Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T18:41:47.325Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

ECT: III: Enduring Cognitive Defecits?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

D. Weeks
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, (Royal Edinburgh Hospital), Morningside Park, Edinburgh EH10 5HF
C. P. L. Freeman
Affiliation:
Royal Victoria Hospital, Comely Bank, Edinburgh EH4 2DN
R. E. Kendell
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, (Royal Edinburgh Hospital), Morningside Park, Edinburgh EH10 5HF

Summary

Cognitive function was compared in carefully matched groups of ECT and non-ECT treated depressives and in matched normal volunteer controls on admission, at 4 months and at 7 months. ECT caused little impairment at 4 months and no impairment at 7 months on a comprehensive cognitive test battery. Severity of depression had a marked effect on cognitive function. Within the ECT group bilateral ECT caused more impairment than unilateral ECT one week after a course but 3 months later the differences had disappeared. They were equally antidepressant.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bendefeldt, F., Miller, L. L. & Ludwig, A. M. (1976) Cognitive performance in conversion hysteria. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(10), 1250–4.Google Scholar
Bidder, T. G., Strain, J. J. & Brunschwig, L. (1970) Bilateral and unilateral ECT: follow-up study and critique. American Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 737–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, H. W., Russell, R. W. & Patton, R. A. (1949) Duration of decrements in learning and retention following electroshock convulsions in the white rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 42/2, 87106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braun, H. W., Barnes, H. W. & Patton, R. A. (1957) Effects of electroshock convulsions upon the learning performance of monkeys: IV. Discrimination—reversal learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 641–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadhurst, P. L., Stone, C. P. & Lawrence, D. H. (1952) The effects of epanutin and electroconvulsive shock on the maze performance of rats. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 8593.Google Scholar
Brower, D. & Oppenheim, S. (1951) The effects of electroshock therapy on mental functions as revealed by psychological tests. Journal of General Psychology, 45, 171–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, W. L. & Simpson, A. D. (1956) Electroconvulsive shock and the learning of visual patterns. Journal of General Psychology, 89, 219–20.Google Scholar
Brown, W. L. & Wilbanks, W. A. (1952) The effect of different periods of electroconvulsive shock on spatial learning. Journal of General Psychology, 81/1, 2944.Google Scholar
Brown, W. L. & De La Garza, C. O. (1953) The effect of electroconvulsive shock on the anticipatory responses of the rat in a semi-linear maze. Journal of General Psychology, 82, 1120.Google Scholar
Byrne, D. G. (1976) Choice reaction times in depressive states. British Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, 15, 149–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carney, M. W. P., Roth, M. & Garside, R. F. (1965) The diagnosis of depressive syndromes and the prediction of ECT response. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 659–74.Google Scholar
Cronholm, B. & Molander, L. (1964) Memory disturbances after ECT. V. Conditions one month after a series of treatments. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 40/2, 212–16.Google Scholar
Crown, S. & Crisp, A. H. (1966) A short clinical diagnostic self-rating scale for psychoneurotic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 917–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vietti, T. L. & Bucy, C. E. (1975) Recovery of memory after reminder; evidence for two forms of retrieval deficit induced by ECS. Physiological Psychology, 3, 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham-White, J., Merrick, M. & Herbison, J. J. M. (1969) Williams Scale for the Measurement of Memory: Test reliability and validity in a psychiatric population. British Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, 8, 141–51.Google Scholar
Halliday, A. M., Davison, K., Browne, M. W. & Kreeger, L. C. (1968) A comparison of the effects on depression and memory of bilateral ECT and unilateral ECT to the dominant and non dominant hemispheres. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 9971012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, M. (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 5662.Google Scholar
Hemsi, L. K., Whitehead, A. & Post, F. (1968) Cognitive functioning and cerebral arousal in elderly depressives and dements. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 12, 145–56.Google Scholar
Heshe, J., Roder, E. & Theilgaard, A. (1978) Unilateral and Bilateral ECT. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Supp. No. 275.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M. W. & Stone, C. P. (1947) The disorganizing effects of electroconvulsive shock on a light discrimination habit in albino rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 40, 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, B. (1978) The effects of unilateral and bilateral ECT on verbal and visual spatial memory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 413.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendrick, D. C. & Post, F. (1967) Differences in cognitive status between healthy, psychiatrically ill and diffusely brain-damaged elderly subjects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 7581.Google Scholar
Korin, H., Fink, M. & Kwalwasser, S. (1956) Relation of changes in memory and learning to improvement in electroshock. Confinia Neurologica, 16, 8896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, N. P., Steinert, A. R. & Frost, I. (1958) Unilateral electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of Mental Science, 104, 221–7.Google Scholar
McGinnies, E. & Schlosberg, H. (1945) The effects of electroshock convulsions on double alternation lever-pressing in the white rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 361–73.Google Scholar
Meyer, V. (1959) Cognitive changes following temporal lobectomy for relief of temporal lobe epilepsy. A.M.A. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 81, 299309.Google Scholar
Miller, E. (1970) The effect of ECT on memory and learning. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 43, 5762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphree, O. D. & Peters, J. E. (1956) The effect of electroconvulsions, insulin comas and certain chemical agents on fixations in the rat. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 124/1, 7883.Google Scholar
Newcombe, F. (1969) Missile Wounds of the Brain. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nielson, H. C. (1968) Evidence that electroconvulsive shock alters memory retrieval rather than memory consolidation. Experimental Neurology, 20, 320.Google Scholar
Porteus, S. D. (1965) Porteus Maze Tests: Fifty Years Application. Palo Alto, California: Pacific Books.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, G. (1970) Aspects of Disordered Perception. D. Phil Thesis: Oxford University.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1958) Advanced Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1962) Guide to using the Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Ray, W. S. (1960) An Introduction to Experimental Design. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
Russell, R. W. (1949) Effects of electroshock convulsions on learning and retention in the rat as functions of the difficulty of the task. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 42, 137–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, K. W. & Stother, C. R. (1968) A cross sequential study of age changes in cognitive behaviour. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 671–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, P. S. (1943) The effect of electro-shock convulsions on the acquisition of a simple running response in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 36, 61–5.Google Scholar
Siegel, P. S., McGinnies, E. M. & Box, J. C. (1949) The runway performance of rats subjected to ECS following nembutal anaesthesia. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 42, 417–21.Google Scholar
Snaith, R. P., Ahmed, S. N., Mehta, S. & Hamilton, M. (1971) Assessment of the severity of primary depressive illness—Wakefield self-assessment depression inventory. Psychological Medicine, 1, 143–9.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. & Chace, P. W. (1975) Memory functions six to nine months after electroconvulsive therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 1557–64.Google Scholar
Stern, J. A. (1956) The effect of a series of electroconvulsive seizures on some measures of activity in the male albino rat. Journal of General Psychology, 82, 141–51.Google Scholar
Stone, C. P. (1946a) Effects of electroconvulsive shocks on daily activity of albino rats in revolving drums. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 61, 150–1.Google Scholar
Stone, C. P. (1946b) Characteristic losses and gains on the Wechsler Memory Scale as applied on psychotic patients before, during, and after ECT. American Psychologist, 1, 245.Google Scholar
Turek, I. S. & Block, B. (1974) Memory changes with ECT in depression. British Journal of Clinical Practice, 28, 94–5.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1945) A standardized memory scale for clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 8795.Google Scholar
Weeks, D. J. (1979) Do chronic cigarette smokers forget people's names? British Medical Journal, ii, 1627 Google Scholar
Wilcox, K. W. (1954) Intellectual functioning as related to electro-convulsive therapy. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Williams, L. N. (1959) The effect of ECS on response flexibility and retention. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Washington.Google Scholar
Zinkin, S. & Miller, A. J. (1967) Recovery of memory after amnesia induced by electroconvulsive shock. Science, 155, 102–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.