Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 5-8/2014

Open Access 01.10.2014 | Research

Striatal and extrastriatal dopamine transporter levels relate to cognition in Lewy body diseases: an 11C altropane positron emission tomography study

verfasst von: Marta Marquie, Joseph J Locascio, Dorene M Rentz, J Alex Becker, Trey Hedden, Keith A Johnson, John H Growdon, Stephen N Gomperts

Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy | Ausgabe 5-8/2014

Abstract

Introduction

The biological basis of cognitive impairment in parkinsonian diseases is believed to be multifactorial. We investigated the contribution of dopamine deficiency to cognition in Parkinson disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) with dopamine transporter (DAT) imaging.

Methods

We acquired 11C altropane PET, magnetic resonance imaging and cognitive testing in 19 nondemented subjects with PD, 10 DLB and 17 healthy control subjects (HCS). We analyzed DAT concentration in putamen, caudate, anterior cingulate (AC), orbitofrontal and prefrontal regions, using the Standardized Uptake Volume Ratio with partial volume correction, and we related DAT concentration and global cortical thickness to neuropsychological performance.

Results

DAT concentration in putamen and in caudate were similar in PD and DLB groups and significantly lower than in HCS. Reduced caudate DAT concentration was associated with worse Clinical Dementia Rating Scale-sum of boxes (CDR-SB) scores and visuospatial skills in DLB but not in PD or HCS groups. Adjusting for putamen DAT concentration, as a measure of severity of motor disease, caudate DAT concentration was lower in DLB than in PD. Higher AC DAT concentration was associated with lower putamen DAT concentration in DLB and with higher putamen DAT concentration in PD. Higher AC DAT concentration in DLB correlated with greater impairment in semantic memory and language.

Conclusions

Caudate and AC dopamine dysfunction contribute in opposing directions to cognitive impairment in DLB.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s13195-014-0052-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

KAJ has received funding for travel and speaker honoraria from Pfizer Inc.; serves as a consultant for GEHC Ltd, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc./Eli Lilly and Company, Bayer Schering Pharma, Pfizer Inc, Elan Corporation/Janssen, and Bristol-Myers Squibb; and receives research support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc./Eli Lilly and Company, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Janssen (Janssen AI), and Pfizer Inc. The remaining authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Abkürzungen
AC
anterior cingulate
CDR-SB
Clinical Dementia Rating - sum of boxes
DAT
dopamine transporter
DLB
dementia with Lewy bodies
GCT
global cortical thickness
GLM
general linear model
H&Y
Hoehn and Yahr
HCS
healthy control subjects
LB
Lewy body
LED
l-dopamine equivalent dose
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
MRI
magnetic resonance imaging
PD
Parkinson disease without dementia
PET
positron emission tomography
ROI
region of interest
UPDRS
Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale

Introduction

Neuropathological studies support multiple causative factors for cognitive impairment in Lewy body (LB) diseases. These factors include alpha-synuclein aggregation at synapses [1] and in cortical Lewy aggregates [2], amyloid deposition [3], and loss of the brain’s neuromodulators [4]. Dopamine plays a central role in the regulation of movement, reward-seeking behavior and cognition [5]. Dopamine neurons that innervate the putamen regulate movement selection [6], while those projecting to the ventral striatum, caudate, and cognitively eloquent cortices such as the cingulate cortex participate in reward-seeking behavior and cognition [7]-[10].
The dopamine transporter (DAT) is a reliable marker of dopamine neuron synapses in most brain regions. Localized to the presynaptic terminals of dopamine cells, the DAT terminates neurotransmission by reuptaking synaptically released dopamine [11]. DAT levels are high in the striatum and moderate in multiple cortical regions, including the anterior cingulate and the orbitofrontal cortex [8],[9].
DAT imaging via positron emission tomography (PET) or single-photon emission computed tomography permits its measurement in vivo. In LB disorders, striatal DAT levels reflect the dopamine concentration and fall as nigral dopamine neurons are damaged [12]. Whereas reduction in the putamen DAT concentration correlates with motor impairment [13],[14], reduction in caudate DAT relative to putamen DAT levels has been linked to cognitive impairment in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) [15],[16]. We explored these relations with altropane (2β-carbomethoxy-3β(4-fluorophenyl)-n-(1-iodoprop-1-en-3-yl) nortropane), which is a cocaine analog DAT ligand with fast kinetics [17]. Its high selectivity for the DAT over other monoamine transporters (dopamine/serotonin=25:1, with minimal staining of the locus coeruleus) [18] makes altropane a specific marker of dopamine neurons, compared with β-CIT (dopamine/serotonin=2.4:1) [19] or FP-CIT (dopamine/serotonin=2.8:1) [20]. 18F-DOPA, another dopamine imaging marker, has even lower specificity because it labels all cells expressing aromatic acid dopamine decarboxylase [21]-[23], which include dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine neurons.
In this study we measured 11C-altropane PET retention to assess the striatal and extrastriatal DAT concentration in cognitively normal Parkinson disease without dementia (PD) subjects, DLB subjects and healthy control subjects (HCS), and related regional DAT levels to cognitive function. We hypothesized that the putamen DAT concentration would distinguish the LB groups from HCS, and that, relative to putamen DAT, the caudate and extrastriatal DAT concentrations would fall in DLB subjects compared with the PD and HCS groups. We further hypothesized that the DAT concentration in the caudate, in midline and ventromedial frontal cortical regions, and in medial temporal regions would relate to cognitive function.

Methods

Participants

We enrolled 46 individuals, including 19 with PD and 10 with DLB. Seventeen HCS served as a control group. Subjects were recruited from the Massachusetts General Hospital Movement Disorders and Memory Disorders Units. They gave informed consent to participate in this research study according to the protocol approved by the Partners HealthCare Inc. Institutional Review Board. They underwent standardized neurological examination, cognitive testing, 11C-altropane PET imaging, and structural brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) for Freesurfer-derived partial volume correction of PET data. Cohort demographics, clinical features, and neuropsychological performance are presented in Table 11.
Table 1
Participant characteristics and neuropsychological performance
 
PD subjects
DLB subjects
HCS
n
19
10
17
Age (years)
70.1 ± 7.0
72.1 ± 6.2
73.9 ± 6.7
Gender (female/male)
7/12a
1/9b
10/7a
Education (years)
16.4 ± 3.2
16.4 ± 3.5
16.2 ± 2.9
l-Dopamine equivalent dose (mg)
476.2 ± 379.8c
256.0 ± 280.3
0d
Hoehn and Yahr stage
2.5 ± 0.4c
2.25 ± 1.1c
0e
Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale
20.9 ± 7.4 f
30.3 ± 2.9b
1.2 ± 2.5e
Duration of motor symptoms (years)
8.6 ± 5.1a
3.0 ± 0.7d
-
Mini-Mental State Examination
28.1 ± 2.4a
19.3 ± 9.2b
28.9 ± 1.5a
Clinical Dementia Rating Scale - sum of boxes
0.4 ± 0.6a
7.2 ± 3.7b
0.1 ± 0.2a
Aggregate cognitive domain factors
   
Executive
   
Trailmaking Test B (seconds)
112.4 ± 67.7a
298.8 ± 3.0b
73.3 ± 27.7a
Digit Symbol
41.3 ± 9.0a
14.1 ± 11.1b
47.8 ± 21.6a
Episodic memory
   
Logical Memory I
12.9 ± 5.4a
5.6 ± 1.5b
16.5 ± 3.8a
Logical Memory II
12.1 ± 6.0a
5.4 ± 4.9b
16.1 ± 4.1a
Semantic memory and language
   
Free Selective Reminding Test
28.8 ± 6.0a
18.4 ± 10.0b
32.9 ± 6.2a
Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test
47.0 ± 1.6
45.3 ± 2.6
46.3 ± 4.0
Boston Naming Test
27.7 ± 1.7a
23.1 ± 7.9b
27.9 ± 3.0a
Letter Fluency
44.9 ± 12.4a
24.0 ± 14.1b
52.7 ± 12.2a
Category Fluency
31.8 ± 6.9a
13.9 ± 9.5b
38.9 ± 15.0a
Visuospatial skills
   
Visual Form Discrimination Test
28.9 ± 3.5a
24.7 ± 5.0b
30.6 ± 2.4
Values presented as mean ± standard deviation. PD, Parkinson disease without dementia; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; HCS, healthy control subjects. Significant differences (P<0.05) between groups are indicated as follows: adifferent from DLB; bdifferent from PD and HCS; cdifferent from HCS; ddifferent from PD; edifferent from PD and DLB; and fdifferent from DLB and HCS.
PD subjects fulfilled diagnostic criteria for idiopathic PD [24] and were nondemented. The diagnosis of DLB was consistent with current consensus criteria [25]. HCS had normal neurological examinations, no cognitive complaints, a global Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0 [26],[27], and cognitive test scores in the normal range.

Clinical and neuropsychological evaluations

Testing was acquired in the motor ‘on’ state to optimize cognitive performance [28]. The evaluation of motor function included the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stage [29] and the motor subscale of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) [30]. The following neuropsychological tests were administered: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [31], Logical Memory I and II (LogIA, LogIIA) [32], Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (Free Recall, Cued Recall) [33], Letter Fluency [34], Category Fluency [35], 30-item Boston Naming Test [36], Digit Symbol component of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised [37], Trailmaking Tests A and B [38], and the Visual Form Discrimination Test [34]. Functional status was assessed with the Clinical Dementia Rating - sum of boxes (CDR-SB) [26],[27]. Dopaminergic drug use was quantified as the l-dopamine equivalent dose (LED) [39]. We applied correlated factor analysis to the cognitive test performance of subjects to form four aggregate cognitive domain factors [40]: executive (Trailmaking Test B, Digit Symbol), episodic memory (Logical Memory I and II), semantic memory and language (Free Recall, Cued Recall, Boston Naming Test, Letter Fluency, Category Fluency), and visuospatial skills (Visual Form Discrimination Test). Cognitive domain factor scores were calculated as the average z score of the nonmissing component tests. A small number of subjects lacked some cognitive test scores, primarily due to dementia. No more than one-half of each factor’s component tests were allowed to be missing for a given subject.

Imaging acquisition and analysis

Altropane was prepared onsite at the Nuclear Medicine Department at the Massachusetts General Hospital. PET images were acquired using an HR+PET camera (Siemens, Munich, Germany) operating in three-dimensional mode. After a transmission scan, 15 mCi of 11C-altropane was injected as a bolus and followed by a 60-minute dynamic acquisition. PET data were reconstructed and corrected for attenuation with vendor-provided software. Each frame was evaluated to verify adequate count statistics and absence of head motion.
MRI data (Siemens 3 T) were acquired using an MP-RAGE sequence. Freesurfer [41] (version 5.1 [42]) was used to align cortical folding patterns [43] and to parcellate the cortical surface and segment the subcortical grey matter into predefined regions of interest (ROIs). Each subject’s altropane data volume was mapped onto the Freesurfer-derived cortical surface in native MRI space by sampling the PET data at the midpoint of the gray-matter ribbon. ROIs included the putamen, caudate, thalamus, anterior cingulate (AC; formed by caudal and rostral AC), orbitofrontal (formed by lateral and medial orbitofrontal), prefrontal (formed by pars orbitalis, pars triangularis, rostral middle frontal, superior frontal and frontal pole) and medial temporal (formed by entorhinal, parahippocampus and hippocampus) areas [44]. Each subject’s altropane data were rigidly mapped to the MP-RAGE, and the resulting transformation was used to map the Freesurfer-derived ROI definitions (cortical gray-matter ribbon and subcortical gray matter) onto the native-space PET volume, in order to derive ROI DAT concentration regional averages.
The DAT concentration was estimated with specific binding of altropane, which was computed in ROIs using the standardized uptake value ratio [45], a ratio of uptake in the target ROI to the reference region measured between 40 and 60 minutes post injection. Pericalcarine (visual) cortex was selected as a reference on the basis of its low DAT concentration [9] and low altropane binding [17]. To compensate for the dilutional effect resulting from the low spatial resolution of PET, partial volume correction was applied to the altropane standardized uptake value ratio using the correction factor derived from the convolved binary brain mask (two-component Meltzer method) as described previously [46]. Global cortical thickness (GCT, mm) and the caudate volume (mm3) were derived from Freesurfer.
PET and MRI-derived measurements are presented in Table22. DLB subjects showed a small (0.15 mm) but significant reduction in GCT compared with PD subjects (P=0.003) but not compared with HCS. The caudate volume did not differ across diagnostic groups.
Table 2
Participant imaging data
 
PD subjects
DLB subjects
HCS
Putamen DAT (SUVR-PV)
1.20 ± 0.18a
1.26 ± 0.38a
2.21 ± 0.46b
Caudate DAT
1.64 ± 0.35a
1.43 ± 0.48a
2.13 ± 0.46b
Anterior cingulate DAT
1.22 ± 0.12
1.12 ± 0.14
1.19 ± 0.12
Orbitofrontal DAT
1.26 ± 0.16
1.18 ± 0.16
1.27 ± 0.12
Prefrontal DAT
1.21 ± 0.21
1.02 ± 0.05
1.15 ± 0.12
Global cortical thickness (mm)
2.43 ± 0.12c
2.28 ± 0.12d
2.37 ± 0.08
Caudate volume (mm3)
3317 ± 424
3646 ± 647
3554 ± 665
Values presented as mean ± standard deviation. For the PET data, left-right averaged partial volume corrected SUVR values are shown. PD, Parkinson disease without dementia; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; HCS, healthy control subjects; DAT, dopamine transporter; SUVR-PV, standardized uptake value ratio partial volume corrected. Significant differences (P<0.05) between groups are indicated as follows: adifferent from HCS; bdifferent from PD and DLB; cdifferent from DLB; and ddifferent from PD.

Data analysis

Group differences for demographic and neuropsychological measures were assessed with the analysis of variance test followed by the Tukey post hoc test for quantitative variables, and with the Fisher’s exact test for qualitative variables.
Medial temporal and thalamus ROIs were eliminated from the analysis because their group mean DAT concentration did not differ significantly from 1.0 according to three tests: a one-sample t test, and two nonparametric tests (sign test and Wilcoxon signed-rank test). The DAT concentration in the remaining ROIs was evaluated using a backward elimination general linear model (GLM) regressed on the initial predictor pool: diagnostic group, age, education, putamen DAT, duration of motor symptoms, LED and the interaction of diagnosis with each of the other predictors. The cutoff P value for removal from the model was 0.01. Note that we did not perform a multiple comparison analysis across ROIs. For HCS, measures of duration of motor symptoms and LED were set to 0 (with the addition of slight random normal perturbations).
We sought to use an imaging measure of motor disease severity in order to isolate it from cognitive performance [15],[16]. The putamen DAT concentration and its interaction with diagnosis were thus included as predictor terms when the putamen DAT concentration was not the dependent variable. The basis for this approach rests on the correlation of putamen DAT concentration with dopamine concentration [12], and on our observation that the putamen DAT concentration correlates strongly with UPDRS and H&Y scores in the whole cohort (UPDRS, r 2=0.40, P<0.0001; H&Y scores, r 2=0.59, P<0.0001), a finding shared by prior investigations [47],[48]. To touch base with previous imaging studies [47], we independently assessed the caudate/putamen DAT ratio. This is a less general approach than the GLM analysis.
A covariate of caudate volume and its interaction with diagnosis were also included when the caudate DAT concentration was the dependent variable. Tukey post hoc tests were performed as required to follow up significant diagnostic main effects.
We assessed the relation of the DAT concentration to cognitive test performance using GLM analyses with backward elimination (P > 0.01 for removal from the model), with a pool of predictors that initially included: diagnostic group, DAT concentration in the selected ROIs (excluding putamen), interaction of diagnosis with ROIs, GCT, caudate volume, age, education, duration of motor symptoms, and LED.
Distributions of residuals for all analyses were checked and verified as meeting test assumptions.
To exclude the possibility that the GCT covariate was redundant and multicollinear with DAT concentration, given that the altropane retention data were already partial volume corrected (using local rather than global volume), we reran all the analyses without including GCT in the GLM’s initial predictor pool, and the results were virtually unchanged.
SAS software (version 9.3; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and JMP Pro software (version 10; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) were used for analysis and graphs.

Results

Subject characteristics

Diagnostic group differences in demographic, clinical, and cognitive variables are presented in Table11. As expected, DLB subjects were more impaired than PD subjects and HCS on the CDR-SB (P<0.0001), MMSE (P<0.0001), and each cognitive domain factor: semantic memory and language (P<0.001), episodic memory (P<0.0001), executive function (P<0.0001), and visuospatial skills (P=0.0001). Cognitive scores in the PD and HCS groups did not differ significantly. Mean H&Y values were comparable between DLB and PD subjects (P=0.50), and UPDRS scores were higher in DLB subjects than in PD subjects (P=0.03). All DLB subjects were taking cholinesterase inhibitors when the PET scan, neurological examination and cognitive testing were performed, while none of the PD or HCS subjects were. Previous studies have demonstrated that treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors does not affect DAT uptake [49].

Group differences in caudate and putamen dopamine transporter concentration

Unadjusted DAT concentrations in the putamen and the caudate were similar in PD and DLB subjects, and significantly lower than in HCS (P<0.0001 for putamen, P=0.0002 for caudate, analyses of variance; Table 11, Figure 11A,B). The use of parkinsonian medications such as levodopa and dopamine agonists did not contribute to the regional DAT concentration. For the entire sample, lower putamen and caudate DAT concentration was associated with worse motor function (putamen: UPDRS, r 2=0.40, P<0.0001; H&Y, r 2=0.59, P<0.0001; caudate: UPDRS, r 2=0.24, P=0.023; H&Y, r 2=0.24, P=0.0005).
To study the relationship of the caudate DAT concentration with diagnosis independently of the severity of motor disease, we evaluated the caudate DAT concentration using a GLM that included putamen DAT concentration as a covariate in the original pool of predictors. This reduced to an analysis of covariance model, in which the parallel slopes assumption was satisfied for diagnostic groups (P=0.43 for the test of difference in slopes). The adjusted caudate DAT concentration in the PD group was significantly higher than that for both the DLB (P=0.041) and HCS (P=0.018) groups (Figure 11C). The difference between PD and DLB subjects reflected a difference in relative caudate DAT binding, while the difference between PD subjects and HCS was driven by differences in putamen DAT levels. We found identical results when we used the caudate/putamen DAT ratio: a higher ratio in the PD group than in the DLB (P=0.01) and HCS (P<0.0001) groups (see Additional file 1).

Group differences in cortical dopamine transporter concentration

We assessed group differences in DAT concentration in three cortical regions that subserve cognition: AC, orbitofrontal cortex and prefrontal cortex. Unadjusted DAT concentration in the AC was similar across groups (Table22). Using a GLM model that related AC DAT concentration to putamen DAT concentration, global cortical thickness and other predictors (age, education, LED, duration of motor symptoms, and the interaction of diagnosis with each of the other predictors), we found that the diagnostic group and its interaction with putamen DAT concentration (P=0.003) and with GCT (P=0.008) was related to the AC DAT concentration (backward elimination GLM; for the model as a whole, R 2=0.45, P=0.003). Specifically, the relation of diagnostic group and AC DAT concentration was modified by the putamen DAT concentration, such that higher putamen DAT was associated with higher AC DAT concentration in the PD group, with lower AC DAT concentration in the DLB group, and with an essentially flat relation for the HCS group. The relation of diagnostic group and AC DAT concentration was also modified by GCT, such that higher GCT was associated with lower AC DAT concentration in the PD group, with higher AC DAT concentration in the HCS, and with a flat relation for the DLB group. These effects were limited to the AC, because the DAT concentration in the orbitofrontal and prefrontal ROIs did not differ between groups, even after accounting for differences in putamen DAT concentration and GCT (Table22).

Dopamine transporter concentration and cognitive performance

The diagnosis-dependent association of DAT concentration in the caudate and AC relative to DAT levels in the putamen led us to explore the relation of caudate and extrastriatal DAT concentration to cognitive function. For these analyses, we related regional DAT concentration to the CDR-SB, MMSE, and the four aggregate cognitive domain factors (see Data analysis).
We found in the DLB group alone that lower caudate DAT concentration was associated both with greater functional impairment, as measured with the CDR-SB (R 2=0.84 and P<0.0001 for model; P=0.0008 for interaction between caudate DAT concentration and diagnosis; Figure22A), and with greater visuospatial impairment (R 2=0.45 and P=0.0006 for model; P=0.008 for interaction between caudate DAT concentration and diagnosis; Figure22B). Further, in the DLB group alone, higher AC DAT concentration predicted greater impairment of semantic memory and language (R 2=0.69 and P<0.0001 for model; P=0.0003 for interaction between AC DAT concentration and diagnosis; Figure22C). The DAT concentration was not significantly associated with cognitive measures in either the PD or HCS groups. In contrast to DAT concentration, reduced GCT was associated with impairment in the MMSE (R 2=0.53 and P<0.0001 for model; P=0.0035 for main effect of GTC) and in episodic memory (R 2=0.50 and P<0.0001 for model; P=0.009 for main effect of GCT) in all diagnostic groups.

Discussion

In this study we measured the regional DAT concentration with altropane PET and related the DAT values to diagnosis and cognitive function in nondemented PD subjects, DLB subjects and HCS. We found that both caudate DAT and AC DAT concentrations varied across the diagnostic groups, yet contributed to cognitive function in DLB in opposing ways: low caudate DAT levels and high AC DAT levels were associated with greater impairment on cognitive testing.
Consistent with prior reports [50],[51], putamen and caudate DAT concentrations were significantly reduced in LB disorders compared with HCS, independent of the presence of dementia. Although unadjusted caudate DAT concentration was comparable between PD and DLB subjects, as shown previously [52], the caudate DAT concentration adjusted for putamen DAT concentration as a measure of severity of motor disease was significantly higher in PD subjects compared with DLB subjects and HCS. These data confirm prior reports showing a higher caudate/putamen DAT ratio in nondemented PD compared with DLB [47]. The data suggest that when dopamine neurons that project to the putamen are damaged in LB disorders, a parallel injury of caudate-projecting dopamine neurons contributes to cognitive impairment. Although HCS had the highest unadjusted caudate DAT concentration, we also observed a smaller adjusted caudate DAT concentration in HCS compared with PD subjects, driven by the higher putamen DAT signal of HCS.
The AC DAT concentration related to the diagnostic group, after adjusting for putamen DAT, GCT and their interactions. Specifically in PD, the AC DAT concentration decreased as the putamen DAT concentration fell, whereas it was independent of putamen DAT levels in HCS. Surprisingly, however, in the DLB group the AC DAT concentration actually increased as the putamen DAT concentration fell. Thus, as dopamine cells die and putamen DAT levels fall into the range associated with parkinsonism, the pathological processes responsible for dementia affect how the AC DAT concentration changes.
The relations of caudate and AC DAT concentrations to cognitive test performance were consistent with the diagnostic group differences in regional DAT concentration. Loss of caudate DAT concentration in DLB subjects was associated with greater functional impairment, as measured by the CDR-SB, and with greater impairment of visuospatial skills. Although we are not aware of prior reports relating caudate DAT levels to visuospatial function, caudate dopamine levels have been associated with both executive function [15],[53],[54] and memory [55].
We also found that higher AC DAT concentration in DLB subjects was associated with greater impairment of semantic memory and language. To interpret this unexpected result, further research will be necessary to determine whether, in DLB, the DAT is a faithful marker of dopamine terminal density and dopamine concentration, or whether DAT levels and dopamine terminal density diverge. In this respect, striatal DAT levels in primates correlate tightly with striatal dopamine levels after 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) treatment [12],[56]. In addition, DAT levels transiently downregulate only very early in the course of PD, but do not appear to do so subsequently [57]. Lastly, we found no evidence for dynamic regulation of regional DAT levels as a function of the LED in our cohort, consistent with prior reports [57]. Thus, while it remains possible that AC DAT upregulation could occur in DLB, thereby reducing released dopamine to pathophysiological levels, we propose for now that AC DAT levels in DLB instead reflect dopamine terminal density and the local dopamine concentration.
Prior preclinical and clinical work has demonstrated an inverted U-shaped relation between brain dopamine levels and cognition [58],[59], such that both excessive and insufficient dopamine levels impair cognitive performance. If pathological processes in DLB subjects increased the curvature of this relation in the caudate, this could explain how caudate and AC DAT levels in DLB subjects but not in PD subjects relate to cognition (Figure 33A). In addition, if pathological processes in the AC of DLB subjects shifted this inverted U-shaped curve to the left (or right; Figure 33B), previously optimum dopamine levels would now impair cognitive function and increases (or decreases) in dopamine would further worsen cognition.
Interestingly, cognitively normal PD subjects tolerated similar caudate and AC DAT levels as DLB subjects, without detectable impairment on cognitive testing. Dysfunction of the dopamine system therefore appears to contribute to cognitive impairment in DLB but is not sufficient for dementia. Other neuropathological factors, such as alpha-synuclein deposition [1], synaptic dysfunction [60], amyloid burden [40], and impairment of other neuromodulator systems [61], must make DLB patients susceptible to the impact of dopamine dysfunction.
Cortical DAT levels outside the AC did not distinguish between diagnostic groups or relate to cognitive test performance. This observation may reflect their differential innervation by midbrain dopamine cell groups [62]. Alternatively, the limited role for the DAT in dopamine clearance in some regions of the rat prefrontal cortex [63] may extend to humans. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that limited sample sizes may have obscured weak results. Future research with larger sample sizes will be useful.
Although cortical thickness was not the focus of this study, we also found that GCT was reduced in DLB compared with PD, and reduced GCT was associated with impaired MMSE and episodic memory performance. These observations are consistent with prior reports of cortical thinning in DLB [64].
The strengths of this study include the high specificity of altropane PET for DAT with negligible contamination from other monoamine transporters, accurate clinical diagnoses based on international clinical criteria, and the use of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery. An important technical note is the value of partial volume correction in altropane PET analyses, which enabled us to assess DAT levels not just in the striatum but also in cortical regions implicated in cognition.

Conclusions

The results of this altropane PET imaging study suggest that dopamine dysfunction in the caudate and in the AC, along with cortical atrophy, contribute in opposing ways to cognitive impairment and dementia in LB disorders. To the extent that DAT levels reflect dopamine synapse density in DLB, the association of cognitive impairment with both low caudate DAT levels and high AC DAT levels may limit the potential benefits of a dopamine-related cognitive therapeutic for LB diseases.

Note

This article is part of a series on Lewy Body Dementia, edited by Ian McKeith and James Galvin. Other articles in this series can be found at http://​alzres.​com/​series/​LewyBodyDementia​

Authors’ contributions

MM participated in the conception, organization and execution of the study, wrote the first draft, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. JJL designed and executed the statistical analysis, wrote the first draft, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspect of the work. DMR contributed to acquisition, analysis and interpretation of the cognitive data, reviewed and critiqued the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. JAB participated in the acquisition, analysis and interpretation of the image data, reviewed and critiqued the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. TH participated in the analysis and interpretation of the image data, reviewed and critiqued the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. KAJ participated in the conception, organization and execution of the study, reviewed and critiqued the manuscript, obtained funding for the study, supervised the project, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. JHG participated in the conception, organization and execution of the study, reviewed and critiqued the manuscript, obtained funding for the study, supervised the project, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. SNG participated in the conception, organization and execution of the study, wrote the first draft, obtained funding for the study, supervised the project, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Additional file

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr Randy L Buckner, PhD, from the Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory and Athinoula A Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, who provided research funding for the HCS group.
MM received research support from the Caja Madrid Foundation scholarship for postgraduate studies 2011-2013 (Madrid, Spain). JJL is a consultant for the Harvard Catalyst Bio-Statistical Consulting Group and serves on the Statistical Advisory Board of PLoS-One Journal. DMR received research support from the Alzheimer Association IIRG-08-90934 and the Michael J. Fox Foundation. JAB received research support from the National Institute of Health. TH received research support from the National Institute of Aging (K01 grant AG040197). KAJ receives research support from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, the National Institute of Health (National Institute of Aging, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke), the Alzheimer Association, and the American Health Assistance Foundation. JHG received research support from the National Institute on Aging and from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, and he served on the editorial board of JAMA Neurology, and on the Scientific Advisory Board of Neurimmune Therapeutics. SNG is funded by National Institute of Health grants from National Institute of Mental Health (1K08MH81207), the National Institute of Aging (National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, expired), and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R21 NS060310, expired), and receives research support from the Michael J. Fox Foundation.
This work was supported by the Michael J. Fox Foundation (to SNG and JHG), National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center Collaborative Project 5 U01 AG016976-11 (to SNG, KAJ, and JHG), the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (to KAJ), the National Institute on Aging (to KAJ and TH), the Alzheimer’s Disease Association (to KAJ), the Caja Madrid Foundation Scholarship for Postgraduate Studies 2011-2013 (to MM), and the Harvard Center for Neurodegeneration and Repair Pilot grant (to SNG). Funding for the HCS group was supported by the National Institute of Health to Dr Randy L Buckner, who serves as a contributor to this study. This study is not industry-sponsored.

Competing interests

KAJ has received funding for travel and speaker honoraria from Pfizer Inc.; serves as a consultant for GEHC Ltd, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc./Eli Lilly and Company, Bayer Schering Pharma, Pfizer Inc, Elan Corporation/Janssen, and Bristol-Myers Squibb; and receives research support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc./Eli Lilly and Company, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Janssen (Janssen AI), and Pfizer Inc. The remaining authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulz-Schaeffer WJ: The synaptic pathology of alpha-synuclein aggregation in dementia with Lewy bodies, Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 2010, 120: 131-143. 10.1007/s00401-010-0711-0.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schulz-Schaeffer WJ: The synaptic pathology of alpha-synuclein aggregation in dementia with Lewy bodies, Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 2010, 120: 131-143. 10.1007/s00401-010-0711-0.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Hurtig HI, Trojanowski JQ, Galvin J, Ewbank D, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, Clark CM, Glosser G, Stern MB, Gollomp SM, Arnold SE: Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 2000, 54: 1916-1921. 10.1212/WNL.54.10.1916.PubMedCrossRef Hurtig HI, Trojanowski JQ, Galvin J, Ewbank D, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, Clark CM, Glosser G, Stern MB, Gollomp SM, Arnold SE: Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 2000, 54: 1916-1921. 10.1212/WNL.54.10.1916.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Compta Y, Parkkinen L, O'Sullivan SS, Vandrovcova J, Holton JL, Collins C, Lashley T, Kallis C, Williams DR, de Silva R, Lees AJ, Revesz T: Lewy- and Alzheimer-type pathologies in Parkinson’s disease dementia: which is more important?. Brain. 2011, 134: 1493-1505. 10.1093/brain/awr031.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Compta Y, Parkkinen L, O'Sullivan SS, Vandrovcova J, Holton JL, Collins C, Lashley T, Kallis C, Williams DR, de Silva R, Lees AJ, Revesz T: Lewy- and Alzheimer-type pathologies in Parkinson’s disease dementia: which is more important?. Brain. 2011, 134: 1493-1505. 10.1093/brain/awr031.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Zgaljardic DJ, Foldi NS, Borod JC: Cognitive and behavioral dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: neurochemical and clinicopathological contributions. J Neural Transm. 2004, 111: 1287-1301. 10.1007/s00702-004-0178-z.PubMedCrossRef Zgaljardic DJ, Foldi NS, Borod JC: Cognitive and behavioral dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: neurochemical and clinicopathological contributions. J Neural Transm. 2004, 111: 1287-1301. 10.1007/s00702-004-0178-z.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Brooks DJ: Dopaminergic action beyond its effects on motor function: imaging studies. J Neurol. 2006, 253: IV8-IV15. 10.1007/s00415-006-4003-5.PubMed Brooks DJ: Dopaminergic action beyond its effects on motor function: imaging studies. J Neurol. 2006, 253: IV8-IV15. 10.1007/s00415-006-4003-5.PubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL: Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986, 9: 357-381. 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041.PubMedCrossRef Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL: Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986, 9: 357-381. 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR: A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997, 275: 1593-1599. 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593.PubMedCrossRef Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR: A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997, 275: 1593-1599. 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Sesack SR, Hawrylak VA, Matus C, Guido MA, Levey AI: Dopamine axon varicosities in the prelimbic division of the rat prefrontal cortex exhibit sparse immunoreactivity for the dopamine transporter. J Neurosci. 1998, 18: 2697-2708.PubMed Sesack SR, Hawrylak VA, Matus C, Guido MA, Levey AI: Dopamine axon varicosities in the prelimbic division of the rat prefrontal cortex exhibit sparse immunoreactivity for the dopamine transporter. J Neurosci. 1998, 18: 2697-2708.PubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis DA, Melchitzky DS, Sesack SR, Whitehead RE, Auh S, Sampson A: Dopamine transporter immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex: regional, laminar, and ultrastructural localization. J Comp Neurol. 2001, 432: 119-136. 10.1002/cne.1092.PubMedCrossRef Lewis DA, Melchitzky DS, Sesack SR, Whitehead RE, Auh S, Sampson A: Dopamine transporter immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex: regional, laminar, and ultrastructural localization. J Comp Neurol. 2001, 432: 119-136. 10.1002/cne.1092.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Brooks DJ, Piccini P: Imaging in Parkinson’s disease: the role of monoamines in behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 59: 908-918. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.017.PubMedCrossRef Brooks DJ, Piccini P: Imaging in Parkinson’s disease: the role of monoamines in behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 59: 908-918. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.017.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Amara SG, Kuhar MJ: Neurotransmitter transporters: recent progress. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993, 16: 73-93. 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.000445.PubMedCrossRef Amara SG, Kuhar MJ: Neurotransmitter transporters: recent progress. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993, 16: 73-93. 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.000445.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Tian L, Karimi M, Loftin SK, Brown CA, Xia H, Xu J, Mach RH, Perlmutter JS: No differential regulation of dopamine transporter (DAT) and vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2) binding in a primate model of Parkinson disease. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e31439-10.1371/journal.pone.0031439.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tian L, Karimi M, Loftin SK, Brown CA, Xia H, Xu J, Mach RH, Perlmutter JS: No differential regulation of dopamine transporter (DAT) and vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2) binding in a primate model of Parkinson disease. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e31439-10.1371/journal.pone.0031439.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Pirker W: Correlation of dopamine transporter imaging with parkinsonian motor handicap: how close is it?. Mov Disord. 2003, 18: S43-S51. 10.1002/mds.10579.PubMedCrossRef Pirker W: Correlation of dopamine transporter imaging with parkinsonian motor handicap: how close is it?. Mov Disord. 2003, 18: S43-S51. 10.1002/mds.10579.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang J, Zuo CT, Jiang YP, Guan YH, Chen ZP, Xiang JD, Yang LQ, Ding ZT, Wu JJ, Su HL: 18 F-FP-CIT PET imaging and SPM analysis of dopamine transporters in Parkinson’s disease in various Hoehn & Yahr stages. J Neurol. 2007, 254: 185-190. 10.1007/s00415-006-0322-9.PubMedCrossRef Wang J, Zuo CT, Jiang YP, Guan YH, Chen ZP, Xiang JD, Yang LQ, Ding ZT, Wu JJ, Su HL: 18 F-FP-CIT PET imaging and SPM analysis of dopamine transporters in Parkinson’s disease in various Hoehn & Yahr stages. J Neurol. 2007, 254: 185-190. 10.1007/s00415-006-0322-9.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger HJ, Cools AR, Horstink MW, Oyen WJ, Verhoeven EW, van der Werf SP: Striatal dopamine and learning strategy-an (123)I-FP-CIT SPECT study. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42: 1071-1078. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.007.PubMedCrossRef Berger HJ, Cools AR, Horstink MW, Oyen WJ, Verhoeven EW, van der Werf SP: Striatal dopamine and learning strategy-an (123)I-FP-CIT SPECT study. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42: 1071-1078. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.007.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Muller U, Wachter T, Barthel H, Reuter M, von Cramon DY: Striatal [123I]beta-CIT SPECT and prefrontal cognitive functions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm. 2000, 107: 303-319. 10.1007/s007020050025.PubMedCrossRef Muller U, Wachter T, Barthel H, Reuter M, von Cramon DY: Striatal [123I]beta-CIT SPECT and prefrontal cognitive functions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm. 2000, 107: 303-319. 10.1007/s007020050025.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischman AJ, Bonab AA, Babich JW, Livni E, Alpert NM, Meltzer PC, Madras BK: [(11)C, (127)I] altropane: a highly selective ligand for PET imaging of dopamine transporter sites. Synapse. 2001, 39: 332-342.PubMedCrossRef Fischman AJ, Bonab AA, Babich JW, Livni E, Alpert NM, Meltzer PC, Madras BK: [(11)C, (127)I] altropane: a highly selective ligand for PET imaging of dopamine transporter sites. Synapse. 2001, 39: 332-342.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Elmaleh DR, Fischman AJ, Shoup TM, Byon C, Hanson RN, Liang AY, Meltzer PC, Madras BK: Preparation and biological evaluation of iodine-125-IACFT: a selective SPECT agent for imaging dopamine transporter sites. J Nucl Med. 1996, 37: 1197-1202.PubMed Elmaleh DR, Fischman AJ, Shoup TM, Byon C, Hanson RN, Liang AY, Meltzer PC, Madras BK: Preparation and biological evaluation of iodine-125-IACFT: a selective SPECT agent for imaging dopamine transporter sites. J Nucl Med. 1996, 37: 1197-1202.PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Seibyl JP, Marek K, Sheff K, Zoghbi S, Baldwin RM, Charney DS, van Dyck CH, Innis RB: Iodine-123-beta-CIT and iodine-123-FPCIT SPECT measurement of dopamine transporters in healthy subjects and Parkinson’s patients. J Nucl Med. 1998, 39: 1500-1508.PubMed Seibyl JP, Marek K, Sheff K, Zoghbi S, Baldwin RM, Charney DS, van Dyck CH, Innis RB: Iodine-123-beta-CIT and iodine-123-FPCIT SPECT measurement of dopamine transporters in healthy subjects and Parkinson’s patients. J Nucl Med. 1998, 39: 1500-1508.PubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Abi-Dargham A, Gandelman MS, DeErausquin GA, Zea-Ponce Y, Zoghbi SS, Baldwin RM, Laruelle M, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Neumeyer JL, Innis RB: SPECT imaging of dopamine transporters in human brain with iodine-123-fluoroalkyl analogs of beta-CIT. J Nucl Med. 1996, 37: 1129-1133.PubMed Abi-Dargham A, Gandelman MS, DeErausquin GA, Zea-Ponce Y, Zoghbi SS, Baldwin RM, Laruelle M, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Neumeyer JL, Innis RB: SPECT imaging of dopamine transporters in human brain with iodine-123-fluoroalkyl analogs of beta-CIT. J Nucl Med. 1996, 37: 1129-1133.PubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu XS, Okamura N, Arai H, Higuchi M, Matsui T, Tashiro M, Shinkawa M, Itoh M, Ido T, Sasaki H: 18 F-fluorodopa PET study of striatal dopamine uptake in the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2000, 55: 1575-1577. 10.1212/WNL.55.10.1575.PubMedCrossRef Hu XS, Okamura N, Arai H, Higuchi M, Matsui T, Tashiro M, Shinkawa M, Itoh M, Ido T, Sasaki H: 18 F-fluorodopa PET study of striatal dopamine uptake in the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2000, 55: 1575-1577. 10.1212/WNL.55.10.1575.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Rakshi JS, Uema T, Ito K, Bailey DL, Morrish PK, Ashburner J, Dagher A, Jenkins IH, Friston KJ, Brooks DJ: Frontal, midbrain and striatal dopaminergic function in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease A 3D [(18)F]dopa-PET study. Brain. 1999, 122: 1637-1650. 10.1093/brain/122.9.1637.PubMedCrossRef Rakshi JS, Uema T, Ito K, Bailey DL, Morrish PK, Ashburner J, Dagher A, Jenkins IH, Friston KJ, Brooks DJ: Frontal, midbrain and striatal dopaminergic function in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease A 3D [(18)F]dopa-PET study. Brain. 1999, 122: 1637-1650. 10.1093/brain/122.9.1637.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Klein JC, Eggers C, Kalbe E, Weisenbach S, Hohmann C, Vollmar S, Baudrexel S, Diederich NJ, Heiss WD, Hilker R: Neurotransmitter changes in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease dementia in vivo. Neurology. 2010, 74: 885-892. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d55f61.PubMedCrossRef Klein JC, Eggers C, Kalbe E, Weisenbach S, Hohmann C, Vollmar S, Baudrexel S, Diederich NJ, Heiss WD, Hilker R: Neurotransmitter changes in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease dementia in vivo. Neurology. 2010, 74: 885-892. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d55f61.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Blankson S, Lees AJ: A clinicopathologic study of 100 cases of Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1993, 50: 140-148. 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540020018011.PubMedCrossRef Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Blankson S, Lees AJ: A clinicopathologic study of 100 cases of Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1993, 50: 140-148. 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540020018011.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O'Brien JT, Feldman H, Cummings J, Duda JE, Lippa C, Perry EK, Aarsland D, Arai H, Ballard CG, Boeve B, Burn DJ, Costa D, Del Ser T, Dubois B, Galasko D, Gauthier S, Goetz CG, Gomez-Tortosa E, Halliday G, Hansen LA, Hardy J, Iwatsubo T, Kalaria RN, Kaufer D, Kenny RA, Korczyn A, et al: Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2005, 65: 1863-1872. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1.PubMedCrossRef McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O'Brien JT, Feldman H, Cummings J, Duda JE, Lippa C, Perry EK, Aarsland D, Arai H, Ballard CG, Boeve B, Burn DJ, Costa D, Del Ser T, Dubois B, Galasko D, Gauthier S, Goetz CG, Gomez-Tortosa E, Halliday G, Hansen LA, Hardy J, Iwatsubo T, Kalaria RN, Kaufer D, Kenny RA, Korczyn A, et al: Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2005, 65: 1863-1872. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982, 140: 566-572. 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566.PubMedCrossRef Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL: A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982, 140: 566-572. 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993, 43: 2412-2414. 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a.PubMedCrossRef Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993, 43: 2412-2414. 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange KW, Paul GM, Robbins TW, Marsden CD: L-dopa and frontal cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol. 1993, 60: 475-478.PubMed Lange KW, Paul GM, Robbins TW, Marsden CD: L-dopa and frontal cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol. 1993, 60: 475-478.PubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoehn MM, Yahr MD: Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967, 17: 427-442. 10.1212/WNL.17.5.427.PubMedCrossRef Hoehn MM, Yahr MD: Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967, 17: 427-442. 10.1212/WNL.17.5.427.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang A, Fahn S: Assessment of Parkinson’s disease. Quantification of Neurologic Deficit. 1989, Butterworths, Boston, MA, 285-309. Lang A, Fahn S: Assessment of Parkinson’s disease. Quantification of Neurologic Deficit. 1989, Butterworths, Boston, MA, 285-309.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975, 12: 189-198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.PubMedCrossRef Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975, 12: 189-198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Weschler D: The Wechsler Memory Scale. 1997, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX Weschler D: The Wechsler Memory Scale. 1997, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Grober E, Lipton RB, Hall C, Crystal H: Memory impairment on free and cued selective reminding predicts dementia. Neurology. 2000, 54: 827-832. 10.1212/WNL.54.4.827.PubMedCrossRef Grober E, Lipton RB, Hall C, Crystal H: Memory impairment on free and cued selective reminding predicts dementia. Neurology. 2000, 54: 827-832. 10.1212/WNL.54.4.827.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O: Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. 1983, Oxford University Press, Oxford Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O: Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. 1983, Oxford University Press, Oxford
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, Salmon DP, Katzman R, Thal LJ: Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol. 1992, 49: 1253-1258. 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017.PubMedCrossRef Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, Salmon DP, Katzman R, Thal LJ: Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol. 1992, 49: 1253-1258. 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S: The Boston Naming Test: Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 1983, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia, PA Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S: The Boston Naming Test: Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 1983, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia, PA
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Weschler D: WAIS-III, Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition, Administration and Scoring Manual. 1997, The Psychological Corporation, New York Weschler D: WAIS-III, Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition, Administration and Scoring Manual. 1997, The Psychological Corporation, New York
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Reitan R: Manual for Administration of Neuropsychological Test Batteries for Adults and Children. 1979, Tucson, AZ, Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratories Reitan R: Manual for Administration of Neuropsychological Test Batteries for Adults and Children. 1979, Tucson, AZ, Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratories
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE: Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010, 25: 2649-2653. 10.1002/mds.23429.PubMedCrossRef Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE: Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010, 25: 2649-2653. 10.1002/mds.23429.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Gomperts SN, Locascio JJ, Marquie M, Santarlasci AL, Rentz DM, Maye J, Johnson KA, Growdon JH: Brain amyloid and cognition in Lewy body diseases. Mov Disord. 2012, 27: 965-973. 10.1002/mds.25048.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gomperts SN, Locascio JJ, Marquie M, Santarlasci AL, Rentz DM, Maye J, Johnson KA, Growdon JH: Brain amyloid and cognition in Lewy body diseases. Mov Disord. 2012, 27: 965-973. 10.1002/mds.25048.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI: Cortical surface-based analysis, I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999, 9: 179-194. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395.PubMedCrossRef Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI: Cortical surface-based analysis, I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999, 9: 179-194. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM: High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999, 8: 272-284. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4.PubMedCrossRef Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM: High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999, 8: 272-284. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ: An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006, 31: 968-980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.PubMedCrossRef Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ: An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006, 31: 968-980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Thie JA: Understanding the standardized uptake value, its methods, and implications for usage. J Nucl Med. 2004, 45: 1431-1434.PubMed Thie JA: Understanding the standardized uptake value, its methods, and implications for usage. J Nucl Med. 2004, 45: 1431-1434.PubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Becker JA, Hedden T, Carmasin J, Maye J, Rentz DM, Putcha D, Fischl B, Greve DN, Marshall GA, Salloway S, Marks D, Buckner RL, Sperling RA, Johnson KA: Amyloid-beta associated cortical thinning in clinically normal elderly. Ann Neurol. 2011, 69: 1032-1042. 10.1002/ana.22333.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Becker JA, Hedden T, Carmasin J, Maye J, Rentz DM, Putcha D, Fischl B, Greve DN, Marshall GA, Salloway S, Marks D, Buckner RL, Sperling RA, Johnson KA: Amyloid-beta associated cortical thinning in clinically normal elderly. Ann Neurol. 2011, 69: 1032-1042. 10.1002/ana.22333.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker Z, Costa DC, Walker RW, Lee L, Livingston G, Jaros E, Perry R, McKeith I, Katona CL: Striatal dopamine transporter in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease: a comparison. Neurology. 2004, 62: 1568-1572. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000123248.39847.1D.PubMedCrossRef Walker Z, Costa DC, Walker RW, Lee L, Livingston G, Jaros E, Perry R, McKeith I, Katona CL: Striatal dopamine transporter in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease: a comparison. Neurology. 2004, 62: 1568-1572. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000123248.39847.1D.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Seibyl JP, Marek KL, Quinlan D, Sheff K, Zoghbi S, Zea-Ponce Y, Baldwin RM, Fussell B, Smith EO, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Innis RB: Decreased single-photon emission computed tomographic [123I]beta-CIT striatal uptake correlates with symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1995, 38: 589-598. 10.1002/ana.410380407.PubMedCrossRef Seibyl JP, Marek KL, Quinlan D, Sheff K, Zoghbi S, Zea-Ponce Y, Baldwin RM, Fussell B, Smith EO, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Innis RB: Decreased single-photon emission computed tomographic [123I]beta-CIT striatal uptake correlates with symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1995, 38: 589-598. 10.1002/ana.410380407.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor JP, Colloby SJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, Williams D, Patterson J, O'Brien JT: Cholinesterase inhibitor use does not significantly influence the ability of 123I-FP-CIT imaging to distinguish Alzheimer’s disease from dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007, 78: 1069-1071. 10.1136/jnnp.2006.111666.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Taylor JP, Colloby SJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, Williams D, Patterson J, O'Brien JT: Cholinesterase inhibitor use does not significantly influence the ability of 123I-FP-CIT imaging to distinguish Alzheimer’s disease from dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007, 78: 1069-1071. 10.1136/jnnp.2006.111666.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker Z, Costa DC, Walker RW, Shaw K, Gacinovic S, Stevens T, Livingston G, Ince P, McKeith IG, Katona CL: Differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease using a dopaminergic presynaptic ligand. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002, 73: 134-140. 10.1136/jnnp.73.2.134.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Walker Z, Costa DC, Walker RW, Shaw K, Gacinovic S, Stevens T, Livingston G, Ince P, McKeith IG, Katona CL: Differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease using a dopaminergic presynaptic ligand. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002, 73: 134-140. 10.1136/jnnp.73.2.134.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Colloby SJ, O'Brien JT, Fenwick JD, Firbank MJ, Burn DJ, McKeith IG, Williams ED: The application of statistical parametric mapping to 123I-FP-CIT SPECT in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage. 2004, 23: 956-966. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.045.PubMedCrossRef Colloby SJ, O'Brien JT, Fenwick JD, Firbank MJ, Burn DJ, McKeith IG, Williams ED: The application of statistical parametric mapping to 123I-FP-CIT SPECT in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage. 2004, 23: 956-966. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.045.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Song IU, Kim YD, Cho HJ, Chung SW, Chung YA: An FP-CIT PET comparison of the differences in dopaminergic neuronal loss between idiopathic Parkinson disease with dementia and without dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013, 27: 51-55. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31824acd84.PubMedCrossRef Song IU, Kim YD, Cho HJ, Chung SW, Chung YA: An FP-CIT PET comparison of the differences in dopaminergic neuronal loss between idiopathic Parkinson disease with dementia and without dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013, 27: 51-55. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31824acd84.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Marie RM, Barre L, Dupuy B, Viader F, Defer G, Baron JC: Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal executive tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 1999, 260: 77-80. 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00928-8.PubMedCrossRef Marie RM, Barre L, Dupuy B, Viader F, Defer G, Baron JC: Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal executive tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 1999, 260: 77-80. 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00928-8.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruck A, Portin R, Lindell A, Laihinen A, Bergman J, Haaparanta M, Solin O, Rinne JO: Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 311: 81-84. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02124-3.PubMedCrossRef Bruck A, Portin R, Lindell A, Laihinen A, Bergman J, Haaparanta M, Solin O, Rinne JO: Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 311: 81-84. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02124-3.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Jokinen P, Bruck A, Aalto S, Forsback S, Parkkola R, Rinne JO: Impaired cognitive performance in Parkinson’s disease is related to caudate dopaminergic hypofunction and hippocampal atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009, 15: 88-93. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.03.005.PubMedCrossRef Jokinen P, Bruck A, Aalto S, Forsback S, Parkkola R, Rinne JO: Impaired cognitive performance in Parkinson’s disease is related to caudate dopaminergic hypofunction and hippocampal atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009, 15: 88-93. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.03.005.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Bezard E, Dovero S, Prunier C, Ravenscroft P, Chalon S, Guilloteau D, Crossman AR, Bioulac B, Brotchie JM, Gross CE: Relationship between the appearance of symptoms and the level of nigrostriatal degeneration in a progressive 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-lesioned macaque model of Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci. 2001, 21: 6853-6861.PubMed Bezard E, Dovero S, Prunier C, Ravenscroft P, Chalon S, Guilloteau D, Crossman AR, Bioulac B, Brotchie JM, Gross CE: Relationship between the appearance of symptoms and the level of nigrostriatal degeneration in a progressive 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-lesioned macaque model of Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci. 2001, 21: 6853-6861.PubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Nandhagopal R, Kuramoto L, Schulzer M, Mak E, Cragg J, McKenzie J, McCormick S, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Stoessl AJ: Longitudinal evolution of compensatory changes in striatal dopamine processing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2011, 134: 3290-3298. 10.1093/brain/awr233.PubMedCrossRef Nandhagopal R, Kuramoto L, Schulzer M, Mak E, Cragg J, McKenzie J, McCormick S, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Stoessl AJ: Longitudinal evolution of compensatory changes in striatal dopamine processing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2011, 134: 3290-3298. 10.1093/brain/awr233.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS: The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol. 1994, 71: 515-528.PubMed Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS: The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol. 1994, 71: 515-528.PubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Cools R, D′Esposito M: Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011, 69: e113-e125. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cools R, D′Esposito M: Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011, 69: e113-e125. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Nemani VM, Lu W, Berge V, Nakamura K, Onoa B, Lee MK, Chaudhry FA, Nicoll RA, Edwards RH: Increased expression of alpha-synuclein reduces neurotransmitter release by inhibiting synaptic vesicle reclustering after endocytosis. Neuron. 2010, 65: 66-79. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.023.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nemani VM, Lu W, Berge V, Nakamura K, Onoa B, Lee MK, Chaudhry FA, Nicoll RA, Edwards RH: Increased expression of alpha-synuclein reduces neurotransmitter release by inhibiting synaptic vesicle reclustering after endocytosis. Neuron. 2010, 65: 66-79. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.023.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Dubois B, Ruberg M, Javoy-Agid F, Ploska A, Agid Y: A subcortico-cortical cholinergic system is affected in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 1983, 288: 213-218. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90096-3.PubMedCrossRef Dubois B, Ruberg M, Javoy-Agid F, Ploska A, Agid Y: A subcortico-cortical cholinergic system is affected in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 1983, 288: 213-218. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90096-3.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS: Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex. 1998, 8: 321-345. 10.1093/cercor/8.4.321.PubMedCrossRef Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS: Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex. 1998, 8: 321-345. 10.1093/cercor/8.4.321.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaenmaki M, Tammimaki A, Myohanen T, Pakarinen K, Amberg C, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Mannisto PT: Quantitative role of COMT in dopamine clearance in the prefrontal cortex of freely moving mice. J Neurochem. 2010, 114: 1745-1755. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06889.x.PubMedCrossRef Kaenmaki M, Tammimaki A, Myohanen T, Pakarinen K, Amberg C, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Mannisto PT: Quantitative role of COMT in dopamine clearance in the prefrontal cortex of freely moving mice. J Neurochem. 2010, 114: 1745-1755. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06889.x.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Delli Pizzi S, Franciotti R, Tartaro A, Caulo M, Thomas A, Onofrj M, Bonanni L: Structural alteration of the dorsal visual network in DLB patients with visual hallucinations: a cortical thickness MRI study. PLoS One. 2014, 9: e86624-10.1371/journal.pone.0086624.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Delli Pizzi S, Franciotti R, Tartaro A, Caulo M, Thomas A, Onofrj M, Bonanni L: Structural alteration of the dorsal visual network in DLB patients with visual hallucinations: a cortical thickness MRI study. PLoS One. 2014, 9: e86624-10.1371/journal.pone.0086624.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Striatal and extrastriatal dopamine transporter levels relate to cognition in Lewy body diseases: an 11C altropane positron emission tomography study
verfasst von
Marta Marquie
Joseph J Locascio
Dorene M Rentz
J Alex Becker
Trey Hedden
Keith A Johnson
John H Growdon
Stephen N Gomperts
Publikationsdatum
01.10.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy / Ausgabe 5-8/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1758-9193
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-014-0052-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 5-8/2014

Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 5-8/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.