Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Same Same but Different. Different Trigeminal Chemoreceptors Share the Same Central Pathway

  • Kathrin Kollndorfer,

    Affiliations Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria, Department of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Ksenia Kowalczyk,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Johannes Frasnelli,

    Affiliations Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition, Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada, Centre de Recherche, Hôpital du Sacre Coeur de Montréal, Montréal, Canada

  • Elisabeth Hoche,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Ewald Unger,

    Affiliation Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Christian A. Mueller,

    Affiliation Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Jacqueline Krajnik,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Siegfried Trattnig,

    Affiliation High Field MR Center, Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Veronika Schöpf

    veronika.schoepf@uni-graz.at

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

Abstract

Intranasal trigeminal sensations are important in everyday life of human beings, as they play a governing role in protecting the airways from harm. Trigeminal sensations arise from the binding of a ligand to various sub-types of transient receptor potential (TRP) channels located on mucosal branches of the trigeminal nerve. Which underlying neural networks are involved in the processing of various trigeminal inputs is still unknown. To target this unresolved question fourteen healthy human subjects were investigated by completing three functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning sessions during which three trigeminal substances, activating varying sub-types of chemoreceptors and evoking different sensations in the nose were presented: CO2, menthol and cinnamaldehyde. We identified similar functional networks responding to all stimuli: an olfactory network, a somatosensory network and an integrative network. The processing pathway of all three stimulants was represented by the same functional networks, although CO2 evokes painful but virtually odorless sensations, and the two other stimulants, menthol and cinnamaldehyde are perceived as mostly non painful with a clear olfactory percept. Therefore, our results suggest a common central processing pathway for trigeminal information regardless of the trigeminal chemoreceptor and sensation type.

Introduction

Chemosensory perception is, in part, the result of the interaction between the olfactory and the trigeminal system. Whereas the olfactory system is responsible for the perception of the quality of odors, the latter conveys sensations such as burning, stinging, pungency, temperature, or pain and thereby serves as an additional sentinel to protect the airways from harm [15].

Intranasal trigeminal stimulation evokes neuronal activation of pain processing areas, including the anterior cingulate cortex, the insula, or the primary somatosensory cortex [68], as well as chemosensory processing regions, such as the orbitofrontal cortex [9]. It has recently been shown that trigeminal perception arises from the interaction of a ligand with specific receptors, similar to the olfactory system. So far, various sub-families of transient receptor potential (TRP) channels have been identified to transmit chemosensory information as well as temperature changes. For example, the TRPV1 receptor responds to heat and to compounds, such as capsaicin or carbon dioxide (CO2); its activation leads to a burning and stinging sensation [10,11]. Another example is the TRPM8 receptor, activated by cool temperatures and compounds such as menthol or eucalyptol; creating a cooling sensation [12,13]. A third example is the TRPA1 receptor, which is stimulated by both cold temperatures and substances such as mustard oil or cinnamaldehyde [14,15]; its activation evokes a burning cold sensation. Interestingly, TRPV1 and TRPA1 receptors are often expressed in the same sensory neurons [16,17], whereas the TRPA1 and the TRPM8 are exclusively expressed in different sensory neurons [17]. This has direct functional consequences, as sensitivity to agonists of different trigeminal receptors is not distributed equally in the population [18]. While past studies have provided an increased comprehension of the periphery of the trigeminal system, we do not yet understand the mechanisms that occur during the central processing of trigeminal perception. We, therefore, aimed to investigate central activation patterns evoked by agonists of three different trigeminal chemoreceptors; specifically, we compared: 1) CO2 (activating TRPV1); 2) menthol (stimulating TRPM8); and 3) cinnamaldehyde (activating TRPA1). We hypothesized that we would observe largely overlapping activation patterns with receptor-specific differences.

Functional imaging data was analyzed conducting group independent component analysis (ICA) for all three scanning sessions to compare the determined processing networks for trigeminal stimulation, as previous studies investigating fMRI analyzing techniques in the context of chemosensory experiments revealed the high susceptibility to movement artifacts [19,20] in hypothesis-driven methods like the General Linear Model (GLM).

Materials and Methods

Stimulus rating

To ensure not only the binding of the substances to various chemoreceptors but also the different sensations evoked by the investigated stimuli, all three odorants were evaluated separately. All stimuli were rated by a subject group of 9 raters (7 female, 2 male; mean age, 27.4 years, SD, 3.3), who did not participate in the fMRI experiment. The evaluation was assessed on a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 to 100. The stimulus rating included the assessment of pleasantness, painfulness and familiarity of the applied substance. Furthermore, the raters were asked to label the stimulus.

Subjects

Nineteen healthy subjects (ten female, nine male) participated in our study. All subjects had normal olfactory function (for details see olfactory performance section) and had no history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, which were known to influence olfactory performance. All subjects did not report any severe head trauma in the past. Five subjects had to be excluded from the data set, as they did not complete all fMRI measurements. Fourteen subjects (seven female, seven male; mean age, 30.1 years; SD, 6.7) completed all measurements. All subjects were acquired by announcements at the Medical University of Vienna. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Vienna. All subjects were informed about the aim of the study and gave their written informed consent prior to inclusion.

Olfactory performance

Psychophysiological measurement of olfactory performance was assessed using the Sniffin’ Sticks test battery (Burghart Instruments, Wedel, Germany). This test battery includes three subtests that assess nasal chemosensory function—detection threshold, odor discrimination and odor identification—using pen-like devices for odor presentation [2123]. The olfactory detection threshold of n-butanol was assessed using a single-staircase, three-alternative, forced-choice procedure. Next, we determined odor discrimination ability using 16 triplets of odorants (two pens contained the same odorant; the third pen contained an odd odorant). The participants’ task was to detect the odd pen (forced choice). The odor identification task is composed of 16 common odors using a multiple-choice answering format with a list of four descriptors for each odor. The scores for the detection threshold can range from 1–16, for the other two subtests a score between 0 and 16 can be achieved. The results of all three subtests were summed to obtain the “TDI-score.” Normosmia, or normal olfactory performance, is characterized by a TDI-score of at least 31 [23].

Chemosensory stimulation

All subjects of the fMRI experiment completed three scanning sessions, one for every stimulus: 1) CO2 (50% v/v); 2) cinnamaldehyde (75% v/v dissolved in 1,2-propanediol; Sigma-Aldrich, Germany); and 3) menthol (2.5g; Sigma-Aldrich, Germany). Trigeminal stimuli were delivered in an event-related design, using a computer-controlled, air-dilution olfactometer compatible with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), which was constructed at the Center for Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering (Medical University of Vienna). The mobile olfactometer with a dimension of 37x30x11cm and a total weight of 2.5kg comprises four separated airways channels for stimulation and provides two options of administering chemosensory stimuli; either by using custom-made odor reservoirs or gaseous stimuli, which can be directly connected to the device. These four airways are compounded of 2mm teflon tubes with an inner diameter of 1.5mm. By actuating a valve, air is diverted through and enters the odor reservoir. Each stimulation air-line is controlled by its own electromagnetic valve, which is realized with a 24V linear solenoid (type: 195227–231, Co. Saia-Burges) and a teflon membrane valve. To control the activation of the normally closed solenoid valve, a free programmable microcontroller (PIC16F690, Co. Microchip) in combination with a power output stage is used to define the stimulation sequence. The stimulation session is adjusted by three parameters: the channel number, the on time (stimulation) and the off time (no stimulation). After programming the microcontroller the olfactometer works with this setup without any additional support by a computer and can be started with a single start button. A second possibility for the isolated activation of the gas line is realized by a simple switch parallel connected to the output stage. This feature is used to test the function of the valves and to adjust the flow-rate (up to 1.5l/min) of the stimulation gas with the needle valve of the support gas. The flavoured gas output of all stimulation channels are add to a single air-line, which ends with a nose applicator.

Imaging methods

FMRI measurements were performed on a 3 Tesla Trio System (Siemens Medical Solution, Erlangen, Germany) using a 32-channel head coil, and single-shot, gradient-recalled, echo-planar imaging (EPI). Thirty-six slices (2.7mm thickness, 0.5mm gap) were acquired, with a Field of View (FOV) of 210 x 210mm and an echo time (TE)/ repetition time (TR) of 32/2000ms. Slices were aligned parallel to the connection between the anterior and the posterior commissure. To correct for distortion, caused by inhomogeneity of the magnetic field, online distortion correction was performed with point-spread function mapping [24].

Participants were positioned in head first supine position in the scanner. The mobile olfactometer was placed outside the scanner room. A tube connected to the stimulation device was positioned into the vestibulum nasi without discomfort for the subject, in order to prevent nasal obstruction and was then attached to the head coil to avoid shifting of the tube during the measurements.

All subjects who participated in this study completed three scanning sessions with different trigeminal stimuli, each lasting ten minutes. All stimuli were applied monorhinally to the left nostril. Stimuli were presented for 500ms with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 30s, resulting in 20 trigeminal pulses per scanning session. Stimuli were presented in pseudorandomized order to minimize interaction effects between substances. During fMRI measurements, subjects were asked to use a velopharyngeal closure breathing technique [25] and to keep their eyes closed [26] during all three scanning sessions.

Data analysis

FMRI data were preprocessed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/), implemented in MATLAB (Matlab 7.14.0, Release 2012a, Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, MA, USA). Standard preprocessing in SPM contained 1) slice-timing, to correct for the temporal difference between the first and the last slice of the total volume; 2) motion correction to adjust for head movement; 3) spatial normalization, a registration of the individual brain to a standardized template of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) to correct for individual brain shape and size; and 4) spatial smoothing, whereby small blurring kernels were applied to the image, in order to average parts of neighboring voxels and thereby softening hard edges.

ICA is a data-driven analysis method, which is used to separate a multivariate signal into independent components. Previous research has shown that ICA is especially suitable to analyze chemosensory experiments [19,20], as chemosensory paradigms are generally performed using an event-related design.

Group ICA was performed for all three scanning sessions conjointly using the Group ICA for fMRI Toolbox (GIFT; http://icatb.sourceforge.net/ [27]). The number of independent components (ICs) was estimated using the minimum description length (MDL) criterion [28], as implemented in GIFT. After dimension reduction using principal component analysis (PCA) in two reduction steps, group ICA was performed using the Infomax algorithm [29]. The statistical reliability of estimated ICs was tested using the ICASSO toolbox [30], implemented in GIFT. Using ICASSO, the IC estimation was calculated 20 times, varying the initial conditions of the algorithm as well as the bootstrapped datasets. The reliability of the identified ICs was assessed by clustering the results of each run. As a last step, we calculated differences between the three substances using a one-way ANOVA (FWE-corrected, p < 0.05) using SPM8.

For data visualization, whole-brain spatial activation maps were imported to the Multi-image analysis GUI (MANGO, http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango) and overlaid onto a standardized anatomical template in MNI space.

Results

Olfactory performance

Olfactory testing revealed normal olfactory performance for all investigated participants. The achieved TDI-score ranged from 34 to 40.25 (mean TDI 36.00; SD, 2.05), and no significant differences were found between males (mean TDI 35.93; SD 2.56) and females (mean TDI 36.07; SD 1.59).

Rating of trigeminal stimuli

The investigated stimuli were evaluated with regard to pleasantness, pain perception, familiarity on a visual analogue scale (VAS). The results of the ratings of the applied substances are presented in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Ratings of the three applied substances CO2, menthol and cinnamaldehyde.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121091.t001

fMRI results

All three functional data sets, one for each session and stimulation, were submitted to a combined group ICA estimation, which resulted in 50 independent components. For further analysis, we included within-brain activations of the reported components.

For each stimulus, we observed three major networks, which overlapped largely between stimuli. These networks were labeled the (1) olfactory network, (2) somatosensory, and (3) integrative network.

1) Olfactory network: This network was centered in the putamen, bilaterally, which comprised characteristic olfactory processing areas, including the piriform cortex (PIR), the entorhinal cortex, the amygdala, as well as parts of the thalamus (see Table 2). Importantly, this network was relatively symmetric in both hemispheres, even though the stimuli were only applied to the left nostril. We compared the spatial extent of this network for the three stimuli by using a one-way ANOVA, which revealed no significant differences (see Fig. 1A).

thumbnail
Table 2. Significant FWE- corrected clusters of the olfactory network for all substances separately and for the conjunction of three stimuli.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121091.t002

thumbnail
Fig 1. Axial mean anatomical images overlaid with (A) the olfactory network, (B) the somatosensory network and (C) the integrative network resulting from the combined group ICA (p < 0.05, FWE-corrected).

All three networks were detected for all three investigated substances. For all investigated networks, the conjunction analysis revealed highly overlapping functional network patterns.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121091.g001

2) Somatosensory network: The network comprised some areas of the pain matrix [32], such as the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex and the insula (see Table 3). Although neuronal activation was found bilaterally, clusters were significantly larger in the right hemisphere. Again, a one-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences between the spatial distributions of brain activation due to the three stimuli (see Fig. 1B).

thumbnail
Table 3. Significant FWE- corrected clusters of the somatosensory network for all substances separately and for the conjunction of three stimuli.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121091.t003

3) Integrative network: The identified network included clusters in the orbitofrontal cortex, the insula, the inferior parietal lobule, and the middle and superior temporal gyrus (see Table 4). This network comprised several areas known to be involved in multisensory integration (hence the label), and showed a clear predominance for the left hemisphere, ipsilateral to the application of the stimuli. The comparison of stimulus-specific network patterns again showed no significant differences (see Fig. 1C).

thumbnail
Table 4. Significant FWE- corrected clusters of the chemosensory integration network for all substances separately and for the conjunction of three stimuli.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121091.t004

Discussion

In this study, we were able to show that distinct trigeminal chemoreceptors share the same processing pathway in the brain. Specifically, stimulation of three different TRP channels with relatively specific substances led to the activation of very similar brain networks. In all three stimuli, we observed three distinct networks, namely (1) an olfactory network, (2) a somatosensory network, and (3) an integrative network.

Results of previous studies have shown that TRPV1 and TRPA1 are often expressed in the same sensory neurons [16,17]. In contrast, TRPM8 is exclusively expressed in different sensory neurons [17]. Interestingly, Wang et al. [33] have shown that CO2, which is known to activate TRPV1, also evokes responses in TRPA1. These findings may suggest that the neuronal processing networks evoked by CO2 and cinnamaldehyde show a higher degree of concordance compared to the spatial network pattern induced by menthol. However, the results of our study revealed highly consistent networks for all three substances, indicating a common processing pathway of trigeminal stimuli.

In the following paragraphs, all three determined functional networks will be discussed in relation to our data and the published literature in detail.

The olfactory network

The olfactory network includes clusters in areas related to odor processing, such as the PIR, the entorhinal cortex, the parahippocampal areas, and the insula. These findings are in line with previously published functional imaging data (for review see [34]). This network was determined for all investigated substances. It may be no surprise that menthol and cinnamaldehyde with their clear olfactory precepts evoked an olfactory network. It is, however, interesting to note that even CO2, which is a strong trigeminal stimulus (causing stinging or tingling sensations) with virtually no odor, activated this olfactory network—and to the same extent as the other two stimuli. The evaluation of the three stimuli revealed that the majority of raters were able to label menthol and cinnamaldehyde (cinnamon) correctly, whereas CO2 was labelled as ‘no specific odor’. This activation of olfactory areas evoked by CO2 result supports the close relationship of the olfactory and the trigeminal pathway, as suggested by previous studies [1,3541]. A recently published study that used electrophysiological single- and multi-unit recordings may shed light on the impact of olfactory brain areas on trigeminal stimulus processing [42]. The neuronal activation pattern in the PIR of mice, evoked by CO2 stimulation, differed from the activation pattern induced by olfactory stimuli. This finding suggests that the PIR is responsible for the encoding of the stimulus modality (olfactory or trigeminal). In addition, CO2 stimulation led to a delay in PIR activation, indicating that trigeminal stimuli enter the PIR via a different route compared to olfactory stimuli. Unfortunately, fMRI is not appropriate for the examination of these temporal differences; however, it would be interesting to work on this question in future investigations. Thus, the PIR may be seen as a chemosensory processing area rather than a pure olfactory area.

The somatosensory network

The second network we observed, covered parts of the so called “pain matrix” [32], a network which is involved in processing a large variety of painful sensations [4345]. The core regions of this network are the primary and secondary somatosensory areas, the insula, and the anterior cingulate cortex [4648]. In our study we were able to determine a network involving the primary and secondary somatosensory areas as well as the insula. Even though two of the applied stimuli were not perceived as painful, core regions of the pain network were activated in all three substances. The trigeminal pathway processes a variety of sensory inputs, such as temperature, somatosensory or nociceptive stimuli [49]. Hence, any sensory input which is processed via the trigeminal pathway is potentially harmful and painful and may therefore prepare and activate parts of the pain matrix.

In accordance with previously published findings investigating central processing of pain, we detected larger activation clusters in the right hemisphere contralateral to stimulation [50]. One possible reason for increased activation in the right hemisphere has been described in the homeostatic model of awareness, which claims an asymmetry of emotional awareness [51,52]. Here, both, positive emotions and sensory input are processed in the left anterior insula (AI), whereas negative emotions and sensations, such as pain, are processed predominantly in the right AI. However, our results argue against this interpretation, as we did not observe any significant differences in the neuronal activation pattern between the investigated stimuli; thus, even the non-painful stimuli (cinnamaldehyde and menthol) led to predominantly right-sided activations (see Fig. 1B).

The integrative network

The detected network was lateralized to the left and included neuronal activation in the orbitofrontal gyrus, the inferior parietal lobule, the superior temporal gyrus, and a large cluster in the AI. These areas are involved in multisensory integration [5254], particularly that involving chemosensory stimuli. In fact, the same brain areas were recently identified as constituting a task-independent network for the processing of mixed olfactory-trigeminal stimuli [19]. However, that study reported a predominance of the right hemisphere, in contrast to our findings. We would like to remind the reader that we stimulated participants exclusively through their left nostril, whereas the earlier study used bilateral stimulation. However, our observation of an ipsilateral network is in accordance with the notion that olfactory information is processed predominantly, but not exclusively, ipsilaterally [49].

Conclusion

Within the last several years, the intranasal trigeminal system has been investigated in more detail, resulting in a deeper insight into the transduction of potentially noxious stimuli via the trigeminal pathway. Although previous studies identified different subfamilies of TRP channels, serving as receptors that evoke trigeminal sensations, the neuronal basis and processing pathway of these substances was still unclear. The results of our study suggest a common processing pathway for trigeminal stimulation, as the investigated substances targeted various sub-types of TRP channels and evoked distinct sensations in the nose. Thus, we assume that, although the TRPM8 receptor is exclusively found in different sensory neurons from the TRPV1 and TRPA1 receptors, the neuronal activation is processed in the same neuronal network.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EU JF VS. Performed the experiments: K. Kollndorfer K. Kowalczyk EH CAM JK ST VS. Analyzed the data: K. Kollndorfer JF VS. Wrote the paper: K. Kollndorfer JF VS.

References

  1. 1. Kleemann AM, Albrecht J, Schöpf V, Haegler K, Kopietz R, Hempel JM (2009) Trigeminal perception is necessary to localize odors. Physiol Behav 97: 401–405. pmid:19303891
  2. 2. Frasnelli J, Charbonneau G, Collignon O, Lepore F (2009) Odor localization and sniffing. Chem Senses 34: 139–144. pmid:19001464
  3. 3. Kobal G, Van Toller S, Hummel T (1989) Is there directional smelling? Experientia 45: 130–132. pmid:2493388
  4. 4. Hummel T, Futschik T, Frasnelli J, Hüttenbrink K-B (2003) Effects of olfactory function, age, and gender on trigeminally mediated sensations: a study based on the lateralization of chemosensory stimuli. Toxicol Lett 140–141: 273–280. pmid:12676474
  5. 5. Von Skramlik E (1925) Über die Lokalisation der Empfindungen bei den niederen Sinnen. Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie 56: 69–140. pmid:19349661
  6. 6. Bensafi M, Iannilli E, Gerber J, Hummel T (2008) Neural coding of stimulus concentration in the human olfactory and intranasal trigeminal systems. Neuroscience 154: 832–838. pmid:18485604
  7. 7. Iannilli E, Gerber J, Frasnelli J, Hummel T (2007) Intranasal trigeminal function in subjects with and without an intact sense of smell. Brain Res 1139: 235–244. pmid:17274965
  8. 8. Iannilli E, Del Gratta C, Gerber JC, Romani GL, Hummel T (2008) Trigeminal activation using chemical, electrical, and mechanical stimuli. Pain 139: 376–388. pmid:18583050
  9. 9. Albrecht J, Kopietz R, Frasnelli J, Wiesmann M, Hummel T, Lundström JN (2010) The neuronal correlates of intranasal trigeminal function-an ALE meta-analysis of human functional brain imaging data. Brain Res Rev 62: 183–196. pmid:19913573
  10. 10. Caterina MJ, Schumacher MA, Tominaga M, Rosen TA, Levine JD, Julius D (1997) The capsaicin receptor: a heat-activated ion channel in the pain pathway. Nature 389: 816–824. pmid:9349813
  11. 11. Thürauf N, Friedel I, Hummel C, Kobal G (1991) The mucosal potential elicited by noxious chemical stimuli with CO2 in rats: is it a peripheral nociceptive event? Neurosci Lett 128: 297–300. pmid:1945051
  12. 12. McKemy DD, Neuhausser WM, Julius D (2002) Identification of a cold receptor reveals a general role for TRP channels in thermosensation. Nature 416: 52–58. pmid:11882888
  13. 13. Peier AM, Moqrich A, Hergarden AC, Reeve AJ, Andersson DA, Story GM, et al. (2002) A TRP channel that senses cold stimuli and menthol. Cell 108: 705–715. pmid:11893340
  14. 14. Jordt S- E, Bautista DM, Chuang H- H, McKemy DD, Zygmunt PM, Högestätt ED, et al. (2004) Mustard oils and cannabinoids excite sensory nerve fibres through the TRP channel ANKTM1. Nature 427: 260–265. pmid:14712238
  15. 15. Bandell M, Story GM, Hwang SW, Viswanath V, Eid SR, Petrus MJ, et al. (2004) Noxious cold ion channel TRPA1 is activated by pungent compounds and bradykinin. Neuron 41: 849–857. pmid:15046718
  16. 16. Salas MM, Hargreaves KM, Akopian AN (2009) TRPA1-mediated responses in trigeminal sensory neurons: interaction between TRPA1 and TRPV1. Eur J Neurosci 29: 1568–1578. pmid:19419422
  17. 17. Story GM, Peier AM, Reeve AJ, Eid SR, Mosbacher J, Hricik TR, et al. (2003) ANKTM1, a TRP-like channel expressed in nociceptive neurons, is activated by cold temperatures. Cell 112: 819–829. pmid:12654248
  18. 18. Frasnelli J, Albrecht J, Bryant B, Lundström JN (2011) Perception of specific trigeminal chemosensory agonists. Neuroscience 189: 377–383. pmid:21575683
  19. 19. Frasnelli J, Lundström JN, Schöpf V, Negoias S, Hummel T, Lepore F (2012) Dual processing streams in chemosensory perception. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 288. pmid:23091456
  20. 20. Schöpf V, Windischberger C, Robinson S, Kasess CH, Fischmeister FP, Lanzenberger R, et al. (2011) Model-free fMRI group analysis using FENICA. Neuroimage 55: 185–193. pmid:21078400
  21. 21. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G (1997) “Sniffin” sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 22: 39–52. pmid:9056084
  22. 22. Kobal G, Hummel T, Sekinger B, Barz S, Roscher S, Wolf S (1996) “Sniffin’ sticks”: screening of olfactory performance. Rhinology 34: 222–226. pmid:9050101
  23. 23. Kobal G, Klimek L, Wolfensberger M, Gudziol H, Temmel A, Owen CM, et al. (2000) Multicenter investigation of 1,036 subjects using a standardized method for the assessment of olfactory function combining tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds. Eur Arch oto-rhino-laryngology 257: 205–211.
  24. 24. Zaitsev M, Hennig J, Speck O (2004) Point spread function mapping with parallel imaging techniques and high acceleration factors: fast, robust, and flexible method for echo-planar imaging distortion correction. Magn Reson Med 52: 1156–1166. pmid:15508146
  25. 25. Kobal G (1981) Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen des menschlichen Geruchssinnes. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag.
  26. 26. Wiesmann M, Kopietz R, Albrecht J, Linn J, Reime U, Kara E, et al. (2006) Eye closure in darkness animates olfactory and gustatory cortical areas. Neuroimage 32: 293–300. pmid:16631383
  27. 27. Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 14: 140–151. pmid:11559959
  28. 28. Li Y- O, Adali T, Calhoun VD (2007) Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Hum Brain Mapp 28: 1251–1266. pmid:17274023
  29. 29. Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ (1997) The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters. Vision Res 37: 3327–3338. pmid:9425547
  30. 30. Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004) Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22: 1214–1222. pmid:15219593
  31. 31. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathansassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15: 273–289. pmid:11771995
  32. 32. Ingvar M (1999) Pain and functional imaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354: 1347–1358. pmid:10466155
  33. 33. Wang YY, Chang RB, Liman ER (2010) TRPA1 is a component of the nociceptive response to CO2. J Neurosci 30: 12958–12963. pmid:20881114
  34. 34. Seubert J, Freiherr J, Djordjevic J, Lundström JN (2013) Statistical localization of human olfactory cortex. Neuroimage 66: 333–342. pmid:23103688
  35. 35. Frasnelli J, Schuster B, Zahnert T, Hummel T (2006) Chemosensory specific reduction of trigeminal sensitivity in subjects with olfactory dysfunction. Neuroscience 142: 541–546. pmid:16844306
  36. 36. Frasnelli J, Schuster B, Hummel T (2010) Olfactory dysfunction affects thresholds to trigeminal chemosensory sensations. Neurosci Lett 468: 259–263. pmid:19900505
  37. 37. Frasnelli J, Hummel T (2007) Interactions between the chemical senses: trigeminal function in patients with olfactory loss. Int J Psychophysiol 65: 177–181. pmid:17434636
  38. 38. Frasnelli J, Schuster B, Hummel T (2007) Interactions between olfaction and the trigeminal system: what can be learned from olfactory loss. Cereb Cortex 17: 2268–2275. pmid:17150985
  39. 39. Stuck BA, Baja J, Lenz F, Herr RM, Heiser C (2011) Co-stimulation with an olfactory stimulus increases arousal responses to trigeminal stimulation. Neuroscience 176: 442–446. pmid:21223995
  40. 40. Kobal G, Palisch K, Wolf SR, Meyer ED, Hüttenbrink KB, et al. (2001) A threshold-like measure for the assessment of olfactory sensitivity: the “random” procedure. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 258: 168–172. pmid:11407447
  41. 41. Hummel T, Iannilli E, Frasnelli J, Boyle J, Gerber J (2009) Central processing of trigeminal activation in humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1170: 190–195. pmid:19686136
  42. 42. Carlson KS, Xia CZ, Wesson DW (2013) Encoding and Representation of Intranasal CO2 in the Mouse Olfactory Cortex. J Neurosci 33: 13873–13881. pmid:23966706
  43. 43. Baykara RA, Bozgeyik Z, Akgul O, Ozgocmen S (2013) Low back pain in patients with rheumatoid arthritis: Clinical characteristics and impact of low back pain on functional ability and health related quality of life. J Back Musculoskelet Rehabil 26: 367–374. pmid:23948822
  44. 44. Seidel S, Kasprian G, Furtner J, Schöpf V, Essmeister M, Sycha T, et al. (2011) Mirror therapy in lower limb amputees—a look beyond primary motor cortex reorganization. Rofo 183: 1051–1057. pmid:21959885
  45. 45. Tedeschi G, Russo A, Conte F, Salemi F, Tessitore A (2013) The role of BOLD-fMRI in elucidating migraine pathophysiology. Neurol Sci 34 Suppl 1: S47–50.
  46. 46. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede R- D, Zubieta J- K (2005) Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain 9: 463–484. pmid:15979027
  47. 47. Treede RD, Kenshalo DR, Gracely RH, Jones AK (1999) The cortical representation of pain. Pain 79: 105–111. pmid:10068155
  48. 48. Peyron R, Frot M, Schneider F, Garcia-Larrea L, Mertens P, Barral FG, et al. (2002) Role of operculoinsular cortices in human pain processing: converging evidence from PET, fMRI, dipole modeling, and intracerebral recordings of evoked potentials. Neuroimage 17: 1336–1346. pmid:12414273
  49. 49. Lundström JN, Boesveldt S, Albrecht J (2011) Central Processing of the Chemical Senses: an Overview. ACS Chem Neurosci 2: 5–16. pmid:21503268
  50. 50. Tracey I (2008) Imaging pain. Br J Anaesth 101: 32–39. pmid:18556697
  51. 51. Craig ADB (2005) Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? Trends Cogn Sci 9: 566–571. pmid:16275155
  52. 52. Craig ADB (2009) How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 59–70. pmid:19096369
  53. 53. Brass M, Haggard P (2010) The hidden side of intentional action: the role of the anterior insular cortex. Brain Struct Funct 214: 603–610. pmid:20512363
  54. 54. Craig ADB (2011) Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1225: 72–82. pmid:21534994