Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Trials 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Research

Impact of question order on prioritisation of outcomes in the development of a core outcome set: a randomised controlled trial

verfasst von: Sara T. Brookes, Katy A. Chalmers, Kerry N. L. Avery, Karen Coulman, Jane M. Blazeby, on behalf of the ROMIO study group

Erschienen in: Trials | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Core outcome set (COS) developers increasingly employ Delphi surveys to elicit stakeholders’ opinions of which outcomes to measure and report in trials of a particular condition or intervention. Research outside of Delphi surveys and COS development demonstrates that question order can affect response rates and lead to ‘context effects’, where prior questions determine an item’s meaning and influence responses. This study examined the impact of question order within a Delphi survey for a COS for oesophageal cancer surgery.

Methods

A randomised controlled trial was nested within the Delphi survey. Patients and health professionals were randomised to receive a survey including clinical and patient-reported outcomes (PROs), where the PRO section appeared first or last. Participants rated (1–9) the importance of 68 items for inclusion in a COS (ratings 7–9 considered ‘essential’). Analyses considered the impact of question order on: (1) survey response rates; (2) participants’ responses; and (3) items retained at end of the survey.

Results

In total, 116 patients and 71 professionals returned completed surveys. Question order did not affect response rates among patients, but fewer professionals responded when clinical items appeared first (difference = 31.3%, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 13.6–48.9%, P = 0.001). Question order led to different context effects within patients and professionals. While patients rated clinical items highly, irrespective of question order, more PROs were rated essential when appearing last rather than first (difference = 23.7%, 95% CI = 10.5–40.8%). Among professionals, the greatest impact was on clinical items; a higher percentage rated essential when appearing last (difference = 11.6%, 95% CI = 0.0–23.3%). An interaction between question order and the percentage of PRO/clinical items rated essential was observed for patients (P = 0.025) but not professionals (P = 0.357). Items retained for further consideration at the end of the survey were dependent on question order, with discordant items (retained by one question order group only) observed in patients (18/68 [26%]) and professionals (20/68 [29%]).

Conclusions

In the development of a COS, participants’ ratings of potential outcomes within a Delphi survey depend on the context (order) in which the outcomes are asked, consequently impacting on the final COS. Initial piloting is recommended with consideration of the randomisation of items in the survey to reduce potential bias.

Trial registration

The randomised controlled trial reported within this paper was nested within the development of a core outcome set to investigate processes in core outcome set development. Outcomes were not health-related and trial registration was not therefore applicable.

Background

Core outcome sets (COS) are recommended for use in clinical effectiveness trials to reduce heterogeneity of reported outcomes and aid data synthesis across similar trials, enhancing evidence-based medicine and reducing research waste [14]. A COS is an agreed minimum set of outcomes to be measured and reported in all trials of a particular condition or intervention [4]. Their development requires consensus methodology to establish outcomes considered most essential to patients and health professionals. One increasingly used approach is a Delphi survey [57], where participants are required to anonymously rate the importance of a long list of potential outcomes in sequential (postal or electronic) surveys or ‘rounds’ [8]. Feedback from each round is presented in the subsequent round such that participants can consider the opinions of others before re-rating items. The results of the Delphi inform any further consensus methods (such as a consensus meeting [911]) and the final COS. Guidelines exist for the Delphi process, within the context of a COS [4, 5, 12] and more widely elsewhere [1317], with emphases on selection of stakeholders, number of rounds, presentation of feedback and criteria for consensus. Far less focus has been awarded to the actual design of the Delphi survey itself, which has been criticised as often being poorly formulated [17, 18].
One issue that may be important within Delphi surveys is the ordering of questions and the potential for question order to impact on both overall survey response rate and individual responses to questions. Within Social and Health Sciences, there are numerous publications relating to the design of questionnaires or surveys and question order is frequently discussed [1921]. The choice of initial items may influence a respondent’s willingness or motivation to complete a survey since early items may shape a respondent’s understanding of what the survey is about [19]. Previous literature, including randomised studies, has demonstrated mixed effects in terms of overall survey response rate [2224]. In terms of actual responses to questions, when items are not asked in isolation it is likely (at least for some individuals) that responses to earlier questions will be used as a comparative standard by which to respond; consequently, the order of questions (or the ‘context’ in which questions are asked) may influence responses [21, 25]. This phenomenon is often referred to as a ‘context effect’ [19, 20, 25]. Indeed, such effects have been observed in numerous randomised and non-randomised studies [1921, 2529]. While focus has commonly been on the ordering of general and specific questions (with the recommendation that the general question should precede the specific, since the specific are more likely to influence the general than vice versa) [20, 26, 2831], effects have also been observed with the ordering of two or more similarly specific items [21, 25]. In order to explore question order effects, Moore [25] suggests a comparison of responses to two questions in the non-comparative context (when question asked first) and the comparative context (when question asked after another one). When responses to the two questions become more similar in the comparative than the non-comparative context we observe what is termed a consistency effect [21, 25], where respondents attempt to be consistent with their earlier responses. When responses become more different in the comparative context we observe a contrast effect [21, 25], respondents emphasising differences between items rather than the similarities.
In the context of Delphi surveys, we are only aware of one publication warning of such context effects [16]. Delphi surveys, constructed for COS development, generally include attitudinal questions, asking respondents to rate the importance of a succession of specific outcomes that may be valued differently. In such a setting it seems plausible that question order and context effects may lead to a significant bias [16], which is likely to influence the resulting COS.
This study explored the impact of question order within a Delphi survey used in the development of a COS for oesophageal cancer surgery. The following hypotheses were considered:
1.
The ordering of items impacts on Delphi survey response rates;
 
2.
The ordering of items effects participants’ responses (context effects); and the effect differs among patients and health professionals;
 
3.
The ordering of items influences the items retained at the end of the first Delphi round.
 

Methods

This methodological work employed a parallel randomised controlled trial, nested within a Delphi survey. The Delphi survey aimed to prioritise a ‘long list’ of outcomes to inform a COS (finalised at a subsequent consensus meeting) for clinical effectiveness trials of oesophageal cancer surgery. The development of the COS has been described in detail elsewhere [32]. An exhaustive ‘long list’ of outcomes was identified from a literature review, clinical audit and patient interviews [3336]. Overlapping outcomes were merged and categorised into health domains and included as individual items in the survey. Items consisted of 38 patient-reported outcomes (PROs) and 30 clinical outcomes. Patients and health professionals were asked to rate the importance of each item for inclusion in a COS, from 1 (not essential) to 9 (absolutely essential). Two versions of the survey were created. In version 1, PROs were presented first and the clinical outcomes last (termed ‘PRO first’), and in version 2, the clinical outcomes were presented first and the PROs last (‘PRO last’). For both versions, the items within the PRO and clinical sections were presented in identical order. PROs were grouped under a single heading of ‘quality of life after discharge from hospital’; clinical outcomes were grouped into headed sections of ‘benefits of oesophageal cancer surgery’, ‘in-hospital events’ and ‘events after hospital discharge’. In both versions items were written in lay terms with medical terms in brackets.
While the Delphi process consisted of two survey rounds (round 2 including feedback for each item retained from round 1), this study focused on the impact of question order on round 1 responses. This enabled the impact of question order to be explored in isolation, rather than being compounded by any effect of dropping items or presenting feedback from the previous round.

Stakeholder groups

Patients who had undergone oesophagectomy were identified from one of two UK hospital trusts (University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust and Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust). After establishing patients’ status, living patients were posted an invitation letter and information leaflet and asked to return a consent form indicating willingness to participate in the study. Consenting patients were then sent a postal survey with a pre-paid return envelope. Health professionals from relevant disciplines and clinical backgrounds (oesophagogastric surgeons and clinical nurse specialists) were identified from the membership of the Association of Upper Gastro Intestinal Surgeons of Great Britain and Ireland [32]. These professionals were notified by email about the study and sent a survey through the post with a pre-paid return envelope. Reminders were sent via post or email (for patients and professionals, respectively) to non-responders.

Randomisation

Participants were randomised, using a computer-generated schedule, to receive version 1 (PRO first) or version 2 (PRO last) in a 1:1 ratio. The schedule was generated separately for patients and professionals due to the different approaches to recruitment. For patients, who were recruited dynamically, block randomisation was stratified by centre. Once a consent form was received and logged on the database, the patient received the next allocation. All identified health professionals were randomised before they were notified of the study. In this instance, professionals were assigned an identification number and then simple randomisation was used to determine which received PRO first surveys and which received PRO last. This allocation schedule was used (within a mail-merge) to automatically generate the allocated survey for each participant.

Statistical analyses

Sample size

This nested study was opportunistic in nature, with the sample size determined by the numbers of patients and professionals participating in the Delphi process. Statistical hypothesis testing is, therefore, largely exploratory.
The following analyses were employed to address the study hypotheses. All statistical analyses were performed in Stata version 14 [37].
1. Impact on Delphi survey response rates
Response rates were calculated for each version of the survey (PRO first or PRO last) and for each stakeholder group (patients and health professionals), with the total number of surveys sent out to each sub-group (version and stakeholder group) as the denominator. The proportion responding was compared between randomisation groups, separately for patients and health professionals, with a Chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test as appropriate. The difference in proportions, 95% confidence interval (CI) and P value are reported. Demographic data were not available for non-responders hence we were unable to explore potential causal factors for non-response other than randomisation group.
2. Effect on participants’ responses (context effects) among patients and health professionals
The percentage of PROs rated essential (scored 7–9) [4] and the percentage of clinical items rated essential (7–9) was calculated for each participant. Distributional checks were carried out. Considering patients and professionals separately, two-by-two tables were generated presenting the mean (or median if data skewed) percentage of PRO and clinical items rated essential in the non-comparative context, when each was presented first in the survey, and in the comparative context, when each was presented last (and could therefore be rated in comparison to those items presented first). The difference between PROs and clinical items was calculated for both the non-comparative and comparative context (with 95% CI). The difference between appearing first and last in the survey was also calculated for both PROs and clinical items (with 95% CI). Visual examination of these tables provided insights into potential context effects [25]. While individual statistical tests could be carried out to ascertain if each of these four differences are ‘significantly’ different from zero, a more appropriate approach (and one which reduces the number of statistical tests) is to formally test for an interaction between the percentage of PROs/clinical items rated essential and question order. An appropriate analytical approach is afforded by the equivalence of the design of this study to that of a simple AB vs BA crossover trial [38].
In the analysis of a simple crossover trial, investigators often consider the potential of a treatment-period interaction, that is, where the effectiveness of treatment A compared to B is dependent on the order that treatments are received? Within the current study, a ‘treatment-period’ or PRO/clinical-position interaction is present if respondents rate PRO/clinical items differently if they come first or if they follow the other item type. To explore this, the average percentage of PROs and clinical items rated essential were calculated for each participant and the difference in means (or medians if data are skewed) compared between the randomisation groups [38]. In the absence of an interaction participants’ average percentage of items rated essential would be the same regardless of question order. The resulting distribution was examined by stakeholder and randomisation group. Unpaired t-tests or Mann–Whitney tests were then employed as appropriate (and 95% CI calculated), comparing randomisation groups. Analyses were carried out separately for patients and health professionals.
Analyses were repeated considering the median ratings given to PROs and clinical outcomes (rather than the percentage rated essential) and the consistency of results examined. In additional post-hoc analyses surgeons and clinical nurse specialists were considered separately.
3. Influence on the items retained at the end of the Delphi round 1
In the development of the COS, at the end of round 1 items were retained for round 2 if they were rated 7–9 by 70% or more of respondents and 1–3 by < 15% [32]. These criteria were considered separately for the two stakeholder groups and items retained if they met the criteria for patients and/or professionals. For the purposes of this paper, these criteria were additionally applied within each randomisation group separately. Two-by-two contingency tables categorised the number of items retained by: (1) both the PRO first and PRO last group; (2) the PRO first group only; (3) the PRO last group only; and (4) neither group. The percentage of discordant items, retained by one randomisation group but not the other, was calculated.

Results

The round 1 survey contained 68 items (38 PROs and 30 clinical items). In total, 200 patients were invited to participate in the Delphi survey, of whom 130 (65%) provided consent and were allocated to and sent either a PRO first or PRO last survey. Ninety-six health professionals were identified and randomised and the allocated survey sent for completion (Fig. 1).
1. Impact on Delphi survey response rates
A higher overall rate of questionnaire return, ignoring randomisation group, was observed within patients (89.2%) as compared to health professionals (74.0%). This is likely a consequence of the questionnaire only being sent to patients who had already provided consent. Response rates among patients were high; 59 (91%) and 57 (88%) within the PRO first and PRO last groups, respectively, demonstrating no difference between the question order randomisation groups (difference = 3.1%, 95% CI = −7.6–13.7%, P = 0.572). Among health professionals, however, a difference was observed between question order groups. Within the PRO first group, 40 (83%) surveys were completed compared to only 23 (52%) among the PRO last group (difference = 31.3%, 95% CI = 13.6–48.9%, P = 0.001). Due to study constraints, reminders were only sent to the professionals allocated to PRO last group given the very poor response rate. Even after a reminder, the response rate in this group remained significantly lower (31 [65%]) than in the PRO first group (difference = 18.8%, 95% CI = 1.6–35.9%, P = 0.036).

Baseline comparison of randomisation groups

Table 1 presents baseline characteristics of those responding to the round 1 survey. The question order groups are largely similar except for a lower percentage of male patients and a higher percentage of younger professionals responding to the PRO last survey.
Table 1
Baseline demographics of participants completing questionnaire
Stakeholder group
PRO first
PRO last
Patients
n = 59
n = 57
Male, n (%)
52 (88.1)
42 (73.7)
Age, mean (SD)a
66.7 (7.2)
66.3 (8.9)
Education, n (%)b
 None, GCSE
28 (50.0)
30 (54.5)
 A level, further education
17 (30.3)
17 (30.9)
 Otherc
11 (19.6)
8 (14.5)
Employment, n (%)
 Working full-time
8 (13.6)
10 (17.5)
 Retired
40 (67.8)
36 (63.2)
 Other
11 (18.6)
11 (19.3)
Years since surgery, median (IQR)d
1.3 (0.7–2.3)
1.4 (0.6–2.3)
Hospital stay < 2 weeks, n (%)e
35 (60.3)
37 (67.3)
Health professionals
n = 40
n = 31
Male, n (%)
29 (72.5)
23 (74.2)
Age, n (%) (years)
 ≤40
3 (7.5)
7 (22.6)
 41–50
18 (45.0)
14 (45.2)
 51–60
18 (45.0)
6 (19.4)
 >60
1 (2.5)
4 (12.9)
Job title, n (%)
 Consultant surgeon
30 (75.0)
22 (71.0)
 Surgical registrar
1 (2.5)
1 (3.2)
 Clinical specialist nurse
9 (22.5)
8 (25.8)
aAge missing for one ‘PRO first’ patient
bEducation missing for three ‘PRO first’ patients and two ‘PRO last’ patients
cMajority of ‘other’ are vocational qualifications with insufficient detail for classification
dYears since surgery missing for three ‘PRO first’ patients and three ‘PRO last’ patients
eHospital stay missing for one ‘PRO first’ patient and two ‘PRO last’ patients
2. Effect on participants’ responses (context effects) among patients and health professionals
The percentage of PROs rated essential (scored 7–9) and the percentage of clinical items rated essential was calculated for each participant. In order to explore potential context effects (such as consistency and contrast), PRO and clinical ratings were considered in both a non-comparative and comparative context and among patients and professionals separately. Distributions were heavily negatively skewed; since appropriate transformations would significantly hinder interpretation, median and IQRs are reported.

Patients

Table 2 summarises (as median and IQR) the percentage of PROs and clinical items rated essential by patients when presented first (non-comparative) and last (comparative) [25]. Patients rated clinical items very highly, irrespective of question order (96.7% rated essential in both the PRO first and PRO last groups). However, far more PROs were rated essential when they appeared after clinical items than when they appeared first (difference = 23.7%, 95% CI = 10.5–40.8%). When asked about PROs first, patients on average rated 66% of PROs as essential; when the other half of the patients were asked about clinical outcomes first, they rated on average 97% of clinical outcomes essential. Hence, in a non-comparative context, participants rate on average 31% more clinical items than PROs as essential. However, in the comparative context, the difference (in favour of clinical items) is reduced to just 7%. This demonstrates a consistency effect – the difference between PROs and clinical outcomes becomes smaller in the comparative context. This effect can perhaps more clearly be seen when considered graphically in Fig. 2.
Table 2
Patients: percentage of items rated essential within the non-comparative and comparative context (a consistency effect)
Context of rating
Percentage of items rated essential by a participant, median (IQR)
Difference in medians (clinical minus PROs) (95% CI)a
PROs (38 items)
Clinical (30 items)
Appearing first (non-comparative)
65.8 (47.4–89.5)
96.7 (73.3–100.0)
30.9 (11.8–39.2)
Appearing last (comparative)
89.5 (60.5–97.4)
96.7 (63.3–100.0)
7.2 (−1.4–13.2)
Difference in medians (last minus first) (95% CI)a
23.7 (10.5–40.8)
0.0 (0.0–20.0)
−23.7
Number of patients: PRO first n = 59; PRO last n = 57
aBias-corrected bootstrap 95% CI
The average percentage of PRO and clinical items rated essential by each participant was calculated (to test for a PRO/clinical-position interaction). The resulting distribution was heavily negatively skewed and parametric tests comparing the randomisation groups demonstrated that assumptions for such a test were violated. Non-parametric tests were therefore performed and evidence of a PRO/clinical-position interaction effect observed (Mann–Whitney P = 0.0250) (Table 4).

Health professionals

Health professionals rated a higher percentage of both PROs and clinical items as essential when they appeared last in the survey (Table 3), with the greatest impact seen for clinical items (11.6% more items rated essential when they appeared last, 95% CI = 0.0–23.3%). In the non-comparative context (PRO/clinical items presented first), professionals on average rated 57% PROs as essential compared to 67% clinical outcomes – a 10% difference. In the comparative context (PRO/clinical items presented last) the percentage of essential PROs increased marginally to 61% and clinical outcomes increased to 78%, resulting in a greater difference between the two types of items of 17%. In this instance, we have a contrast effect because the difference increases in the comparative context. Again, this contrast effect can be seen more clearly in Fig. 3. However, there was less evidence of a PRO/clinical-position interaction in this instance (Mann–Whitney P = 0.3567, Table 4), the observed effects likely to be due to chance.
Table 3
Health professionals: percentage of items rated essential within the non-comparative and comparative context (a contrast effect)
Context of rating
Percentage of items rated essential by a participant, median (IQR)
Difference in medians (clinical minus PROs) (95% CI)a
PROs (38 items)
Clinical (30 items)
Appearing first (non-comparative)
56.6 (42.1–85.5)
66.7 (60.0–83.3)
10.1 (−7.7–22.1)
Appearing last (comparative)
60.5 (26.3–89.5)
78.3 (66.7–86.7)
17.8 (−7.5–43.2)
Difference in medians (last minus first) (95% CI)a
3.9 (−23.7–31.6)
11.6 (0.0–23.3)
+7.7
Number of professionals: PRO first n = 40; PRO last n = 31
aBias-corrected bootstrap 95% CI
Table 4
Item type-position interaction effects
 
Averagea percentage of PROs and clinical items rated essential, median (IQR)
Difference in medians (95% CI)b
P valuec
PRO first
PRO last
Patients
80.9 (59.7 to 93.4)
92.1 (61.4 to 98.7)
11.2 (−2.5 to 19.7)
0.025
Health professionals
68.2 (58.3 to 81.6)
70.0 (42.3 to 82.0)
1.8 (−14.8 to 13.3)
0.357
aAverage calculated for each participant as ([% essential PROs] + [% essential clinical])/2
bBias-corrected bootstrap 95% CIs
cP values derived from Mann–Whitney tests
Given the opposite context effects observed, analysis ignoring stakeholder group has not been presented here. In brief, such analysis produced consistent results to those seen for patients, a likely consequence of the larger sample size in this participant group. Within this study, 76% of health professionals were surgeons and only 24% specialist nurses (Table 1). Post-hoc analyses considered potential context effects separately for nurses and surgeons. Unlike surgeons and patients, nurses rated PROs as more essential than clinical items. In the non-comparative context, 20% more PROs on average were rated essential than clinical items, compared to only 5% more in the comparative context – demonstrating a consistency effect within nurses (Additional file 1: Table S1). The number of participants was, however, small within this group. Additional file 2: Table S2 presents the results for surgeons only; unsurprisingly, patterns were similar to those for all professionals combined.
Analyses considering the median ratings given to PROs and clinical outcomes (rather than the percentage rated essential) demonstrated the same effects as those reported above.
3. Influence on the items retained at the end of the Delphi round 1
Applying pre-specified criteria for retaining items at the end of round 1, discordant items were observed where an item was retained by one question order group and not the other (Table 5). The degree of discrepancy was dependent on item type (PRO or clinical) and stakeholder group. The greatest discrepancy within patients was in terms of PROs (42% discordant items) and within professionals it was in terms of clinical items (37% discordant items). If items were retained when the pre-specified criteria were met by patients and/or professionals (criteria used for the overall development of the oesophageal surgery COS [32]), there remained 24% (16/68) discordant items between the question order groups (Table 5). Hence, question order impacts on the items retained for round 2.
Table 5
Number of items retained at end of round 1 by patients and health professionals
Stakeholder group
Participants (n)
Outcome type
Items retained at end of round 1a, n (%)
Discordant items (%)
PRO first
PRO last
Retained by both groups
Retained by PRO first only
Retained by PRO last only
Retained by neither group
Patients
59
57
PRO
14/38
0/38
16/38
8/38
42.1
  
Clinical
28/30
2/30
0/30
0/30
13.3
Health professionals
40
31
PRO
5/38
7/38
2/38
24/38
23.7
  
Clinical
12/30
10/30
1/30
7/30
36.7
Allb
99
88
PRO
17/38
1/38
13/38
7/38
36.8
  
Clinical
28/30
2/30
0/30
0/30
6.7
aItems were retained by each stakeholder group if they were scored 7–9 by 70% or more and 1–3 by < 15%
bFor ‘all’ participants, items were retained if scored 7–9 by 70% or more and 1–3 by < 15% within either stakeholder group
In the development of the oesophageal surgery COS (within which this methodological work is nested), the investigators combined the randomisation groups at the end of round 1 to determine which items to retain for all participants [32]. In round 2 (in which participants received a survey with questions in the same order as their round 1 survey), question order effects were again considered and similar patterns observed as in round 1.

Discussion

This methodological work examined the impact of question order within the first round of a Delphi survey to inform a COS for oesophageal cancer resection surgery. Question order did not impact on response rates within patients; however, fewer health professionals responded to the survey when clinical items appeared first and PRO items last. While participants consistently rated clinical items more essential than PROs (irrespective of question order or stakeholder group), context effects (where prior questions affect responses to later questions) were observed among both stakeholder groups, though the direction of these effects differed. Patients inflated the importance of PROs when rating them last in the survey, being more consistent with their earlier judgments regarding clinical items (consistency effect), whereas professionals inflated the importance of clinical items when they appeared last, emphasising their greater importance compared to PROs previously rated (contrast effect). Moreover, this study observed that question-order impacted on items retained at the end of round 1 (based on pre-specified criteria), which will ultimately influence the final COS and, therefore, is of utmost importance. Given these findings, we would strongly recommend that potential question order effects are considered when designing and implementing a Delphi survey for the development of a COS.
The results of this study agree with previous literature within survey research (including both non-randomised and randomised studies) and extend it to Delphi surveys and COS development. The majority of research into question order effects has dealt with behavioural or factual items that are verifiable. In Delphi surveys for COS development participants are asked attitudinal questions, being required to rate how important they feel different outcomes are relative to each other. In this situation, it is implicit that participants consider items in comparison to previous items; hence, context effects are perhaps more likely than in other settings [16, 39].
Items presented at the beginning of a survey may motivate or demotivate an individual to respond [19]. In this study, health professionals appear to have been less motivated to respond if clinical items appeared first. One may hypothesise that if PROs appear first, a professional might feel strongly compelled to express their opinion that these are not the most important items, whereas if clinical items (such as survival) appear first that same professional might feel less driven (or less need) to respond. Within this study, opposite context effects were seen within patients and professionals. This agrees with Birckart [40] who argues that consistency effects (what he terms ‘carryover’) are more likely when respondents feel they are moderately knowledgeable (such as patients), whereas contrast effects (‘backfire’) are more likely when respondents are highly knowledgeable (such as health professionals in the relevant field).
Recent research has demonstrated that different types of health professionals value different outcomes and that each group should be adequately represented [41, 42]. Within this study, 76% of health professionals were surgeons (consultant and registrar) and only 24% specialist nurses. Additional post-hoc analyses demonstrated that surgeons and nurses prioritised different outcomes. Moreover, question order resulted in different context effects within these two groups of health professionals. While the number of nurses in this analysis was small, given the observed differences we would support recent recommendations that different health professionals should be considered as separate panels during the Delphi process [42].
In the current study, some degree of imbalance was observed between the randomisation groups in terms of the gender of patients and the age of health professionals. This may be due to chance or it may (at least partially) be due to certain individuals being more or less likely to respond to the different versions of the survey (PRO first and PRO last). For example, women may be more likely (or men less likely) and younger professionals more likely (or older professionals less likely) to respond when clinical items are first (PRO last). Previous authors have suggested that the magnitude of order effects may depend on participant demographics [26]; however, few studies have provided empirical evidence. McFarland found no evidence of question order effect varying with sex or education [29], but a later study observed order effects among less-educated respondents only [30, 40]. We are not aware of any studies that have specifically considered age. Further exploration within this current study examined male and female patients and younger and older professionals separately (Additional files 3, 4, 5 and 6: Tables S3-S6). Patterns were largely consistent, with perhaps a greater consistency effect within women than men and a greater contrast effect within younger rather than older professionals; however, numbers of participants were small within individual groups.
Patients and health professionals were the only stakeholder groups included in this study and it is possible that different question order effects may occur in other groups such as methodologists or regulators. However, patients and health professionals are considered the most essential stakeholders to include in the development of a COS [5] and are likely to make up a large majority, if not all, of the Delphi participants. This study included participants only from the UK and within a single disease setting; it is important, therefore, to repeat this study in other countries and settings. In addition, not all Delphi surveys drop items (deemed less essential) at the end of each round, instead retaining all items until the end of the final round. However, in such a scenario it is highly likely that if context effects are present, due to the design of the survey, they will impact on responses in all rounds and the subsequent final COS.
This is the first study we are aware of investigating question order within a Delphi for COS development and, while exploratory in nature, it provides the best evidence at present, that such effects should be considered in this setting. Initial piloting of the Delphi survey may be valuable in identifying potential question order effects and we would recommend that this is always done. Cognitive interviews, such as ‘Think Aloud’ [43], carried out while individuals complete the survey with different orderings of items, may help identify if and how responses are influenced by earlier items. Previous survey research offers potential recommendations to reduce potential question-order effects. Question-order effects are assumed to arise because items similar in content influence one another [26]; this has led to the suggestion that such items could be separated with ‘buffer’ questions [27, 39, 44]. One potential within a Delphi survey for a COS, such as that described in this current paper, might be to alternate clinical and PRO items. However, this may interrupt the flow of the survey, making it less coherent [26], and guidelines suggest that items within the same theme should be grouped together [21]. Future research should explore this approach further.
An alternative approach for COS development is to randomise participants to receive surveys with different question orders and then combine the responses across the different surveys. Indeed, within the field of survey research this approach was recommended as long as 40 years ago [45] and has been reiterated since [16, 19, 20, 28]. The idea here is that when the data are combined across all randomised participants (as in the development of the oesophageal COS) question-order effects will be ‘cancelled out’ or at least diminished. This current paper has only considered the ordering of two ‘blocks’ of items (PRO and clinical), which produces only two different randomised versions. We have not considered potential order effects within those ‘blocks’ which may also exist. Again, initial piloting with cognitive interviews may help identify the extent of randomisation required. While it would be plausible to randomise items within ‘blocks’, it may be more logistically challenging, although this is likely to be easier for an electronic Delphi survey than a postal one. This should be explored further.
Within the context of crossover trials, when strong period-treatment interactions are observed, one recommendation is to use data from the first period only from each of the randomisation groups [38]. This has also been recommended within survey research, where question order has been randomised, in the belief that responses to questions asked in the non-comparative context are a better representation of an individual’s true feelings [29]. However, in the context of prioritising potential outcomes for a COS, it could be argued that an outcome cannot be rated without consideration of other outcomes and so the comparative context may be more appropriate.
While context effects were observed in this exploratory study, further work is needed to replicate and confirm our findings within the development of other core sets. It is, however, plausible that question order may, to some extent, have impacted on previously developed COSs which have employed Delphi surveys. A crucial part of the development of a COS is its subsequent periodic review in order to validate the COS and ensure outcomes are still important [4]. For COSs initially developed without consideration of question order, such a review would afford the opportunity to consider such potential effects. This research does not invalidate previously developed COSs but offers a potential enhancement to the review and updating of COSs and the development of future COSs.
In addition to initial piloting of the Delphi survey, in the absence of further research we would recommend that question order within a Delphi survey is randomised, at least in terms of the presentation of clinical and patient-reported outcomes, and that the responses are then combined across randomisation groups to inform the final COS.
Finally, while this study has considered the use of a Delphi survey to inform a COS, question order is also likely to have an impact in other forms of consensus methodology such as the Nominal Group Technique or less-structured consensus meetings. While these approaches do not generally incorporate a formal questionnaire, items for discussion are still presented to participants in some order. Without running multiple meetings, it is difficult to envisage how randomisation could be utilised in this scenario. The Delphi method enables randomisation of question order and impact of question order to be examined empirically afterwards.

Conclusions

Core outcome set (COS) developers are increasingly employing Delphi surveys to elicit stakeholders’ opinions as to the most essential outcomes to measure and report in trials of a particular condition or intervention. There is currently little guidance as to the optimal structure of such surveys. This paper demonstrates that participants’ ratings of potential outcomes within a Delphi survey depend on the order in which the outcomes are presented. Initial piloting of such surveys is recommended with consideration of the randomisation of items in the Delphi survey.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all the patients and health professionals who gave up their time to participate in the Delphi survey, including the CONSENSUS (Core Outcomes and iNformation SEts iN SUrgical Studies) Esophageal Cancer working group which comprises health professionals who participated in at least one round of the Delphi survey: Derek Alderson (University Hospitals Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Bilal Alkhaffaf (Central Manchester University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), William Allum (The Royal Marsden NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Stephen Attwood (Northumbria Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Hugh Barr (Gloucestershire Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Issy Batiwalla (North Bristol NHS Trust, UK), Guy Blackshaw (University Hospital of Wales, UK), Marilyn Bolter (Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Abrie Botha (Guy and St Thomas’ NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Jim Byrne (University Hospitals Southampton NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Joanne Callan (Heart of England NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Graeme Couper (NHS Lothian, UK), Khaled Dawas (University College London Hospitals, UK), Chris Deans (NHS Lothian, UK), Claire Goulding (Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Simon Galloway (South Manchester University Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Michelle George (Maidstone and Tunbridge Wells NHS Trust, UK), Jay Gokhale (Bradford Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Mike Goodman (The Royal Bournemouth and Christchurch Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Richard Hardwick (Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Ahmed Hassn (Princess of Wales Hospital, UK), Mark Henwood (Glangwili General Hospital, UK), David Hewin (Gloucestershire Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Simon Higgs (Gloucestershire Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Jamie Kelly (University Hospitals Southampton NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Richard Kryzstopik (Royal United Hospitals Bath NHS Trust, UK), Michael Lewis (Norfolk and Norwich University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Colin MacKay (NHS Greater Glasgow and Clyde, UK), James Manson (Singleton Hospital, UK), Robert Mason (Guy and St Thomas’ NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Ruth Moxon (Royal Berkshire NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Muntzer Mughal (University College London Hospitals, UK), Sue Osborne (Yeovil District Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Richard Page (Liverpool Heart and Chest Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Raj Parameswaran (Leeds Teaching Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Simon Parsons (Nottingham University Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Simon Paterson-Brown (NHS Lothian, UK), Anne Phillips (Oxford University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Shaun Preston (Royal Surrey County Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Kishore Pursnani (Lancashire Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), John Reynolds (St James’ Hospital, Dublin, Ireland), Bruno Sgromo (Oxford University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Mike Shackcloth (Liverpool Heart and Chest Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Jane Tallett (Norfolk and Norwich University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Dan Titcomb (University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Olga Tucker (Heart of England Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Tim Underwood (University of Southampton, UK), Jon Vickers (Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Mark Vipond (Gloucestershire Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Lyn Walker (University Hospitals of North Midlands NHS Trust, UK), Neil Welch (Nottingham University Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), John Whiting (University Hospitals Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Jo Price (Royal United Hospitals Bath NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Peter Sedman (Hull and East Yorkshire Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Thomas Walsh (Connolly Hospital, Dublin, Ireland), Jeremy Ward (Lancashire Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, UK).
The ROMIO study group comprises co-applicants on the ROMIO feasibility study (listed in alphabetical order): C Paul Barham (University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Richard Berrisford (Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Jenny Donovan (University of Bristol, UK), Jackie Elliott (Bristol Gastro-Oesophageal Support and Help Group, UK), Stephen Falk (University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Robert Goldin (Imperial College London, UK), George Hanna (Imperial College London, UK), Andrew Hollowood (University Hospitals Bristol NHS Foundation Trust, UK), Sian Noble (University of Bristol, UK), Grant Sanders (Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust, UK), Tim Wheatley (Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust, UK).

Funding

This work was supported by the Medical Research Council ConDUCT-II (Collaboration and innovation for Difficult and Complex randomised controlled Trials In Invasive procedures) Hub for Trials Methodology Research (MR/K025643/1) (http:// http://​www.​bristol.​ac.​uk/​social-community-medicine/​centres/​conduct2/​). JB is an NIHR Senior Investigator. The development of the oesophageal COS was funded within the ROMIO feasibility study by the National Institute for Health Research, Health Technology Assessment Programme (project number 10/50/65). The views and opinions expressed therein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the MRC, the NIHR, the NHS or the Department of Health (UK).

Availability of data and materials

The dataset analysed during the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Ethical approval for this study was granted by the South-West – Frenchay Research Ethics Committee (12/SW/0161). All patients were informed about the study and invited to participate. Only those returning a completed consent form were then posted a round 1 questionnaire. For professionals, a round 1 questionnaire was provided with initial study information and invitation to participate; in this instance the return of a completed questionnaire was deemed consent to participate, as agreed by the local ethics committee.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Williamson PR, Gamble C, Altman DG, Hutton JL. Outcome selection bias in meta-analysis. Stat Methods Med Res. 2005;14(5):515–24.CrossRefPubMed Williamson PR, Gamble C, Altman DG, Hutton JL. Outcome selection bias in meta-analysis. Stat Methods Med Res. 2005;14(5):515–24.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Kirkham JJ, Gargon E, Clarke M, Williamson PR. Can a core outcome set improve the quality of systematic reviews?–a survey of the Co-ordinating Editors of Cochrane Review Groups. Trials. 2013;14(1):21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kirkham JJ, Gargon E, Clarke M, Williamson PR. Can a core outcome set improve the quality of systematic reviews?–a survey of the Co-ordinating Editors of Cochrane Review Groups. Trials. 2013;14(1):21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Williamson PR, Altman DG, Blazeby JM, Clarke M, Devane D, Gargon E, et al. Developing core outcome sets for clinical trials: issues to consider. Trials. 2012;13(1):132.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Williamson PR, Altman DG, Blazeby JM, Clarke M, Devane D, Gargon E, et al. Developing core outcome sets for clinical trials: issues to consider. Trials. 2012;13(1):132.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Sinha IP, Smyth RL, Williamson PR. Using the Delphi technique to determine which outcomes to measure in clinical trials: recommendations for the future based on a systematic review of existing studies. PLoS Med. 2011;8(1):e1000393.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sinha IP, Smyth RL, Williamson PR. Using the Delphi technique to determine which outcomes to measure in clinical trials: recommendations for the future based on a systematic review of existing studies. PLoS Med. 2011;8(1):e1000393.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Gorst SL, Gargon E, Clarke M, Blazeby JM, Altman DG, Williamson PR. Choosing important health outcomes for comparative effectiveness research: an updated review and user survey. PLoS One. 2016;11(1):e0146444.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gorst SL, Gargon E, Clarke M, Blazeby JM, Altman DG, Williamson PR. Choosing important health outcomes for comparative effectiveness research: an updated review and user survey. PLoS One. 2016;11(1):e0146444.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Dalkey N, Helmer O. An experimental application of the Delphi method to the use of experts. Manag Sci. 1963;9(3):458–67.CrossRef Dalkey N, Helmer O. An experimental application of the Delphi method to the use of experts. Manag Sci. 1963;9(3):458–67.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat McNair AG, Whistance RN, Forsythe RO, Macefield R, Rees J, Pullyblank AM, et al. Core outcomes for colorectal cancer surgery: a consensus study. PLoS Med. 2016;13(8):e1002071.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral McNair AG, Whistance RN, Forsythe RO, Macefield R, Rees J, Pullyblank AM, et al. Core outcomes for colorectal cancer surgery: a consensus study. PLoS Med. 2016;13(8):e1002071.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulman KD, Hopkins J, Brookes ST, Chalmers K, Main B, Owen-Smith A, et al. A core outcome set for the benefits and adverse events of bariatric and metabolic surgery: the BARIACT project. PLoS Med. 2016;13(11):e1002187.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Coulman KD, Hopkins J, Brookes ST, Chalmers K, Main B, Owen-Smith A, et al. A core outcome set for the benefits and adverse events of bariatric and metabolic surgery: the BARIACT project. PLoS Med. 2016;13(11):e1002187.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Harman NL, Bruce IA, Kirkham JJ, Tierney S, Callery P, O’Brien K, et al. The importance of integration of stakeholder views in core outcome set development: otitis media with effusion in children with cleft palate. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129514.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Harman NL, Bruce IA, Kirkham JJ, Tierney S, Callery P, O’Brien K, et al. The importance of integration of stakeholder views in core outcome set development: otitis media with effusion in children with cleft palate. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129514.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Brookes ST, Macefield RC, Williamson PR, McNair AG, Potter S, Blencowe NS, et al. Three nested randomized controlled trials of peer-only or multiple stakeholder group feedback within Delphi surveys during core outcome and information set development. Trials. 2016;17(1):409.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Brookes ST, Macefield RC, Williamson PR, McNair AG, Potter S, Blencowe NS, et al. Three nested randomized controlled trials of peer-only or multiple stakeholder group feedback within Delphi surveys during core outcome and information set development. Trials. 2016;17(1):409.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Hasson F, Keeney S, McKenna H. Research guidelines for the Delphi survey technique. J Adv Nurs. 2000;32(4):1008–15.PubMed Hasson F, Keeney S, McKenna H. Research guidelines for the Delphi survey technique. J Adv Nurs. 2000;32(4):1008–15.PubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Keeney S, Hasson F, McKenna HP. A critical review of the Delphi technique as a research methodology for nursing. Int J Nurs Stud. 2001;38(2):195–200.CrossRefPubMed Keeney S, Hasson F, McKenna HP. A critical review of the Delphi technique as a research methodology for nursing. Int J Nurs Stud. 2001;38(2):195–200.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Hsu CC, Sandford BA. The Delphi technique: making sense of consensus. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2007;12(10):1–8. Hsu CC, Sandford BA. The Delphi technique: making sense of consensus. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2007;12(10):1–8.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Hallowell MR, Gambatese JA. Qualitative research: application of the Delphi method to CEM research. J Constr Eng Manag. 2009;136(1):99–107.CrossRef Hallowell MR, Gambatese JA. Qualitative research: application of the Delphi method to CEM research. J Constr Eng Manag. 2009;136(1):99–107.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Landeta J. Current validity of the Delphi method in social sciences. Technol Forecast Soc Change. 2006;73(5):467–82.CrossRef Landeta J. Current validity of the Delphi method in social sciences. Technol Forecast Soc Change. 2006;73(5):467–82.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sackman H. Delphi Critique. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books; 1975. Sackman H. Delphi Critique. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books; 1975.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Krosnick JA, Presser S. Question and questionnaire design. In: Marsden PV, Wright JD, editors. Handbook of survey research. 2nd ed. Bingley, UK: Emerald; 2010. Krosnick JA, Presser S. Question and questionnaire design. In: Marsden PV, Wright JD, editors. Handbook of survey research. 2nd ed. Bingley, UK: Emerald; 2010.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat McColl E, Jacoby A, Thomas L, Soutter J, Bamford C, Steen N, et al. Design and use of questionnaires: a review of best practice applicable to surveys of health service staff and patients. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5(31):1–256.CrossRefPubMed McColl E, Jacoby A, Thomas L, Soutter J, Bamford C, Steen N, et al. Design and use of questionnaires: a review of best practice applicable to surveys of health service staff and patients. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5(31):1–256.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Sudman S, Bradburn NM. Asking questions: a practical guide to questionnaire design. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1982. Sudman S, Bradburn NM. Asking questions: a practical guide to questionnaire design. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1982.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones WH, Lang JR. Sample composition bias and response bias in a mail survey: a comparison of inducement methods. J Market Res. 1980;26(1):69–76.CrossRef Jones WH, Lang JR. Sample composition bias and response bias in a mail survey: a comparison of inducement methods. J Market Res. 1980;26(1):69–76.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberson MT, Sundstrom E. Questionnaire design, return rates, and response favorableness in an employee attitude questionnaire. J Appl Psychol. 1990;75(3):354–7.CrossRef Roberson MT, Sundstrom E. Questionnaire design, return rates, and response favorableness in an employee attitude questionnaire. J Appl Psychol. 1990;75(3):354–7.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakash RA, Hutton JL, Jørstad-Stein EC, Gates S, Lamb SE. Maximising response to postal questionnaires–a systematic review of randomised trials in health research. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2006;6(1):5.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Nakash RA, Hutton JL, Jørstad-Stein EC, Gates S, Lamb SE. Maximising response to postal questionnaires–a systematic review of randomised trials in health research. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2006;6(1):5.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Moore DW. Measuring new types of question-order effects: additive and subtractive. Public Opin Q. 2002;66(1):80–91.CrossRef Moore DW. Measuring new types of question-order effects: additive and subtractive. Public Opin Q. 2002;66(1):80–91.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Schuman H, Presser S. Questions and answers in attitude surveys: experiments on question form, wording, and context. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1996. Schuman H, Presser S. Questions and answers in attitude surveys: experiments on question form, wording, and context. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1996.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Lasorsa DL. Question-order effects in surveys: the case of political interest, news attention, and knowledge. J Mass Commun Q. 2003;80(3):499–512. Lasorsa DL. Question-order effects in surveys: the case of political interest, news attention, and knowledge. J Mass Commun Q. 2003;80(3):499–512.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat DeMoranville CW, Bienstock CC. Question order effects in measuring service quality. Int J Res Mark. 2003;20(3):217–31.CrossRef DeMoranville CW, Bienstock CC. Question order effects in measuring service quality. Int J Res Mark. 2003;20(3):217–31.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat McFarland SG. Effects of question order on survey responses. Public Opin Q. 1981;45(2):208–15.CrossRef McFarland SG. Effects of question order on survey responses. Public Opin Q. 1981;45(2):208–15.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Schuman H, Presser S, Ludwig J. Context effects on survey responses to questions about abortion. Public Opin Q. 1981;45(2):216–23.CrossRef Schuman H, Presser S, Ludwig J. Context effects on survey responses to questions about abortion. Public Opin Q. 1981;45(2):216–23.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Tenvergert E, Gillespie MW, Kingma J, Klasen H. Abortion attitudes, 1984-1987-1988: effects of item order and dimensionality. Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74(2):627–42.CrossRefPubMed Tenvergert E, Gillespie MW, Kingma J, Klasen H. Abortion attitudes, 1984-1987-1988: effects of item order and dimensionality. Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74(2):627–42.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Blencowe NS, Strong S, McNair AG, Brookes ST, Crosby T, Griffin SM, et al. Reporting of short-term clinical outcomes after esophagectomy: a systematic review. Ann Surg. 2012;255:658–66.CrossRefPubMed Blencowe NS, Strong S, McNair AG, Brookes ST, Crosby T, Griffin SM, et al. Reporting of short-term clinical outcomes after esophagectomy: a systematic review. Ann Surg. 2012;255:658–66.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Macefield RC, Jacobs M, Korfage IJ, Nicklin J, Whistance RN, Brookes ST, et al. Developing core outcome sets: methods for identifying and including patient-reported outcomes (PROs). Trials. 2014;15:49.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Macefield RC, Jacobs M, Korfage IJ, Nicklin J, Whistance RN, Brookes ST, et al. Developing core outcome sets: methods for identifying and including patient-reported outcomes (PROs). Trials. 2014;15:49.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat McNair AG, MacKichan F, Donovan JL, Brookes ST, Avery KNL, Griffin SM, et al. What surgeons tell patients and what patients want to know before major cancer surgery: a qualitative study. BMC Cancer. 2016;16:258–65.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral McNair AG, MacKichan F, Donovan JL, Brookes ST, Avery KNL, Griffin SM, et al. What surgeons tell patients and what patients want to know before major cancer surgery: a qualitative study. BMC Cancer. 2016;16:258–65.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2015. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2015.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman and Hall; 1991. Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman and Hall; 1991.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Tourangeau R, Rasinski KA, Bradburn N, D’Andrade R. Belief accessibility and context effects in attitude measurement. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1989;25(5):401–21.CrossRef Tourangeau R, Rasinski KA, Bradburn N, D’Andrade R. Belief accessibility and context effects in attitude measurement. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1989;25(5):401–21.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Bickart BA. Carryover and backfire effects in marketing research. J Market Res. 1993;30(1):52.CrossRef Bickart BA. Carryover and backfire effects in marketing research. J Market Res. 1993;30(1):52.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulman KD, Howes N, Hopkins J, Whale K, Chalmers K, Brookes S, et al. A comparison of health professionals’ and patients’ views of the importance of outcomes of bariatric surgery. Obes Surg. 2016;26(11):2738–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Coulman KD, Howes N, Hopkins J, Whale K, Chalmers K, Brookes S, et al. A comparison of health professionals’ and patients’ views of the importance of outcomes of bariatric surgery. Obes Surg. 2016;26(11):2738–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Potter S, Brookes ST, Holcombe C, Ward JA, Blazeby JM. Exploring methods for the selection and integration of stakeholder views in the development of core outcome sets: a case study in reconstructive breast surgery. Trials. 2016;17:463.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Potter S, Brookes ST, Holcombe C, Ward JA, Blazeby JM. Exploring methods for the selection and integration of stakeholder views in the development of core outcome sets: a case study in reconstructive breast surgery. Trials. 2016;17:463.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Wills GB. Cognitive interviewing: a tool for improving questionnaire design. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2005.CrossRef Wills GB. Cognitive interviewing: a tool for improving questionnaire design. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2005.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz N, Schuman H. Political knowledge, attribution, and inferred interest in politics: the operation of buffer items. Int J Public Opinion Res. 1997;9(2):191–5.CrossRef Schwarz N, Schuman H. Political knowledge, attribution, and inferred interest in politics: the operation of buffer items. Int J Public Opinion Res. 1997;9(2):191–5.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Perreault WD. Controlling order-effect bias. Public Opin Q. 1975;39(4):544–51.CrossRef Perreault WD. Controlling order-effect bias. Public Opin Q. 1975;39(4):544–51.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Impact of question order on prioritisation of outcomes in the development of a core outcome set: a randomised controlled trial
verfasst von
Sara T. Brookes
Katy A. Chalmers
Kerry N. L. Avery
Karen Coulman
Jane M. Blazeby
on behalf of the ROMIO study group
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Trials / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1745-6215
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-017-2405-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

Trials 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe