Skip to main content

Effect of household size on mental problems in children: results from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort study

Abstract

Background

Most people in industrialized societies grow up in core (parents only) families with few if any siblings. Based on an evolutionary perspective, it may be argued that this environment reflects a mismatch, in that the tribal setting offered a larger number of close affiliates. The present project examined whether this mismatch may have a negative impact on mental health.

Methods

We used data from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa), which includes 114 500 children. The mothers were recruited during pregnancy and followed up with questionnaires as the infants grew older. Correlates between number and type of people living in the household and questions probing mental health were corrected for likely confounders.

Results

The number of household members correlated with scores on good mental health at all ages tested (3, 5 and 8 years). The effects were distinct, highly significant, and present regardless of how mental issues were scored. The outcome could be attributed to having older siblings, rather than adults beyond parents. The more siblings, and the closer in age, the more pronounced was the effect. Living with a single mother did not make any difference compared to two parents. Girls were slightly more responsive to the presence of siblings than boys. Household pets did not have any appreciable impact.

Conclusion

A large household is associated with fewer mental problems in children.

Peer Review reports

Background

The high prevalence of anxiety and depression related problems in adolescents and adults suggests that the current environment, or way of life, is not optimal. An evolutionary perspective may help identify possible contributing factors. The concept Environment of Evolutionary Adaptation (EEA) has been coined to suggest a type of environment in which we are genetically designed to flourish [1]. While most discrepancies, or mismatches, between the present setting and the EEA are either neutral or beneficiary, some presumably contribute to mental or physical morbidity. These latter may be referred to as discords [2, 3]. If we can pinpoint the discords responsible for the high prevalence of mental problems, it may be possible to initiate preventive measures. As the brain is most malleable during the first years of life, it seems reasonably to focus on infancy.

While it is relatively easy to suggest mismatches, it requires dedicated research to identify relevant discords. The point is succinctly exemplified in the case of near-sightedness. The difference in prevalence between people living in cities (up to 80 % in young men) compared to rural areas (typically 1 %) [4] strongly suggests the involvement of discords. The leading candidates, in the form of obvious mismatches, were: one, focusing on a close and fixed distance (as in reading); and two, not having a natural diurnal cycle of light (the light being on at night). However, recent research suggests that the main discord is the lack of time infants spend outdoor, as the eyes require a certain amount of strong (sun)light in order to develop correctly [5].

Mental problems are considerably more difficult to deal with than near-sightedness, consequently it is particularly important to find relevant discords. If the environment can be adapted accordingly, it may reduce the future toll of mental agony.

As in the case of near-sightedness, there is a range of candidate discords, particularly in connection with anxiety [6]. Data from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) offer an opportunity to investigate some factors [7].

The MoBa questionnaires were not designed with an evolutionary perspective in mind, and are therefore not ideal for the present purpose. Moreover, the Norwegian population is relatively homogenous as to key child rearing practices, and thus not suitable for the evaluation of all potential discords. We consequently focused on one factor: the number of people present in the household. There is appreciable variation in the MoBa cohort as to household size. Information regarding age-classified members is gathered during pregnancy, implying that the siblings recorded are older than the index child. In a typical (Stone Age) tribal setting, there would be a larger number of close affiliates compared to typical homes in industrialized societies. The affiliates would presumably offer a sense of safety as well as a social context, input that theoretically could reduce the activation of fear and low mood modules of the brain. Less activation would mean less “exercise” of these functions, and thus less strengthening of the underlying neural circuits. In other words, a perceived lack of company by supportive people could be theorized to increase the risk of both anxiety and depression related problems.

A strong social network is well known to contribute to well-being, mental health and longevity in adults [810], including young adults [11]. For infants, the family constitutes the main social context. The question is whether a similar effect can be seen in infants, and if so, which relatives or others contribute in this direction.

There are some previous reports investigating the relationship between mental problems and the size of family in which infants grow up, primarily looking at effects later in life. In a deprived setting, the correlation may actually be positive; i.e., a large number of siblings have a negative impact. For example, a study of poor, rural communities in Mexico found that family size predicts anxiety in adolescents [12]; and a related study of urban slum-children in India found a correlation between family size and psychiatric disorders [13]. However, these results may reflect the stress and problems related to raising many children with limited resources.

Data from Western, affluent settings are more conflicting. A UK based study found a univariate association between family size and increased risk of childhood psychiatric disorder, but the association disappeared when correcting for confounders [14]. In this study, siblings were recorded as either “2 or fewer”, or “3 or more”. Socieconomic factors appeared more important than the number of siblings. Another UK study suggests that having one or two siblings may be protective, while larger families may cause an increased chance of mental problems in the elder siblings [15]. Data from China indicate reduced depression in adolescents who did not have any siblings [16], but the one child family policy in this country may contribute to the result.

In these studies, the outcomes are mental problems of sufficient magnitude to warrant a diagnosis at later stages in life. A report from Australia was based on a design more similar to the one used in the present study [17]. They used questionnaires filled in by the mothers, and found that small family size predicts internalizing behaviour in infants. The present study was also based on mothers’ reports on their infants while they were still young. The questionnaires used in both studies indicate internalising (anxiety, depression) and externalising (aggression, opposition, defiance) behaviour. A high score on these instruments predicts mental health issues later in life [18, 19].

The present study included a number of variables regarding the type of household the infant was born into. Thus, the dataset allowed for the adjustment for key confounders such as maternal age, maternal and paternal educational level, family income, maternal and paternal period of leave from work after birth, maternal breastfeeding status when child is 18 months, and the presence of pets. We aimed to examine the possible effects of: 1) family size in general; and 2) type of members (one or two parents, grandparents, other adults, siblings). The outcome variables were: 1) temperament; 2), behaviour problems; and 3) symptoms of anxiety and depression. The scores were obtained for the age period 3-8 years. As the information was registered for new-born infants, the dataset only include information as to older siblings. The data was not suitable for the question of whether younger siblings would give a similar effect.

Methods

Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) (http://www.fhi.no/morogbarn) is a prospective, population-based cohort study initiated by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health [7, 20]. Participants (95 200 mothers and 114 500 children) were recruited from throughout Norway from 1999 to 2008 and are almost exclusively ethnic Norwegians. Participants did not receive financial compensation, yet 40.6 % of those approached were enrolled. A written informed consent was obtained from participants, as well as a licence from the Norwegian Data Inspectorate. There are follow-ups with new questionnaires at regular intervals. Using data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway, it has been indicated that although prevalence estimates of exposures and outcomes in the MoBa study may be biased owing to selection, estimates of exposure-outcome associations are less likely to be affected, and therefore do not constitute a serious validity problem in terms of representativeness [7].

The present study is based on version 8 (released February 2014) of the quality-assured data files. The study has been approved by the Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics. Data were collected from Questionnaire 1 (gestational week 17), Questionnaire 4 (6 months after birth), Questionnaire 5 (18 months after birth), Questionnaire 6 (3 years after birth), Questionnaire 7 (5 years after birth), and Questionnaire 8 (8 years after birth). The dependent variables were based on responses from mothers of respectively 51 569 children (3 years), 28 627 children (5 years), and 17 594 children (8 years). The reduction in numbers, compared to the initial recruitment, is due to the combination of: 1) A general tendency to drop out as the child ages; 2) lack of response to key questions; 3) the questionnaire was only sent to a subset of parents (5 years); and 4) the participants were recruited over a 10 year period, and the part of the sample recruited most recently had not completed the last questionnaire (8 years).

The present study focused on household size as the main exposure variable. Questionnaire 1, which was submitted during pregnancy, asks about the number of persons sharing the household. There were also more detailed questions about types of relatives in the household. These data were used to probe for correlates to questions relating to mental issues in the children as they grew older.

Measures

Independent variables

Household size and type were reported in Questionnaire 1 (during pregnancy). The mothers responded to items such as: “How many people, including you, live in your home?”, and “With whom do you live?”. Response categories were “Spouse/partner”, “Parents”, “Parents-in-law”, “Children”, “No one”, and “Other, describe”. Number and approximate age of children (presumed to be siblings) present (not including the study child) was reported as: Number of people between: “12–18 years”, “6–11 years”, and “under 6 years”, respectively. The numbers of siblings were coded 1, 2, or >2.

Covariates

Some variables were included because they might confound the relationship between household size and children’s mental health: Maternal and paternal period of leave from work after child’s birth, was reported by the mother for herself and for the child’s father when the child was 18 months. Maternal leave was coded: no leave = 0, <2 months = 1, 2–6 months = 2, 6–9 months = 3, 9–12 months = 4, 12–18 months = 5, >18 months = 6. Paternal leave was coded: no leave = 0, <1 month = 1, 1–2 months = 2, 2–3 months = 3, 3–6 months = 4, and >6 months = 5. Duration of breastfeeding was reported when the child was 18 months. A summative index was generated and scored 0–5 based on whether the mother reported breastfeeding at: 6–8 months = 1, 9–11 months = 2, 12–14 months = 3, 15–18 months = 4. The last category also included those who reported to be breastfeeding at least once a week at 18 months. The presence of Animals (pets) in the family, reported when the child was 6 months, was coded as a dichotomous variable. The data were adjusted for a number of additional covariates: Maternal age was categorized into five year intervals from <20 years to ≥35 years. Maternal and paternal income (reported in Questionnaire 1) were summed and used as a categorical variable with seven response categories from no income to > NOK 500 000. Maternal and paternal education were reported in the same questionnaire as one of six categories ranging from: “9 year elementary school” to “at least 4 years at university”. The child’s sex was also used as a covariate.

Dependent variables

The outcome variables were generated based on symptoms at age 3, 5 and 8 years as reported by the mother. The items at age 3 and 5 were picked from various symptom lists in the questionnaires, based on what the authors judged as good face validity for the present purpose.. The data were factor analysed using an oblique rotation. In the 3 year questionnaire, we used all the 26 available items from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [21]. Four factors were generated: Emotional regulation, Anxiety, Eating/somatic, and Hyperactivity/concentration. Items and the factor loadings are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Factor loadings for the items included (bold) in the outcome measures at age 3 years

Outcome variables at 5 years of age were based on nine selected items from the CBCL, five from the Emotionality, Activity and Shyness Temperament Questionnaire (EAS) [22], and two items made for the MoBa study. Data from these 16 items were factor analysed with an oblique rotation. A two factor solution was chosen. The first factor was referred to as Anxiety, the other Difficult temperament. Items and factor loadings are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Factor loadings for the items included (bold) in the outcome measures at age 5 years

The 8 year questionnaire included short-forms of two instruments: The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) [23] is a multidimensional instrument generated to measure Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-defined anxiety symptom in children. The present Anxiety score was based on a five item version [24]. The response categories were “Not true”, “Sometimes true” and “True”. Our Depression score was based on the 13 item version of the DSM-adapted Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) [25]. The response categories were the same as in SCARED. The list of items is shown in Table 3.

Table 3 The 13 item version of the Short Mood and Feeling Questionnaire

In addition to the mental health outcomes, we used a number of questions on somatic outcomes for supplementary analyses. The purpose was to test for possible confounding effects of maternal temperament. A worried, or particularly attentive, mother might report more symptoms of both mental and somatic nature (see the description of statistical analyses). An index of somatic health problems was compiled based on the 3 and 5 year Questionnaires. A score of 1 was given for long-term issues during either the first 18 months or 18–36 months. The list included: impaired hearing, impaired vision, delayed motor development, joint problems, gained too little weight, gained too much weight, asthma, allergy affecting eyes or nose, eczema, food allergy/intolerance, gastrointestinal problems, late or abnormal speech development, and other long-term illness or health problems. Another index was based on short-term illness reported at age 3, including: ear infection, bronchitis, gastric flu/diarrhoea, injury or accident. A third index included health problems reported when the child was 5: asthma, pollen allergy/hay fever, obstruction/wheezing in chest, impaired hearing, delayed motor development or clumsy, delayed or deviant language development, impaired vision, or other health problem. The three indices were analysed separately and summed to one general index. Although several factors could conceivable contribute to correlations between somatic and mental problems, the comparison should help clarify the issue of reporting bias.

All the dependent variables were transformed to z-scores in order to obtain estimates with easily interpretable effect sizes.

Missing values

The outcome variables on children’s mental health were imputed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) EM imputation procedure, where correlated valid data are used to predict values replacing missing values. Each of the sets of items was imputed separately in cases where at least half the items had valid data. Data sets with more than 50 % missing data were discarded. The variables relating to household size and type, animals in the family, as well as somatic health were based on checking or not checking a number of categories. As there were no contra-categories (no place to check for “no”), there were no missing data for these variables. Maternal and paternal period of leave from work after child’s birth were entered in the analyses as categorical variables, and missing data were recoded to separate categories. Missing breastfeeding data were recoded to the lowest category. Results from cross tabulations of the highly correlated variables maternal and paternal educational level, suggested that missing data should be categorized together with the lowest category. Based on similar reasoning, missing data on family income were recoded to the second lowest income group.

Statistical analyses

We estimated the association between the principal predictor and the outcome variables using a variance analysis procedure, SPSS Generalized Linear Models. In our first set of analyses, household size was specified as a factor together with categorical maternal age, categorical maternal breastfeeding duration, and pets in the family. Maternal and paternal duration of leave after birth, maternal and paternal educational levels, and family income were entered as linear covariates. Each of the eight outcome variables (four at age 3, two at age 5 and two at age 8) were consecutively used as dependent variable. In a second series of analyses, household size was replaced with variables specifying numbers of various types of relatives in the family: 1) Spouse of mother (usually the child’s father), 2) parents of mother, 3) parents of spouse (parents in law), 4) children (siblings of the index child), and 5) others. Only the outcome variables with the strongest association with household size in the first set of analyses were included in the second set. In a third set of analyses possible age specific effects of siblings in the household were examined, entering three separate variables for number (0, 1, or 2+) of older siblings, aged 0–5 years, 6–11 years, and 12–18 years at pregnancy, respectively. There were significant interaction effects between household size and sex of the child, and as a final step the analyses were conducted stratified by sex.

The outcome variables were reported by the mothers, and will to some extent be affected by individual judgement. Among other factors, maternal concern and worriedness for her child might affect the outcome scores. Worried mothers would presumably be more likely to judge signs in their children as negative symptoms. If maternal concern about her child varies systematically with the number of children, for instance if earlier experience with being a mother makes her safer and more relaxed, the mothers’ concern might confound a possible association between birth order and mental health. To the extent that this was the case, one would expect the concern to generalize to somatic health problems. That is, inexperienced mothers would also tend to judge their children’s somatic health as worryingly. To examine such a possible confounding, three general indices of “maternally perceived somatic health problems in the child” were generated. The items included were selected based on whether the responses were likely to depend on personal judgement, and thus be affected by maternal concern. The indices were also relevant as a gauge for other factors that may vary systematically with parity. Thus, the analyses were repeated with the somatic indices as outcome in order to indicate the extent to which such a bias may have affected our results on mental health.

Results

Factor loadings for the various outcome variables at 3 and 5 years are shown in Tables 1 and 2. By including only the items which had their strongest loading on a specific factor, we (conservatively) estimated the alpha reliability for that factor. For instance, the estimate for Emotional regulation at age 3 was based on data from the upper nine items in Table 1. We obtained the following alpha reliabilities for the measures at age 3: 0.73 (Emotional regulation), 0.55 (Anxiety), 0.46 (Eating/somatic), and 0.54 (Hyperactivity/concentration). The values for 5 years were: 0.72 (Anxiety) and 0.71 (Difficult temperament). Values for the instruments used at 8 years were 0.45 (Anxiety) and 0.89 (Depression).

As detailed in the Methods section, the study examined correlates between household composition and symptoms of poor mental health in children. The results presented have been adjusted for the following factors considered to be potential confounders: maternal age, maternal and paternal educational level, family income, maternal and paternal period of leave from work after birth, maternal breastfeeding status when child is 18 months, and animals living with the family. Some of the adjustments did lower the estimated effect sizes, but they did not drastically affect the significance of the results.

In Table 4, the exposure is categorized as to the total number of people present in the household. The value “1” implies that the mother is single, while “2” usually means a couple without any previous children. The latter score was used as a reference. Higher numbers reflect a combination of older siblings and adult relatives. The presence of more than parents had a protective effect on the child (a negative score implies a reduced tendency to have mental problems) regardless of the type of outcome. The results are shown as fractions of standard deviations of the outcome variables compared to the reference group (two persons). The association between household size and child mental health was highly significant, p < 10−9 for all outcomes. There was a distinct tendency for larger households to yield more pronounced results, the effect size reaching -0.39 of for household sizes of 6 or more. The outcome included both typical internalizing problems (anxiety and depression) and problems related to externalizing behaviour (hyperactivity, difficult temperament). Being a single mother did not significantly affect the child’s mental health.

Table 4 Adjusted mean scores (M) with confidence intervals (CI) of mental health related problems by household size

Further analyses were performed in order to elucidate the nature of the observed effect, focusing on the outcome variables showing the strongest effect in Table 4.

The associations between mental health and types of relatives present in the household were examined. Table 5 shows specific effects of the presence of various types of relatives, each included as separate predictors in the multivariate analysis. The results demonstrate that the effect of family size was primarily driven by the presence of siblings. There were no indications that the presence of additional adults improved child behaviour, except for a just-significant protective effect of spouse (usually father of the child) on difficult temperament. The only other significant effect was an increased tendency of difficult temperament in five year old children in the presence of “others” (not children, parents or grandparents).

Table 5 Adjusted mean scores (M) with confidence intervals (CI) of mental health related problems by types of relatives in the household

The above results prompted the investigation of whether the age of siblings mattered. It should be noted that the questionnaires were filled in prior to the birth of the child being examined, thus the actual age of siblings would be higher during the period of exposure. Moreover, some of the children would eventually obtain younger siblings, of which there is no available information. As shown in Table 6, the results were consistent with the finding that the more siblings the better; but the best scores were obtained with siblings not too different in age. Again the effect was observed regardless of the way mental health was evaluated.

Table 6 Adjusted mean scores (M) with confidence intervals (CI) of mental health related problems by age category of siblings

Another question was whether the child’s sex made a difference. We tested “sex x household size” interaction effects by adding interaction terms to the initial analyses (the results from which were shown in Table 4). The interaction effect reached significance (p < .01) for four outcome variables. New analyses of these outcomes were stratified by sex, as displayed in Table 7. The results show somewhat stronger effects of household size for girls than for boys.

Table 7 Adjusted mean scores (M) with confidence intervals (CI) of mental health related problems by household size, stratified by sex

There was no consistent effect of breastfeeding across the various outcomes. A significant positive association for one outcome variable is consistent with a selection effect, where children with emotional difficulties tend to be weaned later than emotionally stable children. There was a consistent but weak trend of protective effect of long maternal leave after birth, reaching significance in three of the outcome variables. There was no consistent effect of paternal leave. There were significant effects of the presence of animals, but pointing in both directions, and with trivial effect sizes.

An analysis using reported somatic problems as outcome found no appreciable effects of having older siblings (Table 8). Out of 24 estimates, five reached significance, but only at p < .05. Four of these were positive, suggesting a slight negative effect on health. This is in the opposite direction of what was expected based on the hypothesis of a negative relationship between maternal concern and number of earlier born children.

Table 8 Adjusted mean scores (M) with confidence intervals (CI) of somatic health problems by age category of siblings

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to identify possible causes of mental problems. The choice of parameters to be examined was based on an evolutionary perspective of the human brain. The strategy implies looking for mismatches, in the form of differences between present way of life and the presumed way humans are “genetically designed” to live. Some of the mismatches, referred to as discords, may help explain the prevalence of mental problems [2, 3].

It is likely that the Stone Age tribes had more close affiliates for the child to interact with on a continuous basis, compared to what is typically the case in industrialized societies. Although kindergartens offer company, this is only for a limited period of the day, and the kids are not expected to be as closely knit as those brought up in the same family or tribe. As pointed out elsewhere [26], caretaking of infants by siblings (or additional adults) is typical for tribal people. According to the author, the point is reflected in improved life prospective for infants with older siblings. The question is whether this mismatch also qualifies as a discord; that is, does it affect the mental health of children (and thus potentially adults) in industrialized societies?

The present results suggest so. Having older siblings correlated with improved scores on mental outcome for all age groups probed (3, 5 and 8 years), regardless of how the outcome was measured (Tables 4). It is important to emphasise that the figures presented were corrected for obvious confounders such as socioeconomic status, education, and age of mothers. Although the results were highly significant (p < 10−9), it should be pointed out that the effect only explains a small part of the variation. Perhaps somewhat surprisingly, having a single mother did not appear to be a disadvantage compared to having two parents without older siblings. It should be noted that single mothers in Norway have better conditions than those in many other countries, in terms of governmental support and lack of social stigma.

The observed symptoms are known to predict poor adult mental health [18, 19], but the present results do not tell whether the reported effect will persist. The MoBa project continues, so the answer to that question will hopefully be available in the future.

One possible explanation for the effect is based on how the human brain is designed to be moulded by the environment – particularly in infants. Functions that are frequently activated tend to “expand” and become stronger. Thus, if fear or low mood is often activated during infancy, the results may be excessive activity of these functions later in life, which in the present vocabulary corresponds to problems related to respectively anxiety and depression. As reasoned elsewhere [3], conditions that cause fear in infants include less proximity of care persons and other close affiliates. Older siblings would be expected to supplement parents in terms of offering the child an environment that induces the feeling of safety and companionship. The data were not informative as to whether younger siblings would offer a similar protective effect, although it seems fair to hypothesis that any siblings may do. However, older siblings may be more valuable than having younger siblings, as the latter would presumably add less to the perceived safety. In the EEA, children would presumably grow up with not only siblings, but agemates from other families as well. The total number of children in a group was presumably was most likely considerably larger than what is found in the average family of today.

Parental investment theory offers another interesting angle on the results. The theory suggests a possible conflict between parents and offspring, in that while the parents’ genes are best served by a large number of progeny, the genes of the individual infant are best served by few siblings – in that the latter implies more parental attention and resources [27]. Whether there is a conflict depends, however, on the circumstances [28]. The theory predicts more sibling conflict in families with many children, which is indicated by a report finding that the amount of sibling aggression correlates with family size [29]. However, as to mental health, rivalry could either promote internalizing and externalising behaviour, or build resilience. Moreover, the aggression would typically be sporadic and relatively benign; thus in sum, the effect of interacting with siblings might be positive despite of occasional quarrels. That is, as long as there are ample resources to care for all the children, which is the case in affluent societies such as Norway. The present results support the above contention.

The results could also be described as a correlate between birth order and internalizing/externalizing behaviour. There are several previous reports on birth order effects, but not much in terms of effect on mental health. The more solid observations imply a modest effect on intelligence in that first born Norwegian [30] and Swedish [31] men obtain higher scores. This effect may relate to the older child taking responsibility for younger siblings, and consequently becomes more ambitious or conscientious. The observation does not conflict with the present findings.

Theoretically, one might expect that the presence of adults would be as important as the presence of siblings. According to the data (Table 5), they are not. Additional adults are relatively rare in Norwegian households. Their presence may correlate with family problems that we could not adjust for, and they may be less present in the child’s immediate surroundings compared to older siblings. Moreover, it was interesting to note that the main effect was observed with siblings only slightly older than the child being investigated (Table 6), suggesting that the optimal situation is to have play mates. From an evolutionary viewpoint, it should be mentioned that long term cooperation relies primarily on age mates, thus social affiliations should be tuned toward those of roughly the same age.

Animals may substitute for humans by being companions. Previous reports suggest they may have a positive effect on mental health [32, 33]. Questionnaires 4 and 5 asked the mother whether there were pets in the household as a dichotomous variable. In the present data, we found no appreciable effects of pets (data not shown).

Limitations

We have interpreted the present results within the theoretical framework of evolutionary adaptation; that is, having few close affiliates is a discord in the sense that it contributes to mental problems by triggering brain functions related to anxiety, loneliness, or lack of social comfort. We cannot, however, exclude alternative explanations. For example, parents who decide to have more than one or two children may be fonder of children and thus offer better child care; or those whose first infant(s) was emotionally stable are more likely to opt for additional offspring. It is also conceivable that mothers (and fathers) of small families have a higher tendency toward anxiety or related mental issues, and pass these traits on either by genetic inheritance or by their way of handling infants. Furthermore, it is unclear whether the effect requires the presence of older siblings, as opposed to infants younger than the index child.

The validity of our outcome measures may also be questioned. Some of the internal consistency reliability estimates are low, but that may be because short version instruments like SCARED, give the best validity and measurement prediction if they sample different types of symptoms, all being criteria of a group of disorders. SCARED was initially generated as a multidimensional anxiety measure, in such a case internal consistency coefficients underestimate the reliability. To the extent that some of the low alpha values really reflect measurement error, this has attenuated the effect estimates of the presence of siblings, meaning that the real effects are even stronger than shown by our results. Also the validity of some of our measures is undocumented and rests on the actual content of the single items (“face validity”). The results are similar for all eight outcomes, however, and the risk that all of them are very poor measures of mental health is minimal.

A major limitation is that all the outcome measures depend on maternal judgement of the child behaviour. Beyond causing imperfect validity and reliability, which is already addressed, we were afraid maternal report would systematically bias the result because the mothers might tend to judge their first child different from later ones. However, there was no appreciable bias as to the mothers’ judgement of somatic health (Table 8).

Although the above caveats are relevant, they seem unlikely to alone explain the observed effects.

Conclusions

According to the present study, living in a family with older siblings – who offer an opportunity for play, comfort and security – protects against developing internalizing and externalizing behavioural problems. The effect is distinct and highly significant. In a world suffering from overpopulation, it is not obvious how the observation should be incorporated in governmental advice.

Abbreviations

CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; EAS, Emotionality, Activity and Shyness Temperament Questionnaire; EEA, Environment of Evolutionary Adaptation; MoBa, Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study; SCARED, Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders; SMFQ, Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; SPSS, Statistical Package for the Social Sciences

References

  1. Crawford C, Krebs D. Foundations of evolutionary psychology. New York: Psychology Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Grinde B. Can the concept of discords help us find the causes of mental diseases? Med Hypotheses. 2009;73(1):106–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Grinde B. The biology of happiness. Dordrecht: Springer; 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. Saw SM. A synopsis of the prevalence rates and environmental risk factors for myopia. Clin Exp Optom. 2003;86(5):289–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Dolgin E. The myopia boom. Nature. 2015;519(7543):276–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Grinde B. An approach to the prevention of anxiety-related disorders based on evolutionary medicine. Prev Med. 2005;40(6):904–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Magnus P, Irgens LM, Haug K, Nystad W, Skjaerven R, Stoltenberg C, et al. Cohort profile: the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(5):1146–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Layard R. Happiness - Lessons from a new science. London: Penguin; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Delhey J, Dragolov G. Happier together. Social cohesion and subjective well-being in Europe. Int J Psychol. 2015. doi:10.1002/ijop.12149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rodriguez-Laso A, Zunzunegui MV, Otero A. The effect of social relationships on survival in elderly residents of a Southern European community: a cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2007;7:19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Winefield HR, Winefield AH, Tiggemann M. Social support and psychological well-being in young adults: the multi-dimensional support scale. J Pers Assess. 1992;58(1):198–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ozer EJ, Fernald LC, Roberts SC. Anxiety symptoms in rural Mexican adolescents: a social-ecological analysis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(12):1014–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Patil RN, Nagaonkar SN, Shah NB, Bhat TS. A Cross-sectional Study of Common Psychiatric Morbidity in Children Aged 5 to 14 Years in an Urban Slum. J Family Med Prim Care. 2013;2(2):164–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H. The relative importance of child, family, school and neighbourhood correlates of childhood psychiatric disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2004;39(6):487–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Riordan DV, Morris C, Hattie J, Stark C. Family size and perinatal circumstances, as mental health risk factors in a Scottish birth cohort. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(6):975–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hesketh T, Ding QJ, Jenkins R. Suicide ideation in Chinese adolescents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2002;37(5):230–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, Price A, Wake M. Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(11):1166–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, Oberklaid F. Longitudinal predictors of behavioural adjustment in pre-adolescent children. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35(3):297–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bosquet M, Egeland B. The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18(2):517–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Nilsen RM, Vollset SE, Gjessing HK, Skjaerven R, Melve KK, Schreuder P, et al. Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2009;23(6):597–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Buss AH, Plomin R. Temperament : early developing personality traits. Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates; 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, Cully M, Balach L, Kaufman J, et al. The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):545–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiappetta L, Bridge J, Monga S, Baugher M. Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(10):1230–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A, Winder F, Silver D. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1995;5(4):237–49.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Seymour S. Multiple caretaking of infants and young children: An area in critical need of a feminist psychological anthropology. Ethos. 2004;32(4):538–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Trivers R. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers; 1972. p. 136–79.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kolliker M, Boos S, Wong JWY, Rollin L, Stucki D, Raveh S, et al. Parent-offspring conflict and the genetic trade-offs shaping parental investment. Nat Commun. 2015;6:6850. doi:10.1038/Ncomms7850.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Tippett N, Wolke D. Aggression Between Siblings: Associations With the Home Environment and Peer Bullying. Aggress Behav. 2015;41(1):14–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kristensen P, Gravseth HM, Bjerkedal T. Educational attainment of Norwegian men: influence of parental and early individual characteristics. J Biosoc Sci. 2009;41(6):799–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Barclay KJ. A within-family analysis of birth order and intelligence using population conscription data on Swedish men. Intelligence. 2015;49:134–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Barker SB, Wolen AR. The Benefits of Human-Companion Animal Interaction: A Review. J Vet Med Educ. 2008;35(4):487–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Polheber JP, Matchock RL. The presence of a dog attenuates cortisol and heart rate in the Trier Social Stress Test compared to human friends. J Behav Med. 2014;37(5):860–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

Funding

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and the Ministry of Education and Research, NIH/NIEHS (contract no N01-ES-75558), NIH/NINDS (grant no.1 UO1 NS 047537-01 and grant no.2 UO1 NS 047537-06A1).

Availability of data and materials

English versions of the questionnaires used are available at: http://www.fhi.no/eway/default.aspx?pid=240&trg=MainContent_6894&Main_6664=6894:0:25,7372:1:0:0:::0:0&MainContent_6894=6706:0:25,7375:1:0:0:::0:0 . Anyone can apply for access to data at a cost.

Authors’ contributions

BG was primarily responsible for study design and drafting the manuscript. KT performed the data analyses and contributed to the interpretation and writing process. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

A written informed consent was obtained from participating mothers, including consent on behalf of their infants, as well as a licence from the Norwegian Data Inspectorate (see [7] for further details). The present study was approved by the Regional (REK sør-øst) Committee for Medical Research Ethics.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bjørn Grinde.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grinde, B., Tambs, K. Effect of household size on mental problems in children: results from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort study. BMC Psychol 4, 31 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-016-0136-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-016-0136-1

Keywords