Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

P300 Aberration in First-Episode Schizophrenia Patients: A Meta-Analysis

  • Yao-qin Qiu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Yao-qin Qiu, Yun-xiang Tang

    Y-qQ and Y-xT are joint first authors on this work.

    Affiliations School of Nursing, Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, P.R. China, Department of Statistics, Faculty of Medical Services, Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, P.R. China

  • Yun-xiang Tang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Yao-qin Qiu, Yun-xiang Tang

    Y-qQ and Y-xT are joint first authors on this work.

    Affiliation Department of Medical Psychology, Faculty of psychology and mental healthy, Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, P.R. China

  • Raymond C. K. Chan,

    Affiliation Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, P.R. China

  • Xin-yang Sun,

    Affiliation Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, P.R. China

  • Jia He

    hejia63@yahoo.com

    Affiliation Department of Statistics, Faculty of Medical Services, Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, P.R. China

Abstract

Background

Decreased P300 amplitude is one of the most consistent findings in patients with schizophrenia. However, whether prolonged P300 latency occurs in patients with schizophrenia, especially first-episode schizophrenia (FES) patients, remains controversial.

Methods

A meta-analyses of P300 aberration in FES patients and healthy control(HC) group was conducted. The meta-regression analysis was performed using a random effects model. The pooled standardized effect size (PSES) was calculated as the division of the difference between the means of the two groups by the common standard deviation.

Results

A total of 569 FES patients and 747 HCs were included in this meta- analysis. P300 amplitude was significantly reduced (PSES = −0.83, 95% CI: −1.02–0.65, P = 0.00001) and P300 latency was delayed significantly in FES patients (PSES = −0.48, 95% CI: 0.14–0.81, P = 0.005). The meta-regression analysis showed that task difficulty was a source of heterogeneity.

Conclusions

The meta-analysis confirms that disrupted information processing is found in FES patients, which is manifested by smaller P300 amplitude and delayed P300 latency.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a serious mental disorder manifesting positive psychotic symptoms such as hallucination and delusion, negative symptoms such as poor motivation and anhedonia, as well as neurocognitive deficits in more than 75% of the patients [1]. Neurocognitive abnormalities may represent a trait marker for schizophrenia [2].

Event-related potentials (ERPs) are cerebral responses associated with various psychological events and are objective parameters reflecting cognitive functions [3][4]. The P300 ERP component is a late cognitive-related ERP component associated with attention and memory processes [4]. The P300 event-related brain potential is an index of endogenous cognitive processes that include directed attention and the contextual updating of working memory [5]. The P300 amplitude is viewed as a measure of central nervous system activity that occurs when stimulus memory representations are generated. It is proportional to the amount of attentional resources devoted to a given task [6]. The P300 latency is considered to be a measure of stimulus classification speed. It is unrelated to response selection processes and independent of behavioral response time [7].

P300 deficits have been reported in many mental disorders, such as Alzheimer disease [8], bipolar disorder [9], depression [10], personality disorder [11], and schizophrenia. Previously, a number of studies reported aberrations in both the P300 wave amplitude and the P300 latency in schizophrenia patients. Jeon and Polich reported P300 abnormality of schizophrenia in a meta-analysis research, containing literature published from 1966 to 1999 [12]. After that, Bramon et al. conducted two meta-analyses of P300, respectively in schizophrenia patients [13] and in patients' relatives [14]. Decreased P300 amplitude and prolonged latency were reported in schizophrenia patients and their relatives. Yet subjects included in former meta-analyses were medicated schizophrenia patients, whose P300 might have been affected by antipsychotics. Whether similar abnormalities are also present in medication free first-episode schizophrenia (FES) patients remains unclear. Until now, the effect of medication on P300 amplitude and latency is still a debated issue. Although some research suggested that antipsychotics, especially the second generation agents, could partially improve the performance of P300 amplitude and latency [15], Jeon and Polich did not find any correlation between medication and P300 amplitude effect size in his meta-analysis [12]. Another factor that impacted P300 latency effect size was the disease duration [12]. Delayed P300 latency found in chronic schizophrenia patient may be inconformity with that in FES patients. Until now, there has been no consistent conclusion on P300 latency changes in patients with FES. Although some researchers reported significantly delayed P300 latency in patients [16][17], others reported inconsistent results [18][20].

This paper aims to conduct a meta-analysis of the studies on P300 in FES patients and to verify the P300 changes in the relatively early stage of schizophrenia.

Methods

1 Study identification

The meta-analysis was designed and reported in line with the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) statement (www.prisma-statement.org). The studies included in this meta-analysis were identified by computerized searching in PubMed, Medline, Cochrane Library, CBM, Chinese sci-tech periodical full-text database, and Full text Chinese academic conference paper database. We searched the databases for papers published between the year 1990 and 2012, using the combined key words “P300” or “P3” or “P3a” or “P3b” and “first episode schizophrenia” or “FES”.

Studies were included if:

  • They were published as research papers. Conference abstracts were not included.
  • They included human subjects.
  • They compared FES patients with a healthy control (HC) group.
  • They used the diagnostic criteria of DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th) or ICD-10 (WHO International Classification of Diseases).
  • They reported both mean and standard deviation of amplitude and/or latency of the P300 waveforms. If means and variances were not reported, we requested them from the authors.
  • They used a standard auditory or visual oddball task for the P300.
  • They reported data at electrodes of Pz and Cz (only those sites were included in the meta-analysis) [13].

Studies were excluded if:

  • They were published as abstracts or reviews.
  • They had a small sample size (less than seven).
  • There were insufficient data on the mean and standard deviation of the P300 amplitude and latency, or the data could not be obtained after two attempted contacts with the authors.
  • They used a non-standard auditory P300 paradigm (eg. Speech sounds [21][24]).

Two investigators worked independently and in duplicate, scanned all abstracts and obtained full text reports of the records. After obtaining full text reports of the candidate studies (either in full peer-reviewed publication or press article), the same reviewers independently assessed the eligibility of the full text papers. According to the above criteria, 17 articles were included in this meta-analysis (The working flow chart of the study identification is shown in Fig. 1). Among them, 13 were in English and four in Chinese. Two reviewers conducted data extraction independently using a standardized pre-piloted form. They collected information of the authors, study sites, diagnostic standards, clinical symptom gradings, stimulus parameters, and results. Some of the studies reported P300 measures on both Cz and Pz locations where the P300 values are usually the largest, so that separated effect sizes could be calculated for each of the two electrodes to maximize analysis sensitivity in the same manner. We entered the data into an electronic database such that the duplicate entries of each study could exist. When the two entries did not match, we reached a consensus through discussions.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study identification in the meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097794.g001

2 Data analysis

In this study, we report our results in standardized effect size (Z scores). Based on the data reported in each study, the Z scores are calculated as the division of the difference between the means of P300 amplitude/latency of the two groups by the pooled standard deviation. The direction of the effect size is negative if the P300 amplitude of FES patients is smaller or if the P300 latency of FES patients is shorter than that of healthy controls. If a study includes one control group and several patient groups, the patient groups are pooled together and their weighted mean is used for analysis as indicated below:Where,

N = number of pooled number, N1, N2 = Number of study A and B, M = Weighted mean, M1, M2 = Mean of study A and B, S = Weighted standard deviation, S1, S2 = Standard deviation of study A and B.

Most studies reported P300 data at both PZ and CZ electrodes. However, some studies reported data only at one of the two locations. To avoid duplication of analysis and to make use of all available studies, some authors chose the mean data between Pz and Cz for meta- analysis [13]. Considering that the application of the average of amplitude and latency may cause more bias, especially for P300 latency, we entered the larger P300 amplitude and the more delayed P300 latency at PZ and CZ. Therefore in our study, the standardized effect sizes were analyzed with random effects meta-analysis.

The random effects regression was used to investigate whether the variability in effect size among the studies could be explained by the variability in study parameters. The following eight predictors were tested: sample size, sex(percentage of male patients), age(mean of patient group), task difficulty(difference between target and non-target), medication(patients were medicated or not), reaction methods(key pressing or counting of targets), target ratio, and study sites (in Chinese or any other country).

We examined the publication bias with the Begg's adjusted rank correlation test and its corresponding funnel plots. A test of heterogeneity (I2) among the study results was also carried out.

Heterogeneity test(I2), merging effects, subgroup analysis and sensitivity analysis were calculated by Revman 5.0 (http://ims.cochrane.org/revman/download). Publication bias and meta-regression were conducted using STATA 10.0 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

A total of 78 articles were found by searching the databases with the predefined key words. Among them, three used meta-analysis, 37 were published in Chinese, and the rest in English. After a preliminary screening, 44 articles entered the intensive screening stage. A blind literature quality score was given independently by two readers and the comprehensive evaluation was made by the researchers. In the end, 17 studies on P300 amplitude and 16 studies on P300 latency were suitable for our meta-analysis. For easy understanding, the P3b is from here on referred to as ‘P300’. Among the 17 studies on P300 amplitude, 13 were written in English and 4 in Chinese. These included a total of 569 FES patients (355 males and 214 females) and 747 HCs (424 males and 323 females) [16][20], [25][36]. The main results of the meta-analysis are summarized in Table 1. As the data of P300 latency could not be acquired from the authors of Liu's study [36], only 16 studies were included in the meta-analysis of P300 latency, with a total of 506 FES (322 males and 184 females) and 707 HCs (405 males and 302 females). In all these meta-analyses on P300 amplitude and P300 latency, 2 researches (Lee et al. 2010 and Wang Qiang et al. 2004) used visual stimuli while the rest used auditory stimuli.

thumbnail
Table 1. Studies on the P300 wave included in this meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097794.t001

1 P300 amplitude and latency in FES patients

There was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity for both P300 amplitude (τ2 = 0.08, χ2 = 35.56, df = 16, p = 0.003) and P300 latency (τ2 = 0.37, χ2 = 103.96, df = 15, p<0.001), which supports the use of random effects meta-analysis method.

Random effects meta-analysis demonstrated that when compared with healthy controls, FES patients had significantly smaller P300 amplitude with a pooled effect size of −0.83 (95% CI −1.02 to −0.65, P = 0.00001). The P300 latency was delayed significantly when compared with controls with a pooled effect size of 0.48 (95% CI 0.14 to 0.81, P = 0.005) (Fig. 2, 3).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Meta-analysis on the forest map of P300 amplitude of FES group and HC group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097794.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Meta-analysis on the forest map of P300 latency of FES group and HC group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097794.g003

We found no evidence of publication bias either for P300 amplitude (Begg's test: z = 0.04, P = 0.967) or for P300 latency (Begg's test: z = 0.32, P = 0.753).

2 Meta regression

Sex, age, task difficulty, medication, reaction methods, target ratio, and study sites might all influence the P300 waves. Meta-regression analysis showed that task difficulty had a significant influence on the effect sizes of P300 amplitude and P300 latency, which could explain the heterogeneity found in our study. No significant influence of sex, age, medication, reaction methods, target ratio and study sites on heterogeneity.

For P300 amplitude, after “task difficulty” was introduced into the meta-regression model, the heterogeneity (τ2) decreased from 0.08 to 0 (see Table 2, 3). Fifteen studies(2 visual oddball studies, Lee(2010) and Wang(2004), were not included in meta-regression)were then divided into two subgroups: a subgroup (7 studies) with high task difficulty (the difference between target and non-target < = 500 Hz) and a subgroup (8 studies) with low task difficulty (the difference between target and non-target >500 Hz). No heterogeneity was found in the two subgroups (I2 = 0), and a fixed-effect model was introduced in the meta-analysis. The merging effect of the two subgroups was consistent with each other.

For P300 latency, fourteen studies (2 visual oddball studies, Lee(2010) and Wang(2004), were not included in meta-regression.) were also divided into high and low task difficulty subgroups. Only the high task difficulty subgroup (7 studies) showed a significantly delayed P300 latency in FES patients compared with healthy controls (z = 3.23, p = 0.001), but with a large heterogeneity among the studies (I2 = 89%). Compared with HCs, no prolonged P300 latency was found in FES patients in the low task difficulty subgroup(7 studies)(z = 1.10, p = 0.27).

Discussion

Clinically, schizophrenia patients often present attention, verbal memory, and working memory dysfunctions [37]. Previous studies suggested that aberrations of P300 amplitude in schizophrenia patients were indicators of these dysfunctions [38], which might be further related to structural and functional changes in the schizophrenia patient's temporal lobe and parietal lobe [39]. Although most of the previous studies reported a reduced P300 amplitude at Pz/Cz electrode in FES patients [16][20], [25][29], [21][36], some studies reported negative results [30], [40]. A variety of factors, such as disease severity and sample size, might explain this apparently inconsistent result [12]. Our meta-analysis results revealed that Pz and Cz electrode merging effects of significantly decreased P300 amplitude in FES patients were lower than that of normal normal healthy control groups. Given that the P300 is often viewed as an index of cognitive function [41], our results may suggest that cognitive impairment could manifest itself even in the early stage of illness. Some research suggested that P300 stemmed from a wide cerebral cortex network, such as parietal lobe [42], cingulated gyrus, and frontal lobe [43]. The parietal lobe was considered to related with selective attention [42], while cingulated gyrus and frontal lobe were related to working memory [43]. Our findings suggest that the dysfunction of corresponding brain area may take place in FES patients.

P300 amplitude deficits were considered as an index of disturbances in attentional allocation mechanisms [6]. Recently, a series of neuropsychological studies on attention dysfunction in schizophrenia patients have supported this conclusion. Brébion [44] suggested that selective attention disorder in schizophrenia patients might reduce reality-differentiating capacity, leading to hallucinations and delusions. Besides, another study revealed a positive correlation between negative symptoms severity and attention disorder [45]. In addition, some studies also showed that the attention disorder found in schizophrenia patients might exhibit disease specificity to a certain degree [46][47]. Evidence also showed that P300 abnormality occurred not only in schizophrenia patients, but also in their non psychotic relatives [48] and ultra high risk (UHR) subjects [49].

In recent years, a number of longitudinal studies on the change of P300 amplitude in schizophrenia patients during different time periods have emerged [15], [50][51]. Some researchers think that P300 aberrations might serve as a trait marker for schizophrenia [15], while the others think P300 is a state marker for the patients. However, loss to follow-up [52], high cost of follow-up [13], medication [53], and other factors have made these studies less convincing. Our meta-regression tests revealed no significant effect of medication on P300 measures. This suggests that symptomatic treatment does not alter pathophysiology, the determinant of P300 values. Furthermore, this results suggested that P300 abnormalities may more likely be a ‘trait’ rather than a ‘state’ marker of schizophrenia. Prospective longitudinal studies of first episode patients need to be carried out in order to fully establish which neurophysiological abnormalities are early ‘traits’ and whether they change with disease progression.

Some studies have examined the effect of the physical characteristics on P300 amplitude and latency [54][55]. P300 effects of task difficulty was reported by Jeon and Polich in their meta-analysis [12]. In this meta-analysis, we also found that task difficulty had a significant influence on the effect sizes of P300 amplitude and P300 latency. The results of subgroup analysis also confirmed that task difficulty was the main source of heterogeneity. Prolonged P300 latency was only found in the subgroup with small difference between target and non-target stimulus, but not in the subgroup with large stimulus difference. We speculate that, more attention resources would be demanded for small difference P300 test (difficult) than for large difference one (easy). Insufficient resources can be consumed in the difficult test in FES, which may cause smaller P300 amplitude and prolonged P300 latency. When the difference between target and non-target is large, insufficient brain resource was engaged in FES, the same as in the difficult test; while the speed of information processing in schizophrenia patients was not affected. Therefore no significant difference of P300 latency was found between patients and healthy controls.

The observation of a prolonged latency in schizophrenia patients is controversial [56][57]. Our meta-analysis results demonstrated prolonged P300 latency in schizophrenia patients relative to healthy controls, which indicated a decline in patients' brain processing speed of external information. This information processing disorder reflects itself not only in P300 changes, but also in other indicators such as mismatch negativity(MMN) [58] and auditory function [59]. Notably, our meta-regression analysis, subgroup analysis, and sensitivity analysis all indicated substantial heterogeneity in merging the effect of P300 latency change in FES patients, with only one exception. One subgroup(low task difficulty) showed no heterogeneity and no statistical significance of P300 latency between FES patients and healthy controls. Consequently, if we want to get more comprehensive explanation of P300 latency variation in schizophrenia, better designed experiments are needed in future research.

One limitation of this meta-analysis is the possibility of publication bias. There was no evidence of publication bias for P300 amplitudes (P = 0.967) and latency(P = 0.753). However, we included only published studies, while certain non-published studies may have been conducted on this topic. We limited our search to English and Chinese language databases, while qualified studies in other languages could exist. When we were conducting meta-regression, two visual oddball studies (Lee(2010) and Wang(2004)), were excluded. In addition, to avoid duplication of analysis, we entered the larger P300 amplitude and the more delayed P300 latency at Pz and Cz. This method may cause some bias in meta-analysis. Besides, some other factors such as EEG recording methodology and clinical severity may also be the source of heterogeneity; however, no sufficient information can be extracted from previous researches. Therefore, at present, it is not possible to explore the effects of these factors.

Conclusions

It could be concluded from this meta-analysis that FES patients demonstrate obvious aberrations of P300 amplitude and latency. As no significant effect of medication on P300 was found in our meta-regression tests, we infer that these abnormalities may be ‘trait’ markers of schizophrenia. Heterogeneity in subgroups would be greatly reduced when the subgroups are differentiated by task difficulty, and the combined effect of P300 amplitude could be more stable.

Supporting Information

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: Y-qQ JH Y-xT. Analyzed the data: Y-qQ X-yS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JH. Wrote the paper: Y-xT Y-qQ RCC.

References

  1. 1. Zhou YF, Zhao JP (2000) Drug treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and its research progress. Sichuan mental health 13: 217–218.
  2. 2. Zaytseva Y, Korsakova N, Agius M, Gurovich I (2013) Neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia and during the early phases of psychosis: targeting cognitive remediation interventions. Biomed Res Int 2013: 819587.
  3. 3. Tanriverdi F, Yapislar H, Karaca Z (2009) Evaluation of cognitive performance by using P300 auditory event related potentials (ERPs) in patients with growth hormone (GH) deficiency and acromegaly. Growth Horm IGF Res 19: 24–30.
  4. 4. Nehra R, Grover S, Chetri D, Sood A, Das CP (2012) P300 latency and neurocognitive functioning in recently diagnosed human immunodeficiency virus patients. Indian J Psychol Med 34: 376–80.
  5. 5. Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, et al. (2007) Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull 33: 69–94.
  6. 6. Polich J, Herbst KL (2000) P300 as a clinical assay: rationale, evaluation, and findings. Int J Psychophysiol 38: 3–19.
  7. 7. Telles S, Singh N, Puthige R (2013) Changes in P300 following alternate nostril yoga breathing and breath awareness. Biopsychosoc Med 7: 11.
  8. 8. Miranda EC, Pinheiro MM, Pereira LD, Iorio MC (2012) Correlation of the P300 evoked potential in depressive and cognitive aspects of aging. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 78: 83–9.
  9. 9. Bestelmeyer PE (2012) The visual P3a in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: effects of target and distractor stimuli on the P300. Psychiatry Res 197: 140–4.
  10. 10. Liu S, Sun N, Xu Y, Yang C, Ren Y, et al. (2010) Possible association of the GSK3β gene with the anxiety symptoms of major depressive disorder and P300 waveform. Genet Test Mol Biomarkers 16: 1382–9.
  11. 11. Shin YW, Krishnan G, Hetrick WP, Brenner CA, Shekhar A, et al. (2010) Increased temporal variability of auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia and Schizotypal Personality Disorder. Schizophr Res 124: 110–8.
  12. 12. Jeon YW, Polich J (2003) Meta-analysis of P300 and schizophrenia: Patients, paradigms, and practical implications. Psychophysiology 40: 684–701.
  13. 13. Bramon E, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sham P, Murray RM, Frangou S (2004) Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 70: 315–29.
  14. 14. Bramon E, McDonald C, Croft RJ, Landau S, Filbey F, et al. (2005) Is the P300 wave an endophenotype for schizophrenia? A meta-analysis and a family study. Neuroimage 27: 960–8.
  15. 15. Mori K, Morita K, Shoji Y (2012) State and trait markers of emotionally charged visual event-related potentials (P300) in drug-naïve schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 66: 261–9.
  16. 16. Demiralp T, Uçok A, Devrim M, Isoglu-Alkaç U, Tecer A, et al. (2002) N2 and P3 components of event-related potential in first-episode schizophrenic patients: scalp topography, medication, and latency effects. Psychiatry Res 111: 167–179.
  17. 17. Chen B, Wang HX, Zhang MD (2010) P300 characteristics in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and its follow-up study. Shan-Xi Yike Daxue Xuebao 41: 253–256.
  18. 18. Lee SY, Namkoong K, Cho HH, Song DH, An SK (2010) Reduced visual P300 amplitudes in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett 486: 156–160.
  19. 19. Devrim-Uçok M, Keskin-Ergen HY, Uçok A (2006) Novelty P3 and P3b in first-episode schizophrenia and chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 30: 1426–1434.
  20. 20. Brown KJ, Gonsalvez CJ, Harris AW, Williams LM, Gordon E (2002) Target and non-target ERP disturbances in first episode vs. chronic schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 113: 1754–1763.
  21. 21. Kayser J, Tenke CE, Gil R, Bruder GE (2010) ERP generator patterns in schizophrenia during tonal and phonetic oddball tasks: effects of response hand and silent count. Clin EEG Neurosci 41: 184–95.
  22. 22. Kayser J, Bruder GE, Tenke CE, Stuart BK, Amador XF, et al. (2001) Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in schizophrenia for tonal and phonetic oddball tasks. Biol Psychiatry 49: 832–47.
  23. 23. Bruder G, Kayser J, Tenke C, Amador X, Friedman M, et al. (1999) Left temporal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia: event-related potential and behavioral evidence from phonetic and tonal dichotic listening tasks. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56: 267–76.
  24. 24. Bruder GE, Kayser J, Tenke CE, Friedman M, Malaspina D, et al. (2001) Event-related potentials in schizophrenia during tonal and phonetic oddball tasks: relations to diagnostic subtype, symptom features and verbal memory. Biol Psychiatry 50(6): 447–52.
  25. 25. Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Sherwood AR, Fischer IA, Yurgelun-Todd DA, et al. (1998) First-episode schizophrenia psychosis differs from first episode affective psychosis and controls in P300 amplitude over left temporal lobe. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 173–180.
  26. 26. Hirayasu Y, Asato N, Ohta H, Hokama H, Arakaki H, et al. (1998) Abnormalities of auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia prior to treatment. Biol Psychiatry 43: 244–253.
  27. 27. van der Stelt O, Lieberman JA, Belger A (2005) Auditory P300 in high-risk, recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 77: 309–320.
  28. 28. Ozgürdal S, Gudlowski Y, Witthaus H, Kawohl W, Uhl I, et al. (2008) Reduction of auditory event-related P300 amplitude in subjects with at-risk mental state for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 105: 272–278.
  29. 29. Wang J, Tang Y, Li C, Mecklinger A, Xiao Z, et al. (2010) Decreased P300 current source density in drug-naive first episode schizophrenics revealed by high density recording. Int J Psychophysiol 75: 249–257.
  30. 30. Korostenskaja M, Dapsys K, Siurkute A, Maciulis V, Ruksenas O, et al. (2006) Effects of risperidone on auditory information processing in neuroleptic-naïve patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 66: 139–144.
  31. 31. Wang J, Hirayasu Y, Hiramatsu K, Hokama H, Miyazato H, et al. (2003) Increased rate of P300 latency prolongation with age in drug-naive and first episode schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 114: 2029–2035.
  32. 32. Chen XS, Lu YZ, Wang JJ, Wang HX, Zhang MD, et al. (2007) Relationship between event-related potential P300 and first episode schizophrenia. Chin Med J (Engl) 120: 339–341.
  33. 33. Umbricht DS, Bates JA, Lieberman JA, Kane JM, Javitt DC (2006) Electrophysiological indices of automatic and controlled auditory information processing in first-episode, recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 59: 762–772.
  34. 34. Wang Q, Sun XL, Ma XH, Shi JJ, Yao J, et al. (2004) Event-related potentials and molecular genetics in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and their relatives. Zhongguo Shenjing Jingshen Jibing Zazhi 30: 229–230.
  35. 35. Wang CH, Li Y, Su LY, Ma JD, Mu JL (2009) Effects of olanzapine and risperidone on memory function and P300 in patients with first episode schizophrenia. Zhongguo Shenjing Jingshen Jibing Zazhi 35: 481–484.
  36. 36. Liu L, Qin XX, Huang YJ, Bi HX (2007) A correlational study on Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia and event-related potential P300. Di-San Junyi Daxue Xuebao 29: 2286–2288.
  37. 37. Möser C, Krieg JC, Zihl J, Lautenbacher S (2006) Attention and memory deficits in schizophrenia: the role of symptoms of depression. Cogn Behav Neurol 19: 150–6.
  38. 38. Pentaraki A, Stahl D, Bramon E (2010) The relationship between visual memory and the P300 in families with schizophrenia. Ann Gen Psychiatry 9: S123.
  39. 39. Higuchi Y, Sumiyoshi T, Kawasaki Y, Matsui M, Arai H, et al. (2008) Electrophysiological basis for the ability of olanzapine to improve verbal memory and functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia: a LORETA analysis of P300. Schizophr Res 101: 320–330.
  40. 40. Alain C, Cortese F, Bernstein LJ, He Y, Zipursky RB (2001) Auditory feature conjunction in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 49: 179–91.
  41. 41. Park EJ, Han SI, Jeon YW (2010) Auditory and visual P300 reflecting cognitive improvement in patients with schizophrenia with quetiapine: a pilot study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 34: 674–80.
  42. 42. Jung EK, Kang KY, Kim YY (2013) Frontoparietal activity during deceptive responses in the P300-based guilty knowledge test: an sLORETA study. Neuroimage 78: 305–15.
  43. 43. Sabeti M, Moradi E, Katebi S (2011) Analysis of neural sources of p300 event-related potential in normal and schizophrenic participants. Adv Exp Med Biol 696: 589–97.
  44. 44. Brébion G, Smith MJ, Gorman JM, Amador X (1996) Reality monitoring failure in schizophrenia: The role of selective attention. Schizophr Res 22: 173–180.
  45. 45. Gráda CO, Barry S, McGlade N, Behan C, Haq F, et al. (2009) Does the ability to sustain attention underlie symptom severity in schizophrenia? Schizophr Res 107: 319–323.
  46. 46. Bozikas VP, Andreou C, Giannakou M, Tonia T, Anezoulaki D, et al. (2005) Deficits in sustained attention in schizophrenia but not in bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res 78: 225–233.
  47. 47. Fleck DE, Sax KW, Strakowski SM (2001) Reaction time measures of sustained attention differentiate bipolar disorder from schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 52: 251–259.
  48. 48. Bramon E, Dempster E, Frangou S, Shaikh M, Walshe M, et al. (2008) Neuregulin-1 and the P300 waveform–a preliminary association study using a psychosis endophenotype. Schizophr Res 103: 178–85.
  49. 49. van Tricht MJ, Nieman DH, Koelman JH, van der Meer JN, Bour LJ, et al. (2010) Reduced parietal P300 amplitude is associated with an increased risk for a first psychotic episode. Biol Psychiatry 68: 642–8.
  50. 50. Higashima M, Nagasawa T, Kawasaki Y, Oka T, Sakai N, et al. (2003) Auditory P300 amplitude as a state marker for positive symptoms in schizophrenia: cross-sectional and retrospective longitudinal studies. Schizophr Res 59: 147–157.
  51. 51. Mathalon DH, Ford JM, Pfefferbaum A (2000) Trait and state aspects of p300 amplitude reduction in schizophrenia: a retrospective longitudinal study. Biol Psychiatry 47: 434–449.
  52. 52. Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Marsh L, Faustman WO, Harris D, et al. (1999) P300 amplitude is related to clinical state in severely and moderately ill patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 46: 94–101.
  53. 53. Coburn KL, Shillcutt SD, Tucker KA, Estes KM, Brin FB, et al. (1998) P300 delay and attenuation in schizophrenia: reversal by neuroleptic medication. Biol Psychiatry 44: 466–474.
  54. 54. Lindín M, Zurrón M, Díaz F (2005) Stimulus intensity effects on P300 amplitude across repetitions of a standard auditory oddball task. Biol Psychol 69: 375–385.
  55. 55. Bonala B, Boutros NN, Jansen BH (2008) Target probability affects the likelihood that a P300 will be generated in response to a target stimulus, but not its amplitude. Psychophysiology 45: 93–99.
  56. 56. Winterer G, Coppola R, Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (2003) Functional and effective frontotemporal connectivity and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 54: 1181–1192.
  57. 57. Mathalon DH, Ford JM, Rosenbloom M, Pfefferbaum A (2000) P300 reduction and prolongation with illness duration in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 47: 413–427.
  58. 58. Thönnessen H, Zvyagintsev M, Harke KC, Boers F, Dammers J, et al. (2008) Optimized mismatch negativity paradigm reflects deficits in schizophrenia patients. A combined EEG and MEG study. Biol Psychol 77: 205–16.
  59. 59. Näätänen R, Kähkönen S (2009) Central auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia as revealed by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm: A review. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12: 125–135.