognitive conflict and the neural prediction of attitude change.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Trial sequence during the task.
Figure 2: fMRI results.
Figure 3: Time courses of activation of the left and right dACC and anterior insula (AI).
Figure 4: Partial correlation plots between activation and final attitude score, controlling for mean reaction time, pretest presence and pretest score.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Stanford University Press, Stanford, California, 1957).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Festinger, L. & Carlsmith, J.M. Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 58, 203–210 (1959).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Harmon-Jones, E. & Mills, J. Cognitive Dissonance: Progress on a Pivotal Theory in Social Psychology (American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1999).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. Jones, E.E. Major developments in social psychology during the past five decades. in Handbook of Social Psychology: Vol. 1. Theory and Methods (eds Lindzey, G. & Aronson, E.) 47–107 (Random House, New York, 1985).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Losch, D.E. & Cacioppo, J.T. Cognitive dissonance may enhance sympathetic tonus, but attitudes are changed to reduce negative affect rather than arousal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 26, 289–304 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Harmon-Jones, E. Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biol. Psychol. 67, 51–76 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Elliot, A.J. & Devine, P.G. On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: dissonance as psychological discomfort. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 382–394 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Croyle, R. & Cooper, J. Dissonance arousal: physiological evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 782–791 (1983).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Linder, D.E., Cooper, J. & Jones, E.E. Decision freedom as a determinant of the role of incentive magnitude in attitude change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 6, 245–254 (1967).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hoyt, M.F., Henley, M.D. & Collins, B.E. Studies in forced compliance: confluence of choice and consequence on attitude change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 23, 205–210 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Harmon-Jones, E., Gerdjikov, T. & Harmon-Jones, C. The effect of induced compliance on relative left frontal cortical activity: a test of the action-based model of dissonance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 35–45 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Cooper, J. & Fazio, R.H. A new look at dissonance theory. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (ed. Berkowitz, L.) 229–264 (Academic Press, Orlando, Florida, 1984).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S. & Cohen, J.D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. van Veen, V. & Carter, C.S. Conflict and cognitive control in the brain. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 237–240 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Botvinick, M.M., Cohen, J.D. & Carter, C.S. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 539–546 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kerns, J.G. et al. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. van Veen, V. & Carter, C.S. Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 27, 497–504 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jones, A.D., Cho, R.Y., Nystrom, L.E., Cohen, J.D. & Braver, T.S. A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: effects of frequency, sequence and conflict. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 300–317 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Van Overwalle, F. & Jordens, K. An adaptive connectionist model of cognitive dissonance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 204–231 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Shultz, T.R. & Lepper, M.R. Cognitive dissonance reduction as constraint satisfaction. Psychol. Rev. 103, 219–240 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Braver, M.C.W. & Braver, S.L. Statistical treatment of the Solomon four-group approach: a meta-analytic approach. Psychol. Bull. 104, 150–154 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Solomon, R.L. An extension of control group designs. Psychol. Bull. 46, 137–150 (1949).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Critchley, H.D. Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective and cognitive integration. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 154–166 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Critchley, H.D. et al. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 126, 2139–2152 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Hajcak, G., McDonald, N. & Simons, R.F. Error-related psychophysiology and negative affect. Brain Cogn. 56, 189–197 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Eisenberger, N.I., Lieberman, M.D. & Williams, K.D. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Luu, P., Collins, P. & Tucker, D.M. Mood, personality and self-monitoring: negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129, 43–60 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Craig, A.D., Reiman, E.M., Evans, A.C. & Bushnell, M.C. Functional imaging of an illusion of pain. Nature 384, 258–260 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Nuñez, J.M., Casey, B.J., Egner, T., Hare, T. & Hirsch, J. Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control. Neuroimage 25, 267–277 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Langleben, D.D. et al. Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 15, 727–732 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Westen, D., Blagov, P.S., Harenski, K., Kilts, C. & Hamann, S. Neural bases of motivated reasoning: an fMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. presidential election. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1947–1958 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Harmon-Jones, E. & Harmon-Jones, C. Action-based model of dissonance: a review of behavioral, anterior cingulate, and prefrontal cortical mechanisms. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 1518–1538 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Bem, D.J. Self-perception: an alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychol. Rev. 74, 183–200 (1967).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Kirchhoff, B.A., Wagner, A.D., Maril, A. & Stern, C.E. Prefrontal circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J. Neurosci. 20, 6173–6180 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Wagner, A.D. et al. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281, 1188–1191 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Brewer, J.B., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H. & Gabrieli, J.D. Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. Science 281, 1185–1187 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Lieberman, M.D., Ochsner, K.N., Gilbert, D.T. & Schacter, D.L. Do amnesics exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role of explicit memory and attention in attitude change. Psychol. Sci. 12, 135–140 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Ursu, S. & Carter, C.S. Outcome representations, counterfactual comparisons and the human orbitofrontal cortex: implications for neuroimaging studies of decision-making. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 23, 51–60 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Amodio, D.M. & Frith, C.D. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Ursu, S., Clark, K.A., Stenger, V.A. & Carter, C.S. Distinguishing expected negative outcomes from preparatory control in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Res. 1227, 110–119 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Coricelli, G. et al. Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1255–1262 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Takahashi, H. et al. When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science 323, 937–939 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Singer, T. et al. Empathy for pain involves the affective, but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Immordino-Yang, M.H., McColl, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 8021–8026 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Hein, G. & Singer, T. I feel how you feel but not always: the empathic brain and its modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 153–158 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Lamm, C., Batson, C.D. & Decety, J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 42–58 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Batson, C.D. et al. “... As you would have them do unto you”: does imagining yourself in the other's place stimulate moral action? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1190–1201 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Sharot, T., De Martino, B. & Dolan, R.J. How choice reveals and shapes expected hedonic outcome. J. Neurosci. 29, 3760–3765 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Delgado, M.R., Locke, H.M., Stenger, V.A. & Fiez, J.A. Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: effects of valence and magnitude manipulations. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 27–38 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Goebel, R., Esposito, F. & Formisano, E. Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with BrainVoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 392–401 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to M. Greenberg, G. Hein and K. Ochsner for discussions, and to S. Ravizza, D. Tamir, S. Ursu and A. Watrous for help with data collection. This research was supported by grants from the US National Institute of Mental Health and the Burroughs-Wellcome Foundation to C.S.C.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

V.v.V., J.W.S. and C.S.C. designed the study. M.K.K. gathered fMRI data and administered questionnaires. V.v.V. developed stimuli, gathered pilot data and conducted analyses. V.v.V. wrote the paper with contributions from M.K.K., J.W.S. and C.S.C.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Vincent van Veen or Cameron S Carter.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Results, Methods, Discussion, Figure 1 and Tables 1,2 (PDF 392 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Veen, V., Krug, M., Schooler, J. et al. Neural activity predicts attitude change in cognitive dissonance. Nat Neurosci 12, 1469–1474 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2413

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2413

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing