Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Stress within a Restricted Time Window Selectively Affects the Persistence of Long-Term Memory

  • Chang Yang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Chang Yang, Jian-Feng Liu, Bai-Sheng Chai

    Affiliation Affiliated Hospital and School of Pharmacy of Guiyang Medical University, Guiyang, China

  • Jian-Feng Liu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Chang Yang, Jian-Feng Liu, Bai-Sheng Chai

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Bai-Sheng Chai ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Chang Yang, Jian-Feng Liu, Bai-Sheng Chai

    Affiliation Affiliated Hospital and School of Pharmacy of Guiyang Medical University, Guiyang, China

  • Qin Fang,

    Affiliation Affiliated Hospital and School of Pharmacy of Guiyang Medical University, Guiyang, China

  • Ning Chai,

    Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang, China, Hebei Brain Ageing and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang, China

  • Li-Yan Zhao,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Yan-Xue Xue,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Yi-Xiao Luo,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Min Jian,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Ying Han,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Hai-Shui Shi,

    Affiliation Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Basic Medical College, Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang, China

  • Lin Lu,

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Ping Wu ,

    wuping@bjmu.edu.cn (PW); jswang_yg@yahoo.com (JSW)

    Affiliation National Institute on Drug Dependence, Peking University, Beijing, China

  • Ji-Shi Wang

    wuping@bjmu.edu.cn (PW); jswang_yg@yahoo.com (JSW)

    Affiliation Affiliated Hospital and School of Pharmacy of Guiyang Medical University, Guiyang, China

Abstract

The effects of stress on emotional memory are distinct and depend on the stages of memory. Memory undergoes consolidation and reconsolidation after acquisition and retrieval, respectively. Stress facilitates the consolidation but disrupts the reconsolidation of emotional memory. Previous research on the effects of stress on memory have focused on long-term memory (LTM) formation (tested 24 h later), but the effects of stress on the persistence of LTM (tested at least 1 week later) are unclear. Recent findings indicated that the persistence of LTM requires late-phase protein synthesis in the dorsal hippocampus. The present study investigated the effect of stress (i.e., cold water stress) during the late phase after the acquisition and retrieval of contextual fear memory in rats. We found that stress and corticosterone administration during the late phase (12 h) after acquisition, referred to as late consolidation, selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM, whereas stress during the late phase (12 h) after retrieval, referred to as late reconsolidation, selectively disrupted the restabilized persistence of LTM. Moreover, the effects of stress on the persistence of LTM were blocked by the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone, which was administered before stress, suggesting that the glucocorticoid system is involved in the effects of stress on the persistence of LTM. We conclude that stress within a restricted time window after acquisition or retrieval selectively affects the persistence of LTM and depends on the glucocorticoid system.

Introduction

Newly learned information goes through several stages to form long-term memory (LTM) [1][4]. In the classical contextual fear conditioning model, a contextual conditioned stimulus (CS) is associated with a fearful unconditioned stimulus (US) [5][7]. After conditioning, the CS-US association memory is labile and undergoes the consolidation process [1]. Consolidation occurs immediately after conditioning but persists for several hours, days, or even months after training, after which memory is stably stored in the brain [8][10]. Consolidated memory reenters an unstable state after retrieval (i.e., exposure to the CS or US), regardless of the timing of the exposure or age of the memory, which requires the reconsolidation process to be stably stored again [7], [11][16]. Memory within the consolidation and reconsolidation time windows can be modified, updated, or disrupted using pharmacological or behavioral manipulations [11], [17][22].

Recently, a late consolidation process was proposed, suggesting that memory after learning within a delayed time window was also labile [23][26]. After learning, a delayed protein synthesis process is critical for the persistence but not formation of LTM [23], [24]. Several types of molecules have been shown to participate in the late consolidation process, and the modulation of their activity can either destroy or enhance the persistence of LTM [24], [25], [27], [28]. Previous research identified a system consolidation phenomenon to explain the persistence of LTM, suggesting that a memory that was recently stored in the hippocampus will ultimately transfer to the neocortex to form a remote memory [4]. Late consolidation was suggested to function as a bridge between cellular and system consolidation [28].

Memory consolidation and reconsolidation can be modulated by environment stimuli, such as stress. Stress distinctly affects memory consolidation and reconsolidation, which has been demonstrated in both humans and animals [29][36]. When stress was administered within the consolidation stage after learning, subsequent memory performance was enhanced [32][35]. But when stress was administered within the reconsolidation stage after retrieval, the original memory was disrupted [30], [31], [37]. The differential effects of stress on different memory stages can be explained by the different timing of the actions of stress hormones, especially norepinephrine and corticosteroids [29], [30], [37][40]. Moreover, although many signaling pathways that are engaged in consolidation play a role in reconsolidation, a distinct mechanism has also been proposed [12] that might be another explanation for the differential effects of stress on these two similar memory processes. Even if the effects of stress on memory consolidation and reconsolidation were clearly identified in previous studies, such studies focused only on a short time window after learning or retrieval. The effects of stress within the delayed time window after learning and retrieval are still unclear [41].

In the present study, we determined the effect of stress within a delayed time window after learning and retrieval on subsequent memory performance. First, we tested the effect of the cold swim stress and corticosterone administration at different time points after contextual fear memory acquisition on memory performance. Second, we tested the effect of cold swim stress and corticosterone administration at different time points after contextual fear memory retrieval on subsequent memory performance. Third, we investigated whether the effects of stress at different time points after contextual fear memory acquisition and retrieval depend on corticosterone synthesis.

Results

Stress and corticosterone administration during the late phase after fear conditioning selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM

In Experiment 1, we first tested the effect of stress at different time points (1, 12, and 24 h) after fear conditioning on LTM. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to analyze the time spent freezing in rats 2 and 7 days after fear conditioning, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Stress (No stress, stress at 1 h, stress at 12 h, and stress at 24 h) as the between-subjects factor. The analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,31 = 35.590, p<0.05) and Stress (F3,31 = 8.975, p<0.05) and a significant Test day × Stress interaction (F3,31 = 3.122, p<0.05). The post hoc analysis showed that stress administered 1 h after fear conditioning enhanced the time spent freezing in the tests on days 2 and 7 (both p<0.05), but stress administered 12 h after fear conditioning only enhanced the time spent freezing in the test on day 7 (p<0.05) but not day 2 (p>0.05; Fig. 1B).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Stress and corticosterone administered 12 h after fear conditioning selectively enhanced the persistence but not formation of long-term memory.

(A) Rats were exposed to cold water stress or administered corticosterone 1, 12, or 24 h after weak contextual fear conditioning (CFC) and then tested for long-term memory (LTM) on day 2 and day 7. (B) Stress exposure 1 h after CFC enhanced LTM formation tested on day 2, whereas stress exposure 12 h after CFC selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM tested on day 7 but did not affect LTM formation tested on day 2. (C) Corticosterone administration 1 h after CFC enhanced LTM formation tested on day 2, whereas corticosterone administration 12 h after CFC selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM tested on day 7 but did not affect LTM formation tested on day 2. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM (n = 6–9 per group). *p<0.05, compared with Control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059075.g001

We then tested the effect of the stress hormone corticosterone at different time points (1, 12, and 24 h) after fear conditioning on LTM. A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to analyze the time spent freezing 2 days and 7 days after fear conditioning, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Corticosterone (No corticosterone, corticosterone at 1 h, corticosterone at 12 h, and corticosterone at 24 h) as the between-subjects factor. The analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,20 = 40.376, p<0.05) and Corticosterone (F3,31 = 8.482, p<0.05) and a significant Test day × Corticosterone interaction (F3,31 = 3.720, p<0.05). The post hoc analysis showed that corticosterone administered 1 h after fear conditioning enhanced the time spent freezing in the tests on days 2 and 7 (both p<0.05), but corticosterone administered 12 h after fear conditioning only enhanced the time spent freezing in the test on day 7 (p<0.05), leaving the time spent freezing on day 2 intact (p>0.05; Fig. 1C). These results indicate that both stress and corticosterone enhanced the formation of LTM when administered 1 h after fear conditioning but selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM when administered 12 h after fear conditioning. The enhancement of the formation or persistence of LTM by stress might be mediated by the stress hormone corticosterone.

Metyrapone blocked the enhancement of LTM formation and persistence induced by stress after fear conditioning

In Experiment 2, we tested whether a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor can block the effect of stress after fear conditioning on LTM. The rats were pretreated with metyrapone 40 min before stress exposure. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Metyrapone (0 and 40 mg/kg) and Stress (no stress, stress at 1 h, and stress at 12 h) as the between-subjects factors, was used to analyze the time spent freezing in rats that were exposed to stress 1 or 12 h after fear conditioning, respectively. For stress exposure 1 h after fear conditioning, the analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,20 = 44.039, p<0.05), Stress (F1,20 = 33.427, p<0.05), and Metyrapone (F1,20 = 29.103, p<0.05) and significant Stress × Metyrapone (F1,20 = 14.313, p<0.05) and Test day × Stress × Metyrapone (F1,20 = 4.423, p<0.05) interactions. The post hoc analysis showed that metyrapone blocked the enhancement of LTM formation induced by stress 1 h after fear conditioning (p<0.05; Fig. 2B). For stress exposure 12 h after fear conditioning, the analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,20 = 40.013, p<0.05), Stress (F1,20 = 6.854, p<0.05), and Metyrapone (F1,20 = 7.998, p<0.05) and significant Stress × Metyrapone (F1,20 = 4.693, p<0.05) and Test day × Stress × Metyrapone (F1,20 = 4.724, p<0.05) interactions. The post hoc analysis showed that metyrapone blocked the enhancement of LTM persistence induced by stress 12 h after fear conditioning (p<0.05; Fig. 2C). These results indicate that the improvement of LTM formation and persistence induced by stress after fear conditioning required corticosterone synthesis.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Metyrapone blocked the enhancement of long-term memory formation or persistence induced by stress after fear conditioning.

(A) Rats were pretreated with the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone 40 min before exposure to cold water stress 1 or 12 h after weak contextual fear conditioning (CFC) and then tested for long-term memory (LTM) on day 2 and day 7. (B) Metyrapone blocked the effects of stress 1 h after CFC on LTM formation on day 2 and reduced the persistence of LTM tested on day 7. (C) Metyrapone blocked the effects of stress 12 h after CFC on the persistence of LTM tested on day 7, leaving LTM formation tested on day 2 intact. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM (n = 6 per group). *p<0.05, compared with Vehicle-No stress group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059075.g002

Stress and corticosterone during the late phase after memory reactivation selectively disrupted the persistence of LTM

In Experiment 3, we tested the effect of stress at different time points after fear memory retrieval on LTM. Memory retrieval was not significantly different among groups (both p>0.05; Fig. 3B, C). A repeated-measures ANOVA, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Stress (No stress, stress at 1 h, stress at 12 h, and stress at 24 h) as the between-subjects factor, was conducted to analyze the time spent freezing on days 2 and 7 after fear memory retrieval. The analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,32 = 13.961, p<0.05) and Stress (F3,32 = 23.435, p<0.05) and a significant Test × Stress interaction (F3,32 = 2.962, p<0.05). The post hoc analysis showed that stress given 1 h after fear memory retrieval decreased the time spent freezing on days 2 and 7 (both p<0.05), whereas stress given 12 h after fear memory retrieval only decreased freezing time on day 7 (p<0.05) but not day 2 (p>0.05; Fig. 3B).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Stress and corticosterone administered 12 h after fear memory retrieval selectively impaired the persistence but not formation of long-term memory.

(A) Rats were exposed to cold water stress or administered corticosterone 1, 12, or 24 h after retrieval of the memory of strong contextual fear conditioning (CFC) and then tested for long-term memory (LTM) on day 2 and day 7. (B) Stress exposure 1 h after CFC retrieval enhanced LTM reformation tested on day 2, whereas stress exposure 12 h after CFC retrieval selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM tested on day 7 but did not affect LTM reformation tested on day 2. (C) Corticosterone administration 1 h after CFC retrieval enhanced LTM formation tested on day 2, whereas corticosterone administration 12 h after CFC retrieval selectively enhanced the persistence of LTM tested on day 7 but did not affect LTM reformation tested on day 2. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM (n = 6–8 per group). *p<0.05, compared with Control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059075.g003

We then tested the effect of the stress hormone corticosterone at different time points (1, 12, and 24 h) after fear memory retrieval on LTM. A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to analyze the time spent freezing 2 days and 7 days after fear memory retrieval, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Corticosterone (No corticosterone, corticosterone at 1 h, corticosterone at 12 h, and corticosterone at 24 h) as the between-subjects factor. The analysis showed significant effects of Test day (F1,20 = 8.465, p<0.05) and Corticosterone (F3,20 = 24.017, p<0.05) and a significant Test × Corticosterone interaction (F3,20 = 3.196, p<0.05). The post hoc analysis showed that corticosterone administered 1 h after fear memory retrieval decreased the time spent freezing on days 2 and 7 (both p<0.05), but corticosterone administered 12 h after fear memory retrieval only decreased the time spent freezing on day 7 (p<0.05), leaving the time spent freezing on day 2 intact (p>0.05; Fig. 3C). These results indicate that both stress and corticosterone disrupted the restabilization of LTM when administered 1 h after fear memory retrieval but selectively disrupted the persistence of LTM when administered 12 h after fear conditioning. The disruption of the restabilization and persistence of LTM by stress might be mediated by the stress hormone corticosterone.

Metyrapone blocked the impairment of LTM formation and persistence induced by stress after fear retrieval

In Experiment 4, we tested whether a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor can block the effect of stress after fear memory retrieval on LTM. The rats were pretreated with metyrapone 40 min before stress exposure. Memory retrieval was not significantly different among groups (both p>0.05; Fig. 4B, C). A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with Test day (day 2 and day 7) as the within-subjects factor and Metyrapone (0 and 40 mg/kg) and Stress (no stress, stress at 1 h, and stress at 12 h) as the between-subjects factors, was used to analyze the time spent freezing in rats that were exposed to stress 1 or 12 h after fear memory retrieval, respectively. For stress exposure 1 h after fear memory retrieval, the analysis showed significant effects of Stress (F1,20 = 21.834, p<0.05) and Metyrapone (F1,20 = 15.999, p<0.05) and a significant Stress × Metyrapone interaction (F1,20 = 16.542, p<0.05) but no significant effect of Test day (F1,20 = 3.098, p>0.05). The post hoc analysis showed that metyrapone blocked the disruption of LTM reformation induced by stress administered 1 h after fear memory retrieval (p<0.05; Fig. 4B). For stress exposure 12 h after fear memory retrieval, the two-way repeated-measures ANOVA showed significant effects of Test day (F1,20 = 7.820, p<0.05), Stress (F1,20 = 4.685, p<0.05), and Metyrapone (F1,20 = 4.457, p<0.05) and significant Stress X Metyrapone (F1,20 = 7.720, p<0.05) and Test day X Stress X Metyrapone (F1,20 = 4.906, p<0.05) interactions. The post hoc analysis showed that metyrapone blocked the disruption of LTM persistence induced by stress administered 12 h after fear memory retrieval (p<0.05; Fig. 4C). These results indicate that the disruption of LTM restabilization and persistence induced by stress after fear memory reactivation requires corticosterone synthesis.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Metyrapone blocked the impairment of long-term memory formation and persistence induced by stress after fear memory retrieval.

(A) Rats were pretreated with the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone 40 min before exposure to cold water stress 1 or 12 h after retrieval of the memory of strong contextual fear conditioning (CFC) and then tested for long-term memory (LTM) on day 2 and day 7. (B) Metyrapone blocked the effects of stress 12 h after CFC retrieval on LTM formation on day 2 and decreased the persistence of LTM tested on day 7. (C) Metyrapone blocked the effects of stress 12 h after CFC retrieval on the persistence of LTM tested on day 7 but did not affect LTM formation tested on day 2. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM (n = 6 per group). *p<0.05, compared with Vehicle-No stress group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059075.g004

Discussion

In the present study, we demonstrated the differential effects of stress at different time points after learning or memory retrieval on memory performance. Delayed stress or corticosterone administration after fear learning selectively promoted the persistence of LTM. Delayed stress or corticosterone administration after fear memory reactivation selectively destroyed the persistence of LTM. The effects of stress during the late phase after fear learning or fear memory reactivation both required corticosterone synthesis.

The effects of stress on memory are complex. Many factors, such as the emotional valence of the memory, interval between learning and stress, and predominant hormone, might affect the effects of stress on memory [29], [42], [43]. The effects induced by stress have been shown to depend on the memory stage when the subject is exposed to stress [44]. Stress exposure before learning or acquisition can disrupt or enhance memory [45][48]. Memory retention performance can be reduced if stress exposure occurs shortly before the memory test [49][51]. Studies of the effects of stress have focused on the consolidation of normal or emotional memories and found an inverse U-shaped dose-response relationship with consolidation [52][54]. Only a few studies have investigated the role of stress after memory retrieval, suggesting effects of stress on the reconsolidation or extinction process [30], [37], [55]. Animal and human studies have both indicated that stress exposure during the consolidation or reconsolidation process can affect LTM performance, mostly tested 24 h after learning or retrieval, which was consistent with the present results. Interestingly, our study found that stress during the late phase after fear conditioning or fear retrieval selectively affected the persistence of LTM.

The persistence of LTM has been shown to depend on late-phase protein synthesis-dependent processes in the dorsal hippocampus, which was referred to as late consolidation because of its consolidation-like features [23], [24], [27], [28], [41], [56]. Our results showed that stress during the late consolidation time window enhanced the persistence of LTM, without affecting performance in the LTM retention test the next day, which was considered to be the formation of LTM. Consolidated memory can be reactivated to undergo reconsolidation process [11], [13][15]. Reconsolidation engages several molecular signaling pathways that are the same as those that are activated during the consolidation process, but an independent mechanism has also been identified [11], [12]. Similar to consolidation, reconsolidation also requires new protein synthesis and has a limited time window of approximately several hours after memory reactivation. Our previous results showed that the restabilized LTM after retrieval also requires a late-phase protein synthesis process after memory reactivation (unpublished data). In the present study, we demonstrated that stress during late reconsolidation disrupted the persistence of restabilized LTM after retrieval but left LTM reformation intact.

Previous research concluded that stress distinctly affects emotional memory consolidation and reconsolidation, improving consolidation but disrupting reconsolidation [29], [30], [35], [44], [57][59], which is consistent with our results. The present results showed that stress during the late phase of consolidation and reconsolidation also has different effects, facilitating late consolidation but disrupting late reconsolidation. Below are possible explanations for the distinct effects of stress on late consolidation and late reconsolidation.

Molecular signaling pathway

Since the discovery of the late consolidation phenomenon, many molecular signaling pathways in the dorsal hippocampus have been shown to be engaged in this process [23], [56]. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BNDF), mitogen-activated protein kinases (MAPKs), ERK1/2, zif268, and Fos were significantly increased during the late consolidation window and shown to play critical roles in late consolidation [24], [27], [28], [56]. A molecular mechanism was proposed that was different from the one proposed for consolidation and involved BDNF but not zif268, whereas reconsolidation involved zif268 but not BNDF [12]. These independent mechanisms do not appear to fit for late consolidation and late reconsolidation because BDNF and zif268 were both shown to be crucial for late consolidation [23], [27]. To date, the mechanism that underlies late reconsolidation is unclear. Future studies need to elucidate the molecular mechanisms that underlie the difference between late consolidation and late reconsolidation and the distinct effects of stress on these two memory processes.

Stress hormones

Glucocorticoids and norepinephrine are the two main stress hormones that underlie the effects of stress on memory. Corticosterone can mimic the effects of stress on both consolidation and reconsolidation, which can be blocked by a glucocorticoid receptor inhibitor or β-adrenoceptor blocker [29], [40], [60]. A recent study showed that the β-adrenergic antagonist propranolol blocked the effect of stress on late consolidation in an inhibitory avoidance task, indicating a critical role for the adrenergic system in late consolidation [41]. In the present study, we found that corticosterone administration during late consolidation or late reconsolidation exerted stress-like effects on the persistence of LTM. Moreover, the effects of stress on late consolidation and late reconsolidation were blocked by a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor. Our results indicate that the effects of stress on late consolidation and late reconsolidation depend on the glucocorticoid system. However, the involvement of concurrent glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity in the effects of stress on memory does not appear to explain the differential effects of stress on late consolidation and reconsolidation.

Stress-targeted brain areas

The main brain areas that underlie the effects of stress on memory include the basolateral amygdala (BLA), dorsal hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex (PFC) [40], [58], [61][63]. Stress-induced glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activation in the BLA can directly affect memory in the BLA or modulate memory processes in the dorsal hippocampus and PFC [40], [62]. Previous studies indicated that the dorsal hippocampus was the main brain area recruited during late consolidation [23], [56], and stress-induced BLA stress system activation may indirectly regulate the memory process in the dorsal hippocampus. Stress can also directly affect the late consolidation process in the dorsal hippocampus. Direct infusion of norepinephrine into the dorsal hippocampus during late consolidation enhanced the persistence of LTM [28]. Although no direct evidence has demonstrated the role of the hippocampal glucocorticoid system in late consolidation, several studies have identified a critical role for hippocampal glucocorticoids in other memory stages [64], [65]. The present results cannot elucidate the precise role of the stress systems in the BLA and dorsal hippocampus during late consolidation or late reconsolidation on the persistence of LTM. Future studies should reveal such mechanisms.

In summary, we found that stress during the late phase after fear learning or retrieval selectively affected the persistence of LTM. Stress and corticosterone administration improved the late consolidation of memory but disrupted late reconsolidation. The effects of stress on late consolidation and late reconsolidation were dependent on corticosterone synthesis. These results indicate that the corticosterone system participates in the persistence of LTM after both acquisition and retrieval.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Male Sprague Dawley rats, weighing 260–280 g upon arrival, were obtained from the Laboratory Animal Center, Peking University Health Science Center. They were housed in groups of five in a temperature (23±2°C)- and humidity (50±5%)-controlled animal facility and were maintained on a 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. All of the experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by Biomedical Ethics Committee of Peking University of animal use and protection.

Drugs

Corticosterone and the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO, USA). Corticosterone was dissolved in saline that contained 2% ethanol, and metyrapone was dissolved in a vehicle that contained 40% polyethylene glycol and 60% saline to reach an appropriate concentration. All of the drugs were freshly prepared before the experiments. The doses of the drugs (10 mg/kg corticosterone, i.p.; 40 mg/kg metyrapone, i.p.) were based on our previous report [30]. Metyrapone was administered 40 min before stress [30].

Contextual fear memory

Contextual fear conditioning was conducted in four identical isolated shock chambers (Shanghai Jiliang Software Technology Co. Ltd, Shanghai, China) that were used in our previous study [66]. The contextual fear conditioning procedure was modified from previous studies [67]. The rats were handled for 3 days before conditioning. On the day of the experiments, they were placed into the conditioning chamber and allowed to explore the chamber for 2 min, after which they received an electric footshock (1 s). The shock intensity was 0.4 mA for weak training and 1.0 mA for strong training. The 2 min/1 s procedure was repeated a total of three times, and the rats were allowed to explore the conditioning chamber for an additional 1 min. After removing the rat from the chamber, the chamber was cleaned with 75% alcohol to eliminate any residual odor. To reactivate memory (i.e., retrieval), the rats were exposed to the conditioning chamber for 3 min without shock. Memory was tested by exposing the rats to the conditioning chamber for 5 min without shock. All of the experimental sessions were video-recorded for offline analysis. Freezing behavior was defined as the lack of all movement, with the exception of respiration.

Cold swim stress

The cold swim stress protocol was adapted from our previous study [30]. Briefly, the rats were placed for 5 min into a 35 cm diameter ×40 cm height plastic cylinder that was filled to a depth of 30 cm with ice-cold water [30], [68]. The rats were then removed, dried, and returned to their home cage.

Statistical analysis

The data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The statistical analyses were performed using repeat-measures ANOVA for all of the experiments. The within-subjects and between-subjects factors are stated in the Results section. Post hoc analyses of significant effects in the ANOVAs were performed using the Least Significant Difference test. Values of p<0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Special Experiment

Experiment 1: Effect of stress and corticosterone at different time points after fear conditioning on LTM.

We first tested whether stress or corticosterone administration at different time points after conditioning would distinctly affect LTM. Weak training (0.4 mA, 1 s, three shocks) was performed in this experiment. Three groups of rats were separately exposed to forced swim stress 1, 12, or 24 h after contextual fear conditioning. A group of rats without stress was used as a control. To test whether corticosterone affects memory similarly to forced swim stress, three groups of rats were separately administered corticosterone 1, 12, or 24 h after contextual fear conditioning. An additional group of rats that did not receive these treatments were used as a control. Memory was tested 2 and 7 days after fear conditioning (n = 6–9 per group).

Experiment 2: Effect of corticosterone synthesis inhibitor on enhancement of LTM induced by stress after fear conditioning.

We tested whether a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor would affect the post-conditioning stress induced enhancement of LTM formation or persistence. Weak training (0.4 mA, 1 s, three shocks) was performed in this experiment. Eight groups of rats were separately administered the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone 40 min before exposure to forced swim stress, which was given 1 or 12 h after fear conditioning. Memory was tested 2 and 7 days after memory reactivation (n = 6 per group).

Experiment 3: Effect of stress and corticosterone at different time points after fear memory reactivation.

We then tested whether stress or corticosterone at different time points after memory reactivation distinctly affect recent memory. Strong training (1.0 mA, 1 s, three shocks) was performed in this experiment. Two days after fear conditioning, three groups of rats were separately exposed to forced swim stress 1, 12, or 24 h after memory reactivation. A group of rats that were not exposed to stress after memory reactivation was used as a control. To test whether corticosterone affects reactivated memory similarly to forced swim stress, three groups of rats were separately administered corticosterone 1, 12, or 24 h after contextual fear reactivation. An additional group of rats that did not receive these treatments was used as a control. Memory was tested 2 and 7 days after memory reactivation (n = 6–8 per group).

Experiment 4: Effect of corticosterone synthesis inhibitor on disruption of LTM induced by stress after fear memory reactivation.

We tested whether a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor would affect the post-retrieval stress induced disruption of LTM restablization or persistence. Strong training (1.0 mA, 1 s, three shocks) was performed in this experiment. Eight groups of rats were separately administered the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone 40 min before exposure to forced swim stress, which was given 1 or 12 h after fear memory reactivation. Memory was tested 2 and 7 days after memory reactivation (n = 6 per group).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CY JFL QF YXX LL JSW. Performed the experiments: CY JFL BSC NC YXL MJ YH HSS. Analyzed the data: CY JFL LYZ YXX YXL HSS LL PW. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LL PW. Wrote the paper: CY JFL LL PW.

References

  1. 1. McGaugh JL (2000) Memory – a century of consolidation. Science 287: 248–251.
  2. 2. Schafe GE, LeDoux JE (2000) Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J Neurosci 20: RC96.
  3. 3. Maren S (2001) Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 897–931.
  4. 4. Debiec J, LeDoux JE, Nader K (2002) Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron 36: 527–538.
  5. 5. Rudy JW, Huff NC, Matus-Amat P (2004) Understanding contextual fear conditioning: insights from a two-process model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28: 675–685.
  6. 6. Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS (2001) Hippocampus and contextual fear conditioning: recent controversies and advances. Hippocampus 11: 8–17.
  7. 7. Maren S, Holt W (2000) The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning. Behav Brain Res 110: 97–108.
  8. 8. Izquierdo I, Medina JH (1998) On brain lesions, the milkman and Sigmunda. Trends Neurosci 21: 423–426.
  9. 9. McGaugh JL (1966) Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science 153: 1351–1358.
  10. 10. Izquierdo I, Bevilaqua LR, Rossato JI, Bonini JS, Medina JH, et al. (2006) Different molecular cascades in different sites of the brain control memory consolidation. Trends Neurosci 29: 496–505.
  11. 11. McKenzie S, Eichenbaum H (2011) Consolidation and reconsolidation: two lives of memories? Neuron 71: 224–233.
  12. 12. Lee JL, Everitt BJ, Thomas KL (2004) Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Science 304: 839–843.
  13. 13. Lee JL (2010) Memory reconsolidation mediates the updating of hippocampal memory content. Front Behav Neurosci 4: 168.
  14. 14. Dudai Y (2006) Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16: 174–178.
  15. 15. Nader K, Hardt O (2009) A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 224–234.
  16. 16. Debiec J, Diaz-Mataix L, Bush DE, Doyere V, Ledoux JE (2010) The amygdala encodes specific sensory features of an aversive reinforcer. Nat Neurosci 13: 536–537.
  17. 17. Flavell CR, Barber DJ, Lee JL (2011) Behavioural memory reconsolidation of food and fear memories. Nat Commun 2: 504.
  18. 18. Soeter M, Kindt M (2011) Disrupting reconsolidation: pharmacological and behavioral manipulations. Learn Mem 18: 357–366.
  19. 19. Costanzi M, Cannas S, Saraulli D, Rossi-Arnaud C, Cestari V (2011) Extinction after retrieval: effects on the associative and nonassociative components of remote contextual fear memory. Learn Mem 18: 508–518.
  20. 20. Monfils MH, Cowansage KK, Klann E, LeDoux JE (2009) Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science 324: 951–955.
  21. 21. Xue YX, Luo YX, Wu P, Shi HS, Xue LF, et al. (2012) A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science 336: 241–245.
  22. 22. Kindt M, Soeter M (2013) Reconsolidation in a human fear conditioning study: A test of extinction as updating mechanism. Biol Psychol 92: 43–50.
  23. 23. Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Igaz LM, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, et al. (2007) Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis- and BDNF- dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron 53: 261–277.
  24. 24. Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Katche C, Slipczuk L, Rossato JI, et al. (2008) BDNF is essential to promote persistence of long-term memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 2711–2716.
  25. 25. Bekinschtein P, Katche C, Slipczuk L, Gonzalez C, Dorman G, et al. (2010) Persistence of long-term memory storage: new insights into its molecular signatures in the hippocampus and related structures. Neurotox Res 18: 377–385.
  26. 26. Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Cammarota M (2009) Dopamine controls persistence of long-term memory storage. Science 325: 1017–1020.
  27. 27. Katche C, Goldin A, Gonzalez C, Bekinschtein P, Medina JH (2012) Maintenance of long-term memory storage is dependent on late posttraining Egr-1 expression. Neurobiol Learn Mem 98: 220–227.
  28. 28. Katche C, Bekinschtein P, Slipczuk L, Goldin A, Izquierdo IA, et al. (2010) Delayed wave of c-Fos expression in the dorsal hippocampus involved specifically in persistence of long-term memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 349–354.
  29. 29. Schwabe L, Joels M, Roozendaal B, Wolf OT, Oitzl MS (2012) Stress effects on memory: an update and integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 1740–1749.
  30. 30. Wang XY, Zhao M, Ghitza UE, Li YQ, Lu L (2008) Stress impairs reconsolidation of drug memory via glucocorticoid receptors in the basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci 28: 5602–5610.
  31. 31. Zhao LY, Zhang XL, Shi J, Epstein DH, Lu L (2009) Psychosocial stress after reactivation of drug-related memory impairs later recall in abstinent heroin addicts. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 203: 599–608.
  32. 32. Roozendaal B, Nguyen BT, Power AE, McGaugh JL (1999) Basolateral amygdala noradrenergic influence enables enhancement of memory consolidation induced by hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 11642–11647.
  33. 33. Roozendaal B, Okuda S, Van der Zee EA, McGaugh JL (2006) Glucocorticoid enhancement of memory requires arousal-induced noradrenergic activation in the basolateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 6741–6746.
  34. 34. Abercrombie HC, Speck NS, Monticelli RM (2006) Endogenous cortisol elevations are related to memory facilitation only in individuals who are emotionally aroused. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31: 187–196.
  35. 35. Cahill L, Gorski L, Le K (2003) Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learn Mem 10: 270–274.
  36. 36. Smeets T, Otgaar H, Candel I, Wolf OT (2008) True or false? Memory is differentially affected by stress-induced cortisol elevations and sympathetic activity at consolidation and retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33: 1378–1386.
  37. 37. Cai WH, Blundell J, Han J, Greene RW, Powell CM (2006) Postreactivation glucocorticoids impair recall of established fear memory. J Neurosci 26: 9560–9566.
  38. 38. Roozendaal B, McReynolds JR, Van der Zee EA, Lee S, McGaugh JL, et al. (2009) Glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation depend on functional interactions between the medial prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci 29: 14299–14308.
  39. 39. Miranda MI, Quirarte GL, Rodriguez-Garcia G, McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B (2008) Glucocorticoids enhance taste aversion memory via actions in the insular cortex and basolateral amygdala. Learn Mem 15: 468–476.
  40. 40. Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S (2009) Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 423–433.
  41. 41. Parfitt GM, Barbosa AK, Campos RC, Koth AP, Barros DM (2012) Moderate stress enhances memory persistence: are adrenergic mechanisms involved? Behav Neurosci 126: 729–734.
  42. 42. Diamond DM, Campbell AM, Park CR, Halonen J, Zoladz PR (2007) The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: a synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Neural Plast 2007: 60803.
  43. 43. Payne JD, Jackson ED, Hoscheidt S, Ryan L, Jacobs WJ, et al. (2007) Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Learn Mem 14: 861–868.
  44. 44. Roozendaal B (2002) Stress and memory: opposing effects of glucocorticoids on memory consolidation and memory retrieval. Neurobiol Learn Mem 78: 578–595.
  45. 45. Schwabe L, Bohringer A, Chatterjee M, Schachinger H (2008) Effects of pre-learning stress on memory for neutral, positive and negative words: Different roles of cortisol and autonomic arousal. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90: 44–53.
  46. 46. Smeets T, Giesbrecht T, Jelicic M, Merckelbach H (2007) Context-dependent enhancement of declarative memory performance following acute psychosocial stress. Biol Psychol 76: 116–123.
  47. 47. Diamond DM, Campbell AM, Park CR, Woodson JC, Conrad CD, et al. (2006) Influence of predator stress on the consolidation versus retrieval of long-term spatial memory and hippocampal spinogenesis. Hippocampus 16: 571–576.
  48. 48. Elzinga BM, Bakker A, Bremner JD (2005) Stress-induced cortisol elevations are associated with impaired delayed, but not immediate recall. Psychiatry Res 134: 211–223.
  49. 49. de Quervain DJ, Aerni A, Roozendaal B (2007) Preventive effect of beta-adrenoceptor blockade on glucocorticoid-induced memory retrieval deficits. Am J Psychiatry 164: 967–969.
  50. 50. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL (1998) Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature 394: 787–790.
  51. 51. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, McGaugh JL, Hock C (2000) Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neurosci 3: 313–314.
  52. 52. Sandi C, Loscertales M, Guaza C (1997) Experience-dependent facilitating effect of corticosterone on spatial memory formation in the water maze. Eur J Neurosci 9: 637–642.
  53. 53. Roozendaal B, Bohus B, McGaugh JL (1996) Dose-dependent suppression of adrenocortical activity with metyrapone: effects on emotion and memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21: 681–693.
  54. 54. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS (1997) The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 24: 1–27.
  55. 55. Maroun M, Akirav I (2008) Arousal and stress effects on consolidation and reconsolidation of recognition memory. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 394–405.
  56. 56. Medina JH, Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Izquierdo I (2008) Do memories consolidate to persist or do they persist to consolidate? Behav Brain Res 192: 61–69.
  57. 57. de Kloet ER, Joels M, Holsboer F (2005) Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 463–475.
  58. 58. Henckens MJ, Hermans EJ, Pu Z, Joels M, Fernandez G (2009) Stressed memories: how acute stress affects memory formation in humans. J Neurosci 29: 10111–10119.
  59. 59. Joels M, Fernandez G, Roozendaal B (2011) Stress and emotional memory: a matter of timing. Trends Cogn Sci 15: 280–288.
  60. 60. Roozendaal B, Barsegyan A, Lee S (2008) Adrenal stress hormones, amygdala activation, and memory for emotionally arousing experiences. Prog Brain Res 167: 79–97.
  61. 61. Lupien SJ, Lepage M (2001) Stress, memory, and the hippocampus: can't live with it, can't live without it. Behav Brain Res 127: 137–158.
  62. 62. Kim JJ, Koo JW, Lee HJ, Han JS (2005) Amygdalar inactivation blocks stress-induced impairments in hippocampal long-term potentiation and spatial memory. J Neurosci 25: 1532–1539.
  63. 63. Roozendaal B, McReynolds JR, McGaugh JL (2004) The basolateral amygdala interacts with the medial prefrontal cortex in regulating glucocorticoid effects on working memory impairment. J Neurosci 24: 1385–1392.
  64. 64. Oitzl MS, Fluttert M, de Kloet ER (1998) Acute blockade of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors facilitates spatial learning in rats. Brain Res 797: 159–162.
  65. 65. Roozendaal B, Griffith QK, Buranday J, De Quervain DJ, McGaugh JL (2003) The hippocampus mediates glucocorticoid-induced impairment of spatial memory retrieval: dependence on the basolateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100: 1328–1333.
  66. 66. Si J, Yang J, Xue L, Yang C, Luo Y, et al. (2012) Activation of NF-kappaB in basolateral amygdala is required for memory reconsolidation in auditory fear conditioning. PLoS One 7: e43973.
  67. 67. Lubin FD, Sweatt JD (2007) The IkappaB kinase regulates chromatin structure during reconsolidation of conditioned fear memories. Neuron 55: 942–957.
  68. 68. Korneyev A, Binder L, Bernardis J (1995) Rapid reversible phosphorylation of rat brain tau proteins in response to cold water stress. Neurosci Lett 191: 19–22.