Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research Article

Impact of physical and mental health on life satisfaction in old age: a population based observational study

verfasst von: Thomas Puvill, Jolanda Lindenberg, Antonius J. M. de Craen, Joris P. J. Slaets, Rudi G. J. Westendorp

Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

It is widely assumed that poor health lowers life satisfaction when ageing. Yet, research suggests this relationship is not straightforward. This study investigated how older people evaluate their life when facing disease and disabilities.

Methods

The Leiden 85-plus Study, a prospectively followed cohort of a cohort of a middle-sized city in the Netherlands, all aged 85 years, that was age-representative of the general population, was used. Those with severe cognitive dysfunction were excluded (n = 501). Comorbidities, physical performance, cognitive function, functional status, residual lifespan, depressive symptoms and experienced loneliness were measured during home visits. Life satisfaction was self-reported with Cantril’s ladder. All analyses were performed using regression analysis.

Results

Participants reported high life satisfaction (median 8 out of 10 points) despite having representative levels of disease and disability. Comorbidity, low cognitive function, and residual lifespan as markers of health were not associated with life satisfaction. Poor physical performance and low functional status were weakly but significantly associated with lower life satisfaction (p < 0.05 respectively p < 0.001), but significance was lost after adjustment for depressive symptoms and perceived loneliness. Depressive symptoms and perceived loneliness were strongly related to lower life satisfaction (both p < 0.001), even after adjustment for physical health characteristics.

Conclusion

Poor physical health was hardly related to lower life satisfaction, whereas poor mental health was strongly related to lower life satisfaction. This indicates that mental health has a greater impact on life satisfaction at old age than physical health, and that physical health is less relevant for a satisfactory old age.

Background

Advanced age is no longer for the lucky few. It is expected that by 2050, one out of four people will be 60 years or older, with the age group of over 80 experiencing the most rapid relative increase [1]. Unfortunately, the years that we have added to our lifespan are not free of disease and disability [2, 3]. The prospect of decline in bodily functions often disconcerts younger and older individuals: it is widely assumed, and some evidence suggests [4], that a good physical health is imperative for life satisfaction in old age.
Discordant with these assumptions, life evaluations have been found to be largely stable across the lifespan and not parallel to trajectories of physical decline [5, 6]. This stability of life satisfaction far into old age has been coined the ageing paradox, and can be observed until around age 70 when some studies find that life satisfaction starts to decline [7, 8]. Even at this age, however, the life satisfaction of many individuals remains high in spite of physical decline [6, 9]. This raises the question for whom life satisfaction does and does not decrease. It has even led some authors to argue that life satisfaction and physical decline are not directly related at all [10]. The answer is of importance not only for older people but also to healthcare professionals and policy makers, who aim to optimise both physical functioning and life satisfaction.
The objective of this study was to uncover the impact of physical and mental health on life satisfaction at advanced age. To this end, we analysed data from the Leiden 85-plus study, a representative population-based cohort of 85-year-old citizens.

Methods

Design and study sample

For the current paper, we used data from the first wave of the Leiden 85-plus Study. All inhabitants of the Dutch municipality of Leiden who turned 85 between 1 September 1997 and 1 September 1999 (n = 705) were identified from the municipal’s register. No exclusion criteria were employed at inclusion, but for the current analysis, participants were excluded if they had severe cognitive dysfunction, operationalized as MMSE score < 19 points, as self-reported questionnaires on mental health characteristics were not administered to these subjects [11]. Out of the eligible population, 14 died before enrolment, and 92 declined participation. The remaining 599 participants (87%) agreed to participate [11]. 98 Participants had an MMSE score below 19 points, setting the final study sample at 501.

Measures

Participants received home visits of a trained researcher three or four times in their 85th year during which standardised interviews and physiological measurements were conducted [11, 12]. All measurements were performed only once per year. Included measures are hereafter divided in demographics, physical health, mental health characteristics, and life satisfaction.
Data on demographics, sex and marital status was collected from the municipality. Participants provided information on their income (state pension or state pension and additional income), education (primary school or primary school and additional education) and living situation (independent or institutionalized) during one of the interviews.
Physical health was determined as presence of chronic diseases, physical performance, functional status, cognitive function, and residual lifespan. Number of diseases were counted as described previously [12]. Level of physical performance was based on outcomes for upper body physical performance by handgrip strength and lower body physical performance by gait speed [12]. For estimating handgrip strength, participants were asked to grip a dynamometer as hard as they could three times, and their best attempt was used as an indication of their handgrip strength. For gait speed, participants were asked to walk a short distance, using walking aids if necessary. Gait speed was based on the first three meters of the task. Scores for both these measures were ranked in quintiles for males and females separately to account for men’s generally higher level of performance. Quintile scores for upper and lower body physical performance were added up for the final score. Functional status was based on activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) as self-reported on the Groningen Activity Restriction Scale [13]. ADL and IADL scores were combined into a total score ranging from 18 to 72, so that lower numbers indicate more dysfunction. To facilitate interpretation, the aggregated score was coded in reverse for analysis. Cognitive function was measured with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [14], a screening instrument for cognitive dysfunction. Scores range from 0 to 30, higher scores indicating better cognitive function. Information on residual lifespan – the number of years the participant lived after age 85 - was obtained from the municipal registry. For analysis, the total disease count was categorised as ‘comorbidity’ (multiple chronic diseases) or less than two diseases. For figures, categories 0, 1, 2 and 3+ diseases were used. Due to lack of a (consensual) cut-off score in the literature, all other physical health variables were categorised above or below the median for analysis and in quartiles for figures, to allow effect sizes to be compared (see Table 1 for the dichotomous cut-off scores).
Table 1
Characteristics of the participants at age 85
Characteristics
N = 501
Demographics
 Female / male
317 (63.3%) / 184 (36.7%)
 Married / not married
176 (35.1%) / 325 (64.9%)
 State pension / state pension and extra income
71 (14.1%) / 430 (85.9%)
 Only primary school / further education
304 (60.7%) / 197 (39.3%)
 Institutionalised / living independently
55 (11.0%) / 446 (89.0%)
Physical health
 None or one disease / more than one disease
376 (75.0%) / 125 (25.0%)
 Grip strength in kilograms (median, IQR)
22 (18–29)
 Gait speed in seconds (median, IQR)
4.0 (3.1–5.8)
 Cognitive function score ≤ 26 / ≥ 27a
232 (46.3%) / 269 (53.7%)
 Functional status score ≤ 26 / ≥ 27a
238 (47.5%) / 263 (52.5%)
 Residual lifespan ≤ 5 / ≥ 6 yearsa
256 (51.1%) / 245 (48.9%)
Mental health
 Depressive symptoms score ≤ 4 / ≥ 5
78 (15.6%) / 423 (84.4%)
 Perceived loneliness score ≤ 2 / ≥ 3
128 (25.5%) / 373 (74.5%)
Life satisfaction
 Life satisfaction score (median, IQR)
8 (7–9)
 Life satisfaction score ≤ 5
59 (11.8%)
 Life satisfaction score 6
58 (11.6%)
 Life satisfaction score 7
105 (21%)
 Life satisfaction 8
143 (28.5%)
 Life satisfaction score 9
48 (9.6%)
 Life satisfaction score 10
88 (17.6%)
Data are number of patients (%) or median (IQR) For detailed descriptions of the variables, see Methods
aThe cut-off was based on the median
For mental health characteristics, depressive symptoms were measured with the Geriatric Depression Scale 15 (GDS-15) [12]: a 15-item screening instrument for depression developed for older populations. For analysis, the outcome score that can range from 1 to 15 was divided into score ≤ 4 for less depressive symptoms and ≥ 5 for more depressive symptoms [15] and for figures, the outcomes were divided into scores ≤ 4, 5–9, and ≥ 10 [16]. Perceived loneliness was measured with the De Jong-Gierveld Loneliness scale [12]: an 11-item questionnaire designed to measure loneliness with scoring range 0 to 11. For analysis, the total scores were categorized into ≤ 2 for those who did not experience loneliness and ≥ 3 for those who did, and for figures scores were categorized into ≤ 2, 3–9, and ≥ 10 [17].
Life satisfaction was measured with Cantril’s 10-point self-anchoring ladder [18]. Participants were presented with a depiction of a ladder, the steps numbered one to ten. Participants were told that the bottom step is the worst and the top step is the best imaginable life for them and were asked to indicate where on the ladder they felt they stood at that time, resulting in a life satisfaction score ranging from 1 to 10.

Data analysis

Main analyses

To identify determinants of life satisfaction, three sets of multiple linear regression analyses were performed with SPSS Statistics for Windows (IBM, Armonk, NY). The first of the three sets consisted of regression analyses for each physical and mental health characteristic separately, correcting for demographic characteristics only. The second set consisted of regression analyses for each physical health characteristic, correcting for demographic and all mental health characteristics. The third set consisted of regression analyses for each mental health characteristic, correcting for demographic and all physical health characteristics.

Categorisation

In all analyses, predictor variables were added simultaneously to the model as binary variables. Dichotomisation was done as described under ‘Measures’. Figures were supplemented with trend analyses using uncategorised, full-range variables.

Effect sizes

Coefficients of determination (percentage of explained variance) were calculated in order to estimate the effect size of the association between life satisfaction and characteristics of physical and mental health. For this, the full-range variabl/es were used.

Heteroscedasticity

To ensure that our results were not influenced by heteroscedasticity, we repeated the analyses using heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors (HCSE) [19]. The results were not substantially different; therefore we here provide the outcomes of the regular analyses.

Missing values

In total 1.22% of all data was missing. Missing values were imputed with multiple imputation using all full-range variables in this study as predictors. A sensitivity analysis was performed excluding the imputed data. This gave a similar pattern of results.
The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the Leiden University Medical Center. Permission to participate was provided in writing by participants who were able to do so. For cognitively impaired individuals, permission to participate was provided in writing by a guardian.

Results

Description of participants

The original sample of the Leiden 85-plus Study included 599 individuals and was representative of the entire source population that was intended for recruitment and the general Dutch population in terms of physical health, mortality hazard and demographics [11, 20]. For present analyses we excluded 98 participants with severe cognitive impairment, which was operationalized as an MMSE score below 19 points. Table 1 shows characteristics of the 501 participants included in the analyses. Life satisfaction was rated highly by most participants (median 8 out of 10 points (IQR 7 to 9)). Note the additional peak in the distribution at score 10 points (n = 88). Six participants rated their life satisfaction with 1 point and 21 participants rated it with less than 5 points. Ratings of life satisfaction were available for 43 of the 98 participants who were excluded due to severe cognitive impairment; the median score in this group was 8 out of 10 points (IQR 6 to 8).

Determinants of life satisfaction

There were no differences in life satisfaction between the sexes (0.1 points, 95% CI −0.4 to 0.3), nor between types of marital status (0.0 points, 95% CI −0.4 to 0.4), nor between those who had a state pension and those with additional income (0.0 points, 95% CI −0.4 to 0.5), nor between those who went to primary school and those with additional education (0.1 points, 95% CI −0.2 to 0.4). Life satisfaction among those who were institutionalized was 1.0 point lower (p < 0.001, 95% CI −1.5 to −0.5) than among those who were not institutionalized. This difference became smaller and lost statistical significance after adjustment for mental health characteristics (−0.5 points, 95% CI −1.0 to 0.1).
Table 2 shows the results of the regression analyses after dichotomising the various characteristics of health to enable the comparison of the various effect sizes. When adjusting for demographic characteristics only, the only physical health characteristics associated with lower life satisfaction were poorer physical performance and functional status (p < 0.05 and p < 0.001 respectively, both half a point). The negative association with physical performance was lost when further adjusting for mental health characteristics (0.2 points, 95% CI −0.1 to 0.5). The association with functional status weakened when further adjusting for mental health characteristics (p < 0.05, −0.3 points, 95% CI −0.6 to −0.1). In contrast to physical health characteristics, markers of poor mental health were strongly associated with lower life satisfaction (p < 0.001, one to two points) with and without adjustment for unequal distributions in physical health (Table 2).
Table 2
Self-perceived life satisfaction at age 85 by physical and mental health characteristics
 
Difference in life satisfaction score between groups (points; 95% CI)
Characteristics
Crude
Adjusted for mental health
Adjusted for physical health
Physical health
 Comorbidities vs less than two diseases
−0.1 (−0.5–0.2)
0.1 (−0.2 −0.4)
 Below vs above median physical performance
−0.5 (−0.8–−0.2)*
0.2 (−1–0.5)
 Below vs above median cognitive function
−0.3 (−0.6–0.0)
−0.1 (−0.4–0.2)
 Below vs above median functional status
−0.5 (−0.9–−0.3)***
−0.3 (−0.6–0.1)*
 Below vs above median residual lifespan
−0.2 (−0.5–0.2)
−0.0 (−0.3–0.3)
 
Mental health
 Below vs above cut-off depressive symptoms
−2.1 (−2.5–−1.7)***
−2.0 (−2.4–−1.6)***
 Below vs above cut-off perceived loneliness
−1.2 (−1.5–−0.8)***
−1.1 (−1.5–−0.8)***
All estimates from regression adjusted for sex, marital status, income, education and institutionalisation
Each of the estimates comes from a separate analysis
*p < ·05, ***p < ·001
To provide further information, we performed various trend analyses (Figs. 1, 2 and 3). Figure 1 shows mean life satisfaction estimated by various characteristics of physical health: comorbidity, physical performance score, cognitive function, and functional status. Trend analysis showed that poorer physical performance had a significant but small association with lower life satisfaction in the crude model (Fig. 1b), but lost significance after adjustment for characteristics of mental health. Poorer functional status showed a significant but small association with lower life satisfaction in both the crude and the adjusted model corrected for unequal distributions in mental health characteristics. Other physical health characteristics were not related to life satisfaction.
Figure 2 shows the mean life satisfaction dependent on characteristics of mental health: depressive symptoms and perceived loneliness. Trend analysis showed that having more depressive symptoms and perceiving greater loneliness were strongly and significantly associated with lower life satisfaction (both p < 0.001). Further adjustment for unequal distributions of physical health did not substantially change these results.
All participants were followed up for vital status. Figure 3 shows ratings of life satisfaction at age 85 years dependent on their residual lifespan. No significant relation was observed, neither for those who died within one year after assessment, nor for those who died later in time. Participants who lived up to centenarian age, thus had a residual lifespan of 15 years and over, had reported a particularly high life satisfaction at age 85. More specifically, out of this small subgroup of sixteen participants, only two participants reported life satisfaction below the median of 8 points.
Finally, we examined effect sizes of the associations between physical and mental health characteristics and life satisfaction with squared Pearson correlation coefficients using the crude, uncategorised, full range variables. For physical health, the explained variance ranged from 0.1% for comorbidity to 3.6% for functional status. The explained variance of life satisfaction for all physical health variables together was 2.8%. For mental health, the explained variance was 25.9% for depressive symptoms and 13.0% for perceived loneliness, and 28.8% combined. When adjusting for demographic and mental health characteristics, the partially explained variance ranged from 0.0 to 2.0% for the various physical health characteristics. The partially explained variance was 23.1 and 11.1% for depressive symptoms and perceived loneliness respectively when adjusting for demographic and physical health characteristics.

Discussion

We found that among older people age 85, the association between poor physical health and lower life satisfaction was modest, and disappeared when adjusted to mental health, functional status being the exception. Conversely, poor mental health was strongly related to lower life satisfaction, and this association did not change after adjusting for physical health. This suggests that physical health is hardly relevant for older people’s life satisfaction, whereas differences in mental health could distinguish between those with low and those with high life satisfaction.
Life satisfaction varies between high-income Western countries, but is largely unaffected by age within these countries [21]. This observation alone provides reasonable doubt whether age-related disease and disability have an important role in life satisfaction. Earlier publications using selected samples [9, 22] and younger samples [23] already found a limited association between physical health and life satisfaction. In current study, the population of which consisted of a representative sample of older people, this relationship appeared to be even smaller. This is striking, as data from the general population contain a broader range of health states than those from a clinical sample, and greater variety in determinants typically provides larger associations. A possible explanation for may be that participants in clinical samples consider their health more important or feel more burdened by their symptoms than older people of the general population. It is also possible that the larger effects found in other studies were due to confounding by age, which cannot have occurred in the current age-restricted sample. Another relevant contrast to the current study’s findings, is that conversely life satisfaction is a strong predictor of mortality [24], even in older populations [25]. It is possible that life satisfaction leading to worse long-term health outcomes but not vice versa, although sorting this out would require experimental designs, or at least use of longitudinal data and more complex models.
Our analysis showed that participants who died within the first year of follow-up had not reported a lower than average life satisfaction at age 85. This is not in line with earlier findings of a steep drop in life satisfaction in the last year of life that has been suggested as an explanation why life satisfaction decreases in very advanced age [26]. In contrast, we did not find an association between life satisfaction and residual lifespan. This further argues against the idea that high life satisfaction is a reflection of good physical health as indicated by longevity. We did, however, observe that the very few who eventually became centenarians had on average a higher rating of life satisfaction at age 85. If this was not due to physical health, it is tempting to speculate why their wellbeing was higher.
The weak relationship between physical health characteristics and life satisfaction found in the current study provides an important elaboration to a recent proposition of Steptoe, Deaton & Stone [27] that ‘[s]ubjective wellbeing and health are closely related, and the link could become increasingly important at older ages, if only because the prevalence of chronic illness increases with advancing age.’ Our study suggests that while this may hold true for the young old, it may not be true for the oldest old. The low explained variance indicates that health may be less relevant for the latter group. They may hold a more accepting attitude towards disease and disability. This may especially relevant for life satisfaction, as, contrary to hedonic and eudemonic well-being, life satisfaction is thought to consist of the relationship between one’s current experience and one’s future expectations [28, 29]. Expectations may change at old age as poor physical health becomes the norm and there is little expectation of a definite cure [30].
Considering these findings, we suggest that the large role of depressive symptoms and loneliness in life satisfaction for older individuals and the small significance of physical health can be explained by a theoretical model based on the concept vitality [31]. Combining the strengths of oft-cited theories that explain high life satisfaction at old age [29, 32, 33], the model starts from the observation that life satisfaction at old age remains high in the face of disease and disability and that, as qualitative research suggests, maintaining a high life satisfaction is dependent on older people’s ability to adapt personal goals in such a way that they are realistic and meaningful to the individual [30]. We propose that vitality stands for the capabilities that people have at their disposal for setting and achieving appropriate goals and appreciating the results.

Limitations

The original study sample can be considered characteristic for the Dutch population at 85 years, as there were no exclusion criteria. However, in the present study, we had to exclude those with severe cognitive impairments as we did not have (reliable) estimates for, amongst others, mental health for these subjects. The data can therefore not be extrapolated to participants with severe cognitive impairment. We did, however, obtain ratings on life satisfaction for almost half of them; these ratings were in the same range as for those individuals who were included in the analysis. Furthermore, since we showed that residual lifespan was not associated with life satisfaction.
The presented association between mental health and life satisfaction could be inflated by shared personality factors, such as neuroticism or by environmental factors, such as stressful life events. However, the focus of the current paper was on the relationship between physical health and life satisfaction, and mental health was merely included to find an explanation for the differences in life satisfaction between older people, since we showed that these were not due to differences in physical health. Therefore, in this paper we did not delineate specific affective disorders, nor did we investigate the nature of their association with life satisfaction (as long as it was not due to confounding by physical health). An inflated association between life satisfaction and mental health due to self-reporting could be suggested, but this is rather unlikely, because functional status was also self-reported but failed to show an association with life satisfaction in the same magnitude as mental health.
A weakness of the current study is that mental health characteristics were measured less extensively than physical health characteristics. However, this makes it all the more striking that the level of life satisfaction was so strongly explained by the few mental health measures that were available.
The objective of this study was to uncover the impact of physical and mental health on life satisfaction at advanced age. We found that in general, older people in the Netherlands were highly satisfied with their life, even in the presence of disease and disability. Poor physical health, including cognitive impairment, was not related to lower life satisfaction, and functional status was only weakly related to lower life satisfaction, especially when depressive symptoms and perceived loneliness were taken into account. In contrast, poor mental health was strongly related to lower life satisfaction, and characteristics of physical health did not alter these relationships.
The findings of the present study relate to international criticism and discussion on the use of Gross Domestic Product as a measure of a society’s ‘success’ and ‘well-being’ [34]. As is now widely argued, the relationship between Gross Domestic Product and well-being is not straightforward, and using these constructs interchangeably can lead to a narrow and biased view on well-being and its determinants. Our somewhat unexpected finding that physical health is hardly related to life satisfaction in the oldest old indicates that a similar warning is in order here.
The present data compel refraining altogether from measures that are composed of external determinants of wellbeing. Instead, well-being should be measured by asking people directly [28]. Improving well-being is a priority in public health [35]. and healthcare professionals and policymakers now increasingly make use of self-reported life satisfaction and its correlates to decide on the development and implementation of clinical interventions for individuals or on policies to improve well-being of the population [34]. In line with this, we believe that the present study contributes an important message: it suggests that more success in optimising life satisfaction for older people will come from interventions that prevent depressive symptoms and loneliness than from those that target physical health.

Conclusion

Among people aged 85, poor physical health was only modestly associated with lower life satisfaction. This association lost significance when adjusted for mental health for all physical health indicators but functional status. At the same time, we found a strong association between poor mental health and lower life satisfaction, and this association did not lose significance by adjusting for physical health. We conclude that at advanced age, physical health is hardly relevant for older people’s life satisfaction. Mental health, however, separates those with low from those with high life satisfaction. Other studies have showed before that mental health has a stronger association with life satisfaction than physical health, but the current study is the first to show that except for functional status, physical health has no unique association with life satisfaction. This is important as it indicates that physical decline may not be a relevant driver of lower life satisfaction at old age, especially when mental health is good.

Funding

The Leiden 85-plus Study was partly funded by a grant from the Dutch Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports (1997–2001), but the sponsor had no role in any stage of the Leiden 85-plus Study nor in the writing of the current manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Public data deposition is not permitted by the Leiden University Medical Center while legal and ethical aspects are being investigated and policy developed.

Authors’ contributions

TP: data analysis and manuscript preparation. JL: data analysis and manuscript preparation, AC: managing the original study, manuscript preparation. JS: manuscript preparation. RW: managing the original study, manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

AC passed away January 17th 2016, Leiden University Medical Center is his former affiliation.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Not applicable.
This study was ethically approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the Leiden University Medical Center. Permission to participate was provided in writing by participants who were able to do so. For cognitively impaired individuals, permission to participate was provided in writing by a guardian.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Population Division of the Department of Economic and Social Affairs of the United Nations Secretariat. World population prospects: the 2008 revision. New York: United Nations; 2009. Population Division of the Department of Economic and Social Affairs of the United Nations Secretariat. World population prospects: the 2008 revision. New York: United Nations; 2009.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. Health at a glance 2011: OECD indicators. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2011. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. Health at a glance 2011: OECD indicators. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2011.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Bodegom D, Bonneux L, Engelaer F, Lindenberg J, Meij J, Westendorp R. Dutch life expectancy from an international perspective. Leiden: Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing; 2010. Van Bodegom D, Bonneux L, Engelaer F, Lindenberg J, Meij J, Westendorp R. Dutch life expectancy from an international perspective. Leiden: Leyden Academy on Vitality and Ageing; 2010.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Wikman A, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Quality of life and affective well-being in middle-aged and older people with chronic medical illnesses: a cross-sectional population based study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18952.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wikman A, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Quality of life and affective well-being in middle-aged and older people with chronic medical illnesses: a cross-sectional population based study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18952.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans J, Macrory I, Randall C. Measuring national well-being: Life in the UK, 2015. Newport: Office for National Statistics; 2015. Evans J, Macrory I, Randall C. Measuring national well-being: Life in the UK, 2015. Newport: Office for National Statistics; 2015.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Nybo H, Gaist D, Jeune B, McGue M, Vaupel JW, Christensen K. Functional status and self rated health in 2,262 nonagenarians: the Danish 1905 cohort survey. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:601–9.CrossRefPubMed Nybo H, Gaist D, Jeune B, McGue M, Vaupel JW, Christensen K. Functional status and self rated health in 2,262 nonagenarians: the Danish 1905 cohort survey. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:601–9.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Baird BM, Lucas RE, Donnellan MB. Life satisfaction across the lifespan: findings from two nationally representative panel studies. Soc Indic Res. 2010;99:183–203.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Baird BM, Lucas RE, Donnellan MB. Life satisfaction across the lifespan: findings from two nationally representative panel studies. Soc Indic Res. 2010;99:183–203.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerstorf D, Ram N, Mayraz G, Hidajat M, Lindenberger U, Wagner GG, Schupp J. Late-life decline in well-being across adulthood in Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States: something is seriously wrong at the end of life. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:477–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gerstorf D, Ram N, Mayraz G, Hidajat M, Lindenberger U, Wagner GG, Schupp J. Late-life decline in well-being across adulthood in Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States: something is seriously wrong at the end of life. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:477–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Covinsky KE, Wu AW, Landefeld CS, Connors Jr AF, Phillips RS, Tsevat J, Dawson NV, Lynn J, Fortinsky RH. Health status versus quality of life in older patients: does the distinction matter? Am J Med. 1999;106:435–40.CrossRefPubMed Covinsky KE, Wu AW, Landefeld CS, Connors Jr AF, Phillips RS, Tsevat J, Dawson NV, Lynn J, Fortinsky RH. Health status versus quality of life in older patients: does the distinction matter? Am J Med. 1999;106:435–40.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Albrecht GL, Devlieger PJ. The disability paradox: high quality of life against all odds. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:977–88.CrossRefPubMed Albrecht GL, Devlieger PJ. The disability paradox: high quality of life against all odds. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:977–88.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Bootsma-Van der Wiel A, Van Exel E, De Craen A, Gussekloo J, Lagaay A, Knook D, Westendorp R. A high response is not essential to prevent selection bias: results from the Leiden 85-plus study. J Clin Epidemiol. 2002;55:1119–25.CrossRefPubMed Bootsma-Van der Wiel A, Van Exel E, De Craen A, Gussekloo J, Lagaay A, Knook D, Westendorp R. A high response is not essential to prevent selection bias: results from the Leiden 85-plus study. J Clin Epidemiol. 2002;55:1119–25.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Bootsma-Van der Wiel A, Gussekloo J, De Craen AJ, Van Exel E, Bloem BR, Westendorp RG. Common chronic diseases and general impairments as determinants of walking disability in the oldest old population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:1405–10.CrossRefPubMed Bootsma-Van der Wiel A, Gussekloo J, De Craen AJ, Van Exel E, Bloem BR, Westendorp RG. Common chronic diseases and general impairments as determinants of walking disability in the oldest old population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:1405–10.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Von Faber M, Bootsma–vander Wiel A, van Exel E, Gussekloo J, Lagaay AM, van Dongen E, Knook DL, van der Geest S, Westendorp RG. Successful aging in the oldest old: who can be characterized as successfully aged? Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2694–700.CrossRef Von Faber M, Bootsma–vander Wiel A, van Exel E, Gussekloo J, Lagaay AM, van Dongen E, Knook DL, van der Geest S, Westendorp RG. Successful aging in the oldest old: who can be characterized as successfully aged? Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2694–700.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Bootsma-van Der Wiel A, de Craen AJ, Van Exel E, Macfarlane PW, Gussekloo J, Westendorp RG. Association between chronic diseases and disability in elderly subjects with low and high income: the Leiden 85-plus study. Eur J Public Health. 2005;15:494–7.CrossRefPubMed Bootsma-van Der Wiel A, de Craen AJ, Van Exel E, Macfarlane PW, Gussekloo J, Westendorp RG. Association between chronic diseases and disability in elderly subjects with low and high income: the Leiden 85-plus study. Eur J Public Health. 2005;15:494–7.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Marc LG, Raue PJ, Bruce ML. Screening performance of the 15-item geriatric depression scale in a diverse elderly home care population. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:914–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marc LG, Raue PJ, Bruce ML. Screening performance of the 15-item geriatric depression scale in a diverse elderly home care population. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:914–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Alden D, Austin C, Sturgeon R. A correlation between the Geriatric Depression Scale long and short forms. J Gerontol. 1989;44:P124–5.CrossRefPubMed Alden D, Austin C, Sturgeon R. A correlation between the Geriatric Depression Scale long and short forms. J Gerontol. 1989;44:P124–5.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat van Tilburg TG, de Jong GJ. Reference standards for the loneliness scale. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 1999;30:158–63.PubMed van Tilburg TG, de Jong GJ. Reference standards for the loneliness scale. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 1999;30:158–63.PubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Cantril H. Pattern of human concerns. New Brunswich: Rutgers University Press; 1965. Cantril H. Pattern of human concerns. New Brunswich: Rutgers University Press; 1965.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes AF, Cai L. Using heteroskedasticity-consistent standard error estimators in OLS regression: an introduction and software implementation. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:709–22.CrossRefPubMed Hayes AF, Cai L. Using heteroskedasticity-consistent standard error estimators in OLS regression: an introduction and software implementation. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:709–22.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Statistisch Jaarboek. Voorburg: Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek; 1999. Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Statistisch Jaarboek. Voorburg: Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek; 1999.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Deaton A. Income, health and wellbeing around the world: evidence from the Gallup World Poll. J Econ Perspect J Am Econ Assoc. 2008;22:53–72.CrossRef Deaton A. Income, health and wellbeing around the world: evidence from the Gallup World Poll. J Econ Perspect J Am Econ Assoc. 2008;22:53–72.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Berg A, Hassing L, McClearn G, Johansson B. What matters for life satisfaction in the oldest-old? Aging Ment Health. 2006;10:257–64.CrossRefPubMed Berg A, Hassing L, McClearn G, Johansson B. What matters for life satisfaction in the oldest-old? Aging Ment Health. 2006;10:257–64.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Lacruz ME, Emeny RT, Baumert J, Ladwig KH. Prospective association between self-reported life satisfaction and mortality: results from the MONICA/KORA Augsburg S3 survey cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:579.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lacruz ME, Emeny RT, Baumert J, Ladwig KH. Prospective association between self-reported life satisfaction and mortality: results from the MONICA/KORA Augsburg S3 survey cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:579.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well ceing contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2011;3:1–43.CrossRef Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well ceing contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2011;3:1–43.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Kimm H, Sull JW, Gombojav B, Yi S-W, Ohrr H. Life satisfaction and mortality in elderly people: the Kangwha cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:54.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kimm H, Sull JW, Gombojav B, Yi S-W, Ohrr H. Life satisfaction and mortality in elderly people: the Kangwha cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:54.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Change in Life Satisfaction During Adulthood: Findings From the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88:189–202.CrossRefPubMed Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Change in Life Satisfaction During Adulthood: Findings From the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88:189–202.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2015;385:640–8.CrossRefPubMed Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2015;385:640–8.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. OECD Guidelines on Measuring Subjective Well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2013. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. OECD Guidelines on Measuring Subjective Well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2013.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat von Faber M. Maten van succes bij ouderen: gezondheid, aanpassing en sociaal welbevinden. Vianen: Optima; 2002. von Faber M. Maten van succes bij ouderen: gezondheid, aanpassing en sociaal welbevinden. Vianen: Optima; 2002.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Westendorp RGJ, Mulder B, van der Does AJW, van der Ouderaa FJG. When vitality meets longevity: new strategies for health in later life. In: Wellbeing in Later Life: A Complete Reference Guide. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2014. Westendorp RGJ, Mulder B, van der Does AJW, van der Ouderaa FJG. When vitality meets longevity: new strategies for health in later life. In: Wellbeing in Later Life: A Complete Reference Guide. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2014.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Heckhausen J. Developmental regulation across adulthood: primary and secondary control of age-related challenges. Dev Psychol. 1997. 1997;33:176–87. Heckhausen J. Developmental regulation across adulthood: primary and secondary control of age-related challenges. Dev Psychol. 1997. 1997;33:176–87.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990.CrossRef Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Donnell G, Deaton A, Durand M, Halpern D, Layard R. Wellbeing and policy. London: Legatum Institute; 2014. O’Donnell G, Deaton A, Durand M, Halpern D, Layard R. Wellbeing and policy. London: Legatum Institute; 2014.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Bellis MA, Lowey H, Hughes K, Deacon L, Stansfield J, Perkins C. Variations in risk and protective factors for life satisfaction and mental wellbeing with deprivation: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:492.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bellis MA, Lowey H, Hughes K, Deacon L, Stansfield J, Perkins C. Variations in risk and protective factors for life satisfaction and mental wellbeing with deprivation: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:492.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Impact of physical and mental health on life satisfaction in old age: a population based observational study
verfasst von
Thomas Puvill
Jolanda Lindenberg
Antonius J. M. de Craen
Joris P. J. Slaets
Rudi G. J. Westendorp
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Geriatrics / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2318
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-016-0365-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

BMC Geriatrics 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.