Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Molecular Autism 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Short report

Prenatal versus postnatal sex steroid hormone effects on autistic traits in children at 18 to 24 months of age

verfasst von: Bonnie Auyeung, Jag Ahluwalia, Lynn Thomson, Kevin Taylor, Gerald Hackett, Kieran J O’Donnell, Simon Baron-Cohen

Erschienen in: Molecular Autism | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

Studies of prenatal exposure to sex steroid hormones predict autistic traits in children at 18 to 24 and at 96 months of age. However, it is not known whether postnatal exposure to these hormones has a similar effect. This study compares prenatal and postnatal sex steroid hormone levels in relation to autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children.
Fetal testosterone (fT) and fetal estradiol (fE) levels were measured in amniotic fluid from pregnant women (n = 35) following routine second-trimester amniocentesis. Saliva samples were collected from these children when they reached three to four months of age and were analyzed for postnatal testosterone (pT) levels. Mothers were asked to complete the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT), a measure of autistic traits in children 18 to 24 months old.

Finding

fT (but not pT) levels were positively associated with scores on the Q-CHAT. fE and pT levels showed no sex differences and no relationships with fT levels. fT levels were the only variable that predicted Q-CHAT scores.

Conclusions

These preliminary findings are consistent with the hypothesis that prenatal (but not postnatal) androgen exposure, coinciding with the critical period for sexual differentiation of the brain, is associated with the development of autistic traits in 18 to 24 month old toddlers. However, it is recognized that further work with a larger sample population is needed before the effects of postnatal androgen exposure on autistic traits can be ruled out. These results are also in line with the fetal androgen theory of autism, which suggests that prenatal, organizational effects of androgen hormones influence the development of autistic traits in later life.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​2040-2392-3-17) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

BA conceived of and carried out the study and drafted the manuscript. JA and LT enabled the follow-ups of the infants. KT carried out the amniotic fluid radioimmunoassays. GH participated in the recruitment of women who had had an amniocentesis. KJO carried out the salivary immunoassays. SBC contributed to the study design and drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
ASC
Autism Spectrum Conditions
fE
Fetal estradiol
fT
Fetal testosterone
pT
Postnatal testosterone
Q-CHAT
Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers.

Background

Autism Spectrum Conditions (ASC) occur more often in males than females[1]. This sex difference may provide an important clue to the etiology of the condition, though the true sex ratio in ASC is unclear given the known difficulties in the diagnosis of ASC in females[2]. ASC have strong neurobiological and genetic components[3]; however, the specific factors (hormonal, genetic or environmental) that are responsible for the higher risk of ASC in males remain unclear.
Hormones perform critical functions in both neural and physical development throughout life, from pregnancy to adolescence and beyond[4]. There are several lines of evidence that suggest that prenatal exposure to hormones, in particular fetal testosterone (fT), play a role in the development of ASC. This includes the finding that elevated fT is related to reduced eye contact[5], slower language development[6], poorer quality of social relationships and narrower interests[7], and reduced empathy[8]. Higher fT levels also predict increased systemizing ability[9], and more autistic traits in school-age children[10] and in toddlers[11].
To date, no studies have tested if there is also a link between postnatal testosterone and the development of autistic traits in later life. A possible window of interest lies between three and four months of age, when a second peak in testosterone levels is thought to occur[12]. This second peak may play a significant role in physical[13] and behavioral development[14]. In addition to testosterone, the estrogen hormone estradiol is another hormone that is synthesized prenatally from testosterone. Estradiol is considered to be the most biologically active estrogen[15].
The aim of this study is to compare if prenatal and postnatal sex steroid hormone levels in the same individuals are associated with differences in autistic traits at 18 to 24 months of age, using the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT).

Methods

Participants

Mothers were asked for consent to participate in research at the time of having an amniocentesis. Medical records of approximately 700 patients who had undergone amniocentesis in the Cambridge region between January 2004 and July 2006 were examined. Participants were excluded if: (a) amniocentesis revealed a chromosomal abnormality; (b) the pregnancy ended in miscarriage or termination; (c) the child suffered neonatal or infant death; (d) the child suffered significant medical problems after birth; (e) there was a twin pregnancy or (f) the relevant information was absent from medical records. General Practitioners were asked for consent to contact the women, and mothers with babies less than four months of age were invited for participation (n = 104). This study was given ethical approval by the National Health Service Suffolk Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from General Practitioners and participating parents.
A total of n = 47 (22 boys, 25 girls) mothers brought their children in for saliva collection, where a sample of passive drool was taken when their child reached three-months of age. Twelve samples had insufficient amounts of saliva, with a total of 35 samples (15 boys, 20 girls) remaining that were eligible for analysis. Saliva was collected using a suction machine under the supervision of a trained health professional. The families were contacted when their child reached 18 months, and were asked to complete the Q-CHAT[16]. Parents were blind to both the fetal and postnatal hormone levels of their child.

Outcome variable

The Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT) is a 25-item parent-report screening measure developed to identify toddlers at risk for the development of ASC[16]. A total score is obtained with a minimum score of 0 and a maximum score of 100[16].

Hormone measurements

Fetal testosterone (fT) and fetal estradiol (fE) levels were measured by radioimmunoassay in amniotic fluid. See Auyeung et al. (2010) for details. Postnatal testosterone (pT) levels were assayed (without separation or extraction) for testosterone from saliva samples using commercially available immunoassay protocols (Salimetrics, State College, PA, USA). Average intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 10% and 15%, respectively. A serial dilution was performed (2.5) to give a standard curve with greater sensitivity at lower ranges.

Demographic variables

Gestational age at amniocentesis (in weeks), maternal age, parental education level, presence of older siblings and the child’s age were included as control variables.

Finding

Levels of fE were positively skewed (skewness >1) so natural logarithmic transformed data were used in subsequent analyses. Distributions of the other variables (including fT) were not skewed. The ranges of the Q-CHAT scores obtained in this sample were consistent with scores from a large sample of typically developing children[16]. See Table 1 for descriptive information and Table 2 for correlations.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics
 
Combined group
Girls
Boys
 
Variable
n
M
SD
Range
n
M
SD
Range
n
M
SD
range
Cohen’s D
fT level (nmol/L)**
35
0.58
0.45
0.05 to 2.28
20
0.34
0.23
0.05 to 0.95
15
0.91
0.47
0.42 to 2.28
1.54
fE level (nmol/L)
35
0.28
0.18
0.14 to 1.15
20
0.24
0.08
0.14 to 0.45
15
0.34
0.25
0.18 to 1.15
0.54
pT level (nmol/L)
35
0.17
0.06
0.02 to 0.29
20
0.18
0.07
0.02 to 0.29
15
0.16
0.05
0.04 to 0.23
0.31
Gestational age (weeks)
35
16.70
1.47
13 to 21.2
19
16.62
0.90
15 to 18.1
14
16.82
2.04
13 to 21.2
0.13
Child age (months)
35
19.34
2.93
18 to 35
20
19.10
1.33
18 to 23
15
19.67
4.27
18 to 35
0.18
Maternal age (years)
35
36.54
4.74
21 to 46
20
37.75
3.84
33 to 46
15
34.93
5.44
21 to 45
0.60
Parental education (1–5)
35
3.41
0.90
1.5 to 5
20
3.30
0.89
1.5 to 4.5
15
3.57
0.92
2 to 5
0.30
Q-CHAT score**
35
27.03
8.62
10 to 43
20
23.95
8.62
10 to 43
15
31.13
6.94
14 to 40
0.92
*Indicates raw values.
* Sex difference is significant at the P <0.05 level.
** Sex difference is significant at the P <0.01 level.
Table 2
Correlation matrix for all cases
 
fT level
fE level
pT level
Sex
Gest. age
Child age
Matr. age
Parent ed
Older sister
Older brother
fE Level
.03
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
pT Level
.13
-.11
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
Sex
.72**
.28
-.13
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
Gestational age
.17
-.47**
.10
.04
--
--
--
--
--
--
Child age
-.08
.22
-.21
-.09
-.10
--
--
--
--
--
Maternal age
-.40**
-.09
-.18
-.28
.10
.10
--
--
--
--
Parent education
.03
.14
.31
.09
-.12
.02
.01
--
--
--
Older sister
-.12
-.18
-.09
-.14
.02
.38
.32
-.07
--
--
Older brother
-.08
.05
-.31
.02
-.10
.18
-.04
.17
.03
--
Q-CHAT score
.56**
.17
-.09
.46**
-.10
.18
-.32
.09
-.01
.15
* P <0.05.
** P <0.01.
Due to the small sample sizes, nonparametric statistics were used to examine sex differences and correlation values. Mann–Whitney U tests showed no significant sex differences in pT levels (Mann–Whitney U = 127.5, P >0.05) or fE levels (Mann–Whitney U = 101.0, P >0.05). Significant sex differences were found for fT levels (Mann–Whitney U = 24.0, P <0.001) and for Q-CHAT score (Mann–Whitney U = 70.5, P <0.01). fT level and sex were also significantly related (Spearman’s rho = 0.72, P <0.001). However, no relationships between fT levels and fE levels (Spearman’s rho = 0.03, P >0.05) were found. In addition, no relationship was observed between pT levels and sex (Spearman’s rho = −0.13, P >0.05), between fT and pT levels (Spearman’s rho = 0.13, P >0.05) or between pT levels and fE levels (Spearman’s rho = −0.11, P >0.05). A significant relationship was observed between fE levels and gestational age, but neither of these variables were related to Q-CHAT scores.
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was used. In the first stage any predictor variable showing a significant correlation with the outcome at P <0.20 was included using the enter method[17]. Maternal age was the only variable that met the P <0.20 criterion for entry into the first stage (Spearman’s rho = −0.32, P <0.20). The presence of suppressor variables (predictors that are highly correlated with other predictors at P <0.01) was investigated. However, no suppressor variables were observed. The main effects of fT level, pT level and child sex were tested for inclusion in the second stage using the stepwise method (entry criterion P <0.05, removal criterion P >0.10). The interaction between child sex, fT level and the fT/pT interaction were tested for inclusion in the third stage using the stepwise method (entry criterion P <0.05, removal criterion P >0.10). fT level was the only variable included in the final regression model and produced a significant F-change (F-change = 8.82, P <0.01, ΔR2 = 0.21). Table 3 shows the regression results for boys and girls together. See Figure 1 for plots of the relationships between fT and Q-CHAT scores and pT and Q-CHAT scores.
Table 3
Final regression model for Q-CHAT scores
  
Final regression model
Outcome
Predictors
R2
Δ R2
B
SE
β
Sig
Group
Q-CHAT Score
Maternal age
0.05
0.05
0.07
0.30
0.04
P >0.05
 
fT level
0.26
0.21
11.20
3.77
0.49
P <0.001
Within sex correlations are shown in Table 4. A significant correlation between fT levels and Q-CHAT scores was found for both boys (Spearman’s rho = 0.51, P <0.05) and girls (Spearman’s rho = 0.57, P <0.05). No significant relationships between fE, pT or any other demographic variables and Q-CHAT scores were found in boys or girls.
Table 4
Correlation matrix for girls (above diagonal) and boys (below diagonal)
 
fTlevel
fElevel
pT level
Gest. age
Child age
Matr. age
Parent ed
Older sister
Older brother
Q-CHAT score
fT Level (nmol/L)
--
−0.32
0.22
0.45
0.20
−0.14
0.10
0.25
0.09
0.57*
fE Level (nmol/L)
0.10
--
−0.40
−0.50*
0.41
0.13
0.17
−0.13
0.31
0.09
pT Level (nmol/L)
0.43
0.44
--
0.46*
−0.30
−0.41
0.60*
−0.31
−0.21
−0.01
Gestational Age
−0.19
−0.54*
−0.27
--
−0.29
0.00
0.17
0.02
−0.27
−0.21
Child Age
−0.22
−0.04
−0.10
0.21
--
0.13
0.13
0.39
0.24
0.35
Maternal Age
−0.44
−0.20
−0.04
0.30
−0.03
--
−0.13
0.10
−0.23
−0.03
Parent Education
−0.13
−0.07
−0.15
−0.37
−0.13
0.29
--
−0.16
0.18
0.18
Older Sister
−0.48
−0.16
0.27
0.10
0.26
0.59
0.19
--
−0.07
0.12
Older Brother
−0.38
−0.38
−0.42
0.08
0.12
0.25
0.13
0.21
--
0.33
Q-CHAT Score
0.51*
−0.23
−0.09
0.04
−0.13
−0.39
−0.19
−0.27
−0.05
--
* P <0.05.

Discussion

This study examined relationships among fetal testosterone (fT), estradiol (fE), postnatal testosterone (pT) and scores on the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT). Results confirmed earlier findings that sex differences in autistic traits are present in toddlers, with boys scoring much higher than girls[11, 16]. fT level was the only variable which was significantly related to Q-CHAT scores when both sexes were combined. However, no association was found between fT levels and fE levels, between Sex and fE levels (r = −0.02, P >0.05) or between Sex and pT levels. A correlation between fE levels and gestational age was found. However, neither of these variables was related to Q-CHAT scores suggesting that it is fT rather than fE or gestational age that predicts Q-CHAT scores. Levels of postnatal testosterone at three to four months of age were also 70% lower than in the second trimester and did not show a significant effect on Q-CHAT score at 18 to 24 months.
In a regression model, fT level was the only predictor retained in the final stage, whereas both Sex and the fT/Sex interaction were excluded; suggesting this could be an effect of fT, rather than Sex. Within Sex findings also show significant correlations between Q-CHAT score and fT level in boys and in girls. This corresponds with related findings in larger samples both within this age group and in older children[10, 11].
The relationship observed between Q-CHAT scores and fT level (but not pT level) provides preliminary support for the notion that fetal rather than postnatal exposure to testosterone is critical for the development of autistic traits in toddlers. However, it is recognized that this study has a limited sample size and that postnatal testosterone should not be discounted as a factor in the development of autistic traits until further testing of these associations in a larger sample of children.

Conclusions

The current study examined the link between prenatal and postnatal hormone levels and later autistic traits. The Q-CHAT is a validated measure of autistic traits, which has been shown to differentiate toddlers who go on to receive an ASC diagnosis later in childhood. Results showed that fT levels are positively related to Q-CHAT scores in this sample of typically developing children. The lack of any relationship between pT or fE levels and other variables (including Sex) indicates that fetal exposure to testosterone has a greater influence on the development of autistic traits than exposure to the same hormone after birth. The relationship between fT levels and Q-CHAT scores remains consistent with previous findings, despite the smaller sample of children in this study. However, findings relating to pT require validation in a larger study. Although higher levels of fT are unlikely to be the sole cause of autism, this work aids our understanding of the potential mechanisms that could contribute to the development of such conditions. These results are in line with the fetal androgen theory of ASC[18], and a large-scale collaborative project to test the role of prenatal androgens in ASC is underway.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants to SBC from the Nancy Lurie-Marks Family Foundation, the MRC UK and the Wellcome Trust. BA was supported by a scholarship from Trinity College, Cambridge. We are grateful to the families who have taken part in this longitudinal study over many years and to Rebecca Knickmeyer, Carrie Allison, Melissa Hines and Ieuan Hughes for valuable discussions. This study was conducted in association with the NIHR CLAHRC for Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

BA conceived of and carried out the study and drafted the manuscript. JA and LT enabled the follow-ups of the infants. KT carried out the amniotic fluid radioimmunoassays. GH participated in the recruitment of women who had had an amniocentesis. KJO carried out the salivary immunoassays. SBC contributed to the study design and drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillberg C, Cederlund M, Lamberg K, Zeijlon L: Brief report: “the autism epidemic”. The registered prevalence of autism in a Swedish urban area. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006, 363: 429-435.CrossRef Gillberg C, Cederlund M, Lamberg K, Zeijlon L: Brief report: “the autism epidemic”. The registered prevalence of autism in a Swedish urban area. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006, 363: 429-435.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Ernsperger L, Wendel D, Willey L: Girls under the Umbrella of Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2007, Shawnee Mission, Kansas: Autism Asperger Publishing Company Ernsperger L, Wendel D, Willey L: Girls under the Umbrella of Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2007, Shawnee Mission, Kansas: Autism Asperger Publishing Company
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsen PR, Kronenberg HM, Melmed S, Polonsky KS: Williams Textbook of Endocrinology. 2002, Philadelphia: Saunders, 10 Larsen PR, Kronenberg HM, Melmed S, Polonsky KS: Williams Textbook of Endocrinology. 2002, Philadelphia: Saunders, 10
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P: Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12 month old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2002, 25: 327-335. 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00094-2.CrossRef Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P: Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12 month old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2002, 25: 327-335. 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00094-2.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P: Foetal testosterone and vocabulary size in 18- and 24-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2002, 24: 418-424.CrossRef Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P: Foetal testosterone and vocabulary size in 18- and 24-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2002, 24: 418-424.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K: Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46: 198-210. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00349.x.CrossRefPubMed Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K: Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46: 198-210. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00349.x.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test. Soc Neurosci. 2006, 1: 135-148. 10.1080/17470910600992239.CrossRefPubMed Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test. Soc Neurosci. 2006, 1: 135-148. 10.1080/17470910600992239.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. Eur J Endocrinol. 2006, 155: S123-S130. 10.1530/eje.1.02260.CrossRef Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. Eur J Endocrinol. 2006, 155: S123-S130. 10.1530/eje.1.02260.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Brit J Psychol. 2009, 100: 1-22. 10.1348/000712608X311731.CrossRefPubMed Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G: Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Brit J Psychol. 2009, 100: 1-22. 10.1348/000712608X311731.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S: Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2010, 1: 11-10.1186/2040-2392-1-11.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S: Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2010, 1: 11-10.1186/2040-2392-1-11.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Smail PJ, Reyes FI, Winter JSD, Faiman C: The fetal hormonal environment and its effect on the morphogenesis of the genital system. Pediatric Andrology. Edited by: Kogan SJ, Hafez ESE. 1981, Boston; Martinus Nijhoff, 9-19.CrossRef Smail PJ, Reyes FI, Winter JSD, Faiman C: The fetal hormonal environment and its effect on the morphogenesis of the genital system. Pediatric Andrology. Edited by: Kogan SJ, Hafez ESE. 1981, Boston; Martinus Nijhoff, 9-19.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Main KM, Schmidt IM, Skakkebaek NE: A possible role for reproductive hormones in newborn boys: progressive hypogonadism without the postnatal testosterone peak. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2000, 85: 4905-4907. 10.1210/jc.85.12.4905.CrossRefPubMed Main KM, Schmidt IM, Skakkebaek NE: A possible role for reproductive hormones in newborn boys: progressive hypogonadism without the postnatal testosterone peak. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2000, 85: 4905-4907. 10.1210/jc.85.12.4905.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Alexander GM, Wilcox T, Farmer ME: Hormone-behavior associations in early infancy. Horm Behav. 2009, 56: 498-502. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.08.003.CrossRefPubMed Alexander GM, Wilcox T, Farmer ME: Hormone-behavior associations in early infancy. Horm Behav. 2009, 56: 498-502. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.08.003.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Collaer ML, Hines M: Human behavioural sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones during early development?. Psychol Bull. 1995, 118: 55-107.CrossRefPubMed Collaer ML, Hines M: Human behavioural sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones during early development?. Psychol Bull. 1995, 118: 55-107.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Charman T, Richler J, Pasco G, Brayne C: The Q-CHAT (Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers): a normally distributed quantitative measure of autistic traits at 18–24 months of age: preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008, 38: 1414-1425. 10.1007/s10803-007-0509-7.CrossRefPubMed Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Charman T, Richler J, Pasco G, Brayne C: The Q-CHAT (Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers): a normally distributed quantitative measure of autistic traits at 18–24 months of age: preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008, 38: 1414-1425. 10.1007/s10803-007-0509-7.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman DG: Practical Statistics for Medical Research. 1991, London: Chapman and Hall Altman DG: Practical Statistics for Medical Research. 1991, London: Chapman and Hall
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Lutchmaya S, Knickmeyer R: Prenatal Testosterone in Mind. 2004, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press Baron-Cohen S, Lutchmaya S, Knickmeyer R: Prenatal Testosterone in Mind. 2004, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press
Metadaten
Titel
Prenatal versus postnatal sex steroid hormone effects on autistic traits in children at 18 to 24 months of age
verfasst von
Bonnie Auyeung
Jag Ahluwalia
Lynn Thomson
Kevin Taylor
Gerald Hackett
Kieran J O’Donnell
Simon Baron-Cohen
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Molecular Autism / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 2040-2392
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-3-17

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

Molecular Autism 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe