Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Executive Functions in Tobacco Dependence: Importance of Inhibitory Capacities

  • Valentin Flaudias ,

    vflaudias@chu-clermontferrand.fr

    Affiliation Clermont Université, Université d'Auvergne, EA NPsy-Sydo, BP 10448, F-63000, Clermont-Ferrand, France; CHU Clermont-Ferrand, Pôle Psychiatrie B, F-63001, Clermont-Ferrand, France

  • Marie Christine Picot,

    Affiliation DIM, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France

  • Jorge Lopez-Castroman,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, CHRU Nimes / INSERM U1061, Nimes, France

  • Pierre-Michel Llorca,

    Affiliation Clermont Université, Université d'Auvergne, EA NPsy-Sydo, BP 10448, F-63000, Clermont-Ferrand, France; CHU Clermont-Ferrand, Pôle Psychiatrie B, F-63001, Clermont-Ferrand, France

  • Audrey Schmitt,

    Affiliation Clermont Université, Université d'Auvergne, EA NPsy-Sydo, BP 10448, F-63000, Clermont-Ferrand, France; CHU Clermont-Ferrand, Pôle Psychiatrie B, F-63001, Clermont-Ferrand, France

  • Jean Perriot,

    Affiliation Dispensaire Émile Roux, Université d'Auvergne, Clermont-Ferrand, France

  • Vera Georgescu,

    Affiliation DIM, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France

  • Philippe Courtet,

    Affiliation Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Post-Acute Care, CHRU Montpellier / INSERM U1061, Montpellier, France

  • Xavier Quantin,

    Affiliation Respiratory Disease Department, CHU Montpellier / Laboratoire Epsylon, Montpellier, France

  • Sébastien Guillaume

    Affiliation Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Post-Acute Care, CHRU Montpellier / INSERM U1061, Montpellier, France

Abstract

Background

Executive functions are linked to tobacco dependence and craving. In this cross-sectional study, we assessed the impact of three executive functions: updating, inhibition and shifting processes on tobacco craving and dependence.

Method

134 tobacco consumers were included in this study: 81 moderately (Fagerström score <7) and 53 heavily dependent (Fagerström score >7). Dependence was assessed with the Fagerström test and craving with the tobacco craving questionnaire (TCQ 12). We used the Stroop test and the Hayling test to measure inhibition, the Trail Making Test to measure shifting processes and the n-back test to measure updating processes. A multivariate logistic model was used to assess which variables explained best the level of nicotine dependence.

Results

Inhibition (p = 0.002) and updating (p = 0.014) processes, but not shifting processes, were associated with higher tobacco dependence. Inhibition capacity had a significant effect on the nicotine dependence level independently of age, education, time since last cigarette, intellectual quotient, craving, updating and shifting process.

Conclusions

Nicotine dependence level seems better explained by inhibition capacities than by craving and updating effects. The capacity to inhibit our behaviours is a good predictor of the severity of tobacco dependence. Our results suggest a prefrontal cortex dysfunction affecting the inhibitory capacities of heavy tobacco dependent smokers. Further studies are needed to investigate the application of these findings in the treatment of tobacco dependence.

Introduction

Half of all tobacco users will die from a tobacco-related disease, a total of 6 million deaths or one in every 10 deaths occurring each year in the world [1]. Although smoking cessation programs and public policies against tobacco are being reinforced, especially in industrialised countries, 70% of patients relapse in the first six months after quitting smoking [2]. One reason is probably the strength of nicotine dependence. Indeed, Kandel et al [3] found nicotine more addictive than alcohol, marijuana or cocaine in a large population study in the US (N = 87,915). A better understanding of the underpinnings of nicotine dependence may help to improve smoking cessation programs.

A lot of factors are known to have an impact on smoking dependence: craving [4], age of initiation, level of education [5] and cognitive abilities [6,7]. These abilities concern memory, learning, attention and executive functions (EFs) [8,9]. These last functions could be defined as a set of mental processes that control and regulate other abilities and behaviours. For instance, EFs are needed for goal-directed behaviours and to initiate and stop actions. Miyake et al [10] identified three components of EFs (see also [11], for an alternative review):

  1. Updating: the continuous monitoring and quick addition or deletion of contents within one’s working memory. In practice, updating is often interchanged with the concept of working memory [12]. Working memory is a more general concept and updating processes could be seen as a construct of working memory. Given that working memory is probably impaired in smokers compared to non-smokers, the self-medication theory posits that tobacco use could enhance working memory [1315].
  2. Shifting processes: the ability to switch between thinking about two or more different concepts simultaneously [16]. Although this cognitive function has not been specifically explored in smokers, studies with the Trail Making Test (TMT), which measures shifting processes, did not observe any difference between smokers and non-smokers [15,17].
  3. Inhibitory processes: ‘the capacity to deliberately inhibit dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses when necessary’ [10]. The impact of inhibition capacities on tobacco dependence is unclear. Some studies did not find differences in inhibitory control between non-smokers and light or heavy smokers [1719], but other authors have underlined that poor inhibitory control is linked with more dependence [20,21]. Recently, Billieux et al [22] found in non-deprived light-to-moderate smokers that inhibition capacities measured by a go-nogo task had a significant impact on nicotine dependence independent of age, craving and speed processing. Tobacco use could also have an impact on inhibition capacities as measured by a Stroop test or a Continuous Performance Test [23].

A recent study of the neuropsychological factors linked to tobacco use in a large sample (N = 1,002 smokers and 1,161 never smoking controls) found a deficit of visual attention and an excess of impulsivity in smokers compared to non-smokers, but no difference for verbal episodic memory, shifting processes, verbal fluency, verbal working memory and Stroop interference [15]. However, two relevant factors that are likely to influence prefrontal cortex (PFC) dysfunction caused by tobacco dependence were not controlled for: level of dependence and craving.

In this study, we aim to investigate the influence of EFs (shifting, updating and inhibition processes) on the level of dependence of a sample of current smokers, while adjusting for craving. We hypothesise that dependence levels will be associated with updating and inhibition impairments, but not with shifting processes.

Method

Study design and participants

This cross-sectional study is an ancillary project of a cohort study aiming to evaluate neurocognitive factors involved in smoking relapse (clinicaltrials.gov NCT01554436). The local research ethics committee approved the study (Comité de protection des personnes ‘Sud-Méditerranée IV’ Lapeyronie Hospital, Montpellier, France) and all participants completed and returned a consent form.

A total of 134 patients (74 women and 60 men) were included. They were consecutively recruited in a tobacco smoking cessation program in two academic centres in France (Montpellier and Clermont-Ferrand university hospitals). All participants were current and non-deprived smokers at the time of assessment and were seeking a smoking cessation program. Inclusion criteria consisted of: 1) being a tobacco consumer (Fagerström > 3), 2) age between 18 and 60, and 3) being a native or fluent French speaker. Pregnant women and hospitalised patients were not included. They only non-inclusion criterion was an inability to be followed for 6 months. Importantly, having a psychiatric disorder was not an exclusion criterion. twenty eligible subjects refused to participate and were not included during this period. There was no significant difference in demographic characteristics between included and non-included subjects.

Clinical assessment

A senior tobacco specialist collected demographic data, actual medication and smoking characteristics during the inclusion visit. Tobacco dependence and craving were assessed. Previous studies have shown that psychiatric comorbidities have an effect on nicotine dependence, in particular depression [24] and substance use disorders [25]. Thus, we measured comorbidities and in particular alcohol dependence and level of depression. As the acute effect of nicotine on neurocognitive performance is well demonstrated [13], we also measured the time since the last cigarette (TSLC).

Psychiatric Comorbidity.

Psychiatrics diagnoses were assessed using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [26]. We also used the CAGE test [27] to assess alcohol use. This is a brief questionnaire (four questions) to assess alcohol use. Patients with a score of at least 2 on this test were diagnosed as presenting ‘alcohol misuse’. Finally, a dimensional assessment of mood status was obtained using the French version of the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) [28].

The Tobacco Craving Questionnaire (TCQ-12).

The TCQ-12 includes 12 self-report questions that evaluate four dimensions of craving: emotionality, expectancy, compulsivity and purposefulness [29]. The α-Cronbach in our population between the twelve items was 0.77, which represents a good reliability for the tool.

Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence [30].

We used the French version [31] to measure the level of smoking dependence. This self-report questionnaire comprises 6 questions scored from 0 to 1 or 0 to 3, and yields a total score that ranges from 0 (not dependent) to 10 (highest dependence). Two groups of dependent patients, heavy and moderate, were identified according to their score on the Fagerström test. In accordance with the ‘French national consensus conference’ [32], patients with scores equal or greater than 7 were considered heavy smokers.

Neuropsychological assessment

On the same day, patients underwent the neuropsychological tests with a neuropsychologist.

Intellectual functioning: fNART [33].

Premorbid intelligence was assessed using the French language adaptation of the National Adult Reading Test [34]. The test consists of 33 words, graded in difficulty, whose pronunciation cannot be determined from their spelling. A score of correct answers was calculated. This score was proved to be highly correlated with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale score [33]. An estimated intellectual quotient (IQ) could be calculated with the following formula: IQ = 124.44–1.54 x fNART errors [33].

Updating processes: n-back test [35].

The n-back measures the updating process of working memory. In this task, squares are briefly presented on a computer screen to the participants. The subject must remember if the current location of the square is the same as the previous square position on rial back (1-back), of the square position two trials back (2-back) and of the square position three trials back (3-back). The number of correct answers was an indicator of updating capacity.

Shifting processes: TMT A and B [36] measure shifting processes.

On the TMT A the subject is required to draw lines to connect consecutively numbered circles. On the TMT B the subject is required to draw lines to connect consecutively numbered and alphabetised circles alternately. This phase assesses a flexible conceptual shifting while keeping track of the sequences of letters and numbers. Performance is assessed with the time it takes in seconds to complete the two tasks. A lower score indicates a better shifting capacity. The number of errors and the corrected errors are also indicators of performance on this task.

Inhibition capacities: Hayling test and Stroop test

The Hayling test [37] measures the capacity to inhibit an automatic semantic answer which is appropriate. In the first part of this test, the participant has to complete a sentence with a semantically appropriate word (part A). In the second part of this task, which is the inhibition part, the same sentence has to be completed with a non-semantically related word (part B). Three scores can be calculated: i) initiation, the total time to answer the fifteen sentences of part A (Hayling A); ii) inhibition, the total time to answer the fifteen sentences of part B (Hayling B); and iii) interference, the difference between the scores on the two parts (Hayling B-A). The higher the score the lower the inhibition capacity.

The Stroop word color test proposed by Golden [38], which is derived from the classical Stroop test [39]. In this procedure the subject has 45 seconds to: (a) read colour-related words printed in black ink (Stroop W), (b) name the colour of ‘XXXXs’ printed in colour (Stroop C) and (c) identify the colour of words printed in coloured ink, where colours and words do not match (Stroop WC). The number of correct answers is recorded and an interference score is calculated as follows: WC-[(W x C)/(W + C)]. Where these tasks are concerned, the fNART and n-back were computerised tests, and while the TMT, Hayling test and Stroop test were pencil tests.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive analyses were performed in the whole sample and in the groups of moderately and heavily dependent subjects. Demographic, clinical characteristics and neuropsychological scores were compared between the two groups using a Mann-Whitney test for quantitative variables. When the difference was significant, Cohen’s d was calculated. Qualitative variables were compared using either a chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test when the chi-square validity conditions were not met.

Pearson’s correlations were computed between neuropsychological performances, clinical and tobacco-related variables. To correct for multiple testing, several adjustment methods were used and provided similar results: the Bonferroni adjustment, the step-down Bonferroni adjustment and the linear step-up adjustment based on the false discovery rate (SAS proc multtest with the options: bonferroni, holm, fdr).

Finally, a multivariate logistic regression was performed to assess which variables best explained the level of nicotine dependence. Explanatory variables were introduced into the model according to their clinical relevance using expert knowledge. To assess the predictive ability of the model, the concordance rate between predicted and observed responses was calculated. Statistical significance was set to 0.05 for all analyses. Analyses were performed with SPSS 19 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) and SAS 9 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C).

Results

The sample was composed of 81 (36 men and 45 women) moderately dependent smokers and 53 heavily dependent smokers (24 men and 29 women).

Relationship between demographic, clinical and tobacco-related data with dependence level

Table 1 summarises the characteristics of the whole sample and of both moderately and heavily dependant samples. Heavily dependent smokers presented significantly higher levels of craving (p = 0.016; d = -0.47), and a non-significant trend for less time since last cigarette (p = 0.08). There were twice as many patients on antidepressants in the high dependence group than in the moderate dependence group (p = 0.02). There were no other differences between the groups in terms of age, level of education, sex, level of alcohol dependence, level of depression, and intellectual quotient.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of all patients and with moderate and heavy dependence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150940.t001

Relationship between neuropsychological performances and dependence level

In the univariate analysis, no significant differences were observed between moderately and heavily dependents concerning IQ total and shifting processes (TMT; Table 2). Concerning updating, higher n-back total scores were observed among moderate dependents (p = 0.014), a group effect for Hayling part A (p = 0.021), part B (p<0.001), and Hayling B-A (p = 0.002) showed that heavily dependent smokers had poorer inhibition capacities than moderately dependents. None of the Stroop test scores were significantly different between groups.

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of level of dependence with regards to the scores of neuropsychological tests.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150940.t002

We further explored the relationship between neuropsychological performances and the clinical features either associated with dependence in our univariate analysis or known to potentially impact neuropsychological outcomes. There were two significant correlations after correcting for multiple testing. A negative correlation (-0.35; Bonferroni p value of 0.003) was observed between the Stroop interference score measuring inhibition and the TMTB RT score measuring shifting process and a positive correlation was found between IQ and updating process (0.33; Bonferroni p value of 0.009).

No correlation between neuropsychological performances and craving score, time since last cigarette, MADRS score were observed.

Multivariate analysis to explain the level of nicotine dependence

Finally, in order to explore the specific impact of each variable on dependence, all clinically relevant variables were introduced into the model: demographic variables (age, education), tobacco-related variables (time since last cigarette, craving), clinical variables (antidepressant, IQ) and neuropsychological performances (updating process (n-back tot), shifting process (TMTB RT) and inhibition process (Hayling B-A, Stroop interference)). We found that only inhibition capacities (Hayling B-A) significantly affected the level of dependence (p = 0.0138, concordance rate 73.4%).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore the link between executive functions and nicotine dependence considering level of craving and time since last cigarette. Thus, we compared cognitive processes (updating, shifting and inhibition) between moderately and heavily dependent smokers. Our results suggest that nicotine dependence level seems better explained by inhibition capacities assessed by the Hayling test than by craving and updating effects. In line with previous studies [15,17], we did not observe any effect for shifting processes.

Although heavily dependent smokers presented higher levels of craving compared to moderately dependent smokers, craving was no longer a significant predictor of nicotine dependence once inhibition scores, IQ, education, age, TSLC and other EFs were entered into the model. In other words, the capacity to inhibit seems to modulate the association between tobacco craving and dependence: the more inhibition capacities the less craving and dependence. Inhibition is a common factor of many important components of addiction such as impulsivity [40], attentional bias [41], or compulsive behaviours [8,42]. Specific targeting of inhibition in clinical studies might help to improve knowledge and management of addiction.

Our results are in line with those reported by Billieux et al [22], who associated the lack of inhibitory control with tobacco dependence using a different measure of inhibition capacities (a go-nogo task)[22]. Other studies failed to find differences in inhibitory control between non-smokers and light smokers or heavier smokers but were limited by: i) non-representative samples (only adolescents); ii) small sample sizes (n<35 in any of them); and, iii) unclear definitions, such as a confusion between inhibition capacities and impulsivity or between inhibition ‘impairment’ and ‘difficulties’ [1719]. Indeed, inhibition can be seen as a cognitive function, as we do, or as a personality dimension that characterises impulsivity. The Whiteside and Lynam [43] model of impulsivity defines the dimension of ‘urgency’ as the ‘inability to inhibiting a dominant response or automatic’. This definition corresponds to the inhibition capacities, which would be thus a part of general impulsivity.

The other problem concerns the use of some cognitive tests, which were initially designed to assess the cognitive deficit in brain-damaged patients. They provide a threshold that delimitates normal functioning and problematic functioning. In studies where we used these tests as a continuous variable assessing the level of cognitive abilities (and not considering a threshold of dysfunction), we observed a relationship between this cognitive ability and dependence. Thus our results did not suggest that all smokers have impaired inhibition capacities but more that dependents show less capacities compared to non-dependent individuals. Studies with larger samples (n = 50 for Billieux et al; n = 282 for Wilson and MacLean [44]; n = 134 for our study), which assessed inhibition capacities and not impairments, observed a link between inhibition capacities and tobacco dependence.

Surprisingly, no interference effect was observed on the Stroop task, which tests inhibition. This result suggests that the inhibition processes measured by the Stroop task and the Hayling test are not equivalent. Following the proposal by Burgess and Shallice [37], we have chosen the Hayling test, because it is probably more appropriate for clinical practice and more sensitive since it measures the capacity to inhibit a correct answer and also the capacity to create an efficient strategy. Furthermore, the two possible answers in the Hayling test are in the same dimension (words) while the Stroop task requires inhibiting a different dimension (e.g. the meaning of the word) in favour of the dimension of interest of the stimulus (e.g. the ink colour of the word).

A major field of research in addictive disorders endeavours to identify distinctive neurobehavioral endophenotypes that predispose individuals to compulsive drug use. Evidences coming from other fields suggests that response inhibition and inhibitory control might be heritable [4547]. In smoking behaviours, recent data suggest that cognitive deficits in inhibitory control are present before starting the use of tobacco. For instance, prospective studies have shown that inhibitory capacities in young people are predictive of consumption of alcohol or cigarettes in adult life [48,49]. Also, Yakir et al [50] showed that attention and impulsivity were predisposing factors for tobacco dependence. Although we cannot study temporal associations in our data, all these results highlight the interest in assessing if low inhibition abilities might be a heritable marker of vulnerability to tobacco consumption.

Within a brain localisation perspective, substance dependence is known to be associated with the mesolimbic system which originates in the ventral tegmental area and projects to reward-related brain areas such as the nucleus accumbens, the amygdala, the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [51]. This last brain region and its important role in addiction is the object of a large body of literature [52]. In the case of tobacco dependence, the PFC is essentially described as a brain region that contributes to the development of craving, the response to smoking cues, and the compulsive behaviour of dependence [53,54].

The PFC is also known to be responsible for EF [55]. In this area, our results support a PFC dysfunction in tobacco dependent subjects, particularly a dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) dysfunction among heavy smokers. Indeed, n-back and Hayling tests are highly linked to this brain localisation [56,57]. A recent review seems to confirm this hypofunction in various regions of the PFC among patients with substance use disorders [7].

Accordingly, functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have shown a differential activation in the amygdala and the PFC during these tasks with and without nicotine consumption [58,59].

Future treatments of dependence could try to enhance the control of automatic answers and inhibition capacities using DLPFC stimulation. New lines of research have recently opened possibilities in this area. For example, cognitive remediation could improve inhibitory capacities [60]. Recent studies have shown that a non-invasive brain stimulation of the DLPFC with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) could enhance decision-making and thus, inhibition capacities [6163]. However, we did not measure directly the localisation of brain dysfunctions. Therefore, our results should be taken cautiously as future studies exploring this cognitive dysfunction and its precise localisation are needed.

Our results should be interpreted with caution. Firstly, craving is known to have many dimensions, which could explain its lack of effect on nicotine dependence after controlling for inhibition capacities. Further studies are needed to explore the impact of inhibitory control on different dimensions of tobacco craving. Secondly, the results of neurocognitive tests do not correspond exactly to a single function. For example, the n-back test assesses not only working memory, but also updating and visuo-spatial processes. Collette and Van der Linden [64] suggested that EFs should be understood as an interplay between different brain regions. Therefore, as none of the neuropsychological tasks is specific to a function and/or a structure, future investigations are needed to understand the processes involved in the executive tasks, and more particularly their link with PFC localisation.

To conclude, our results show the importance of the link between inhibitory capacities, craving and level of dependence. A better understanding of the relation between inhibitory control and important concepts on addiction such as attentional bias, craving, impulsivity and compulsion are needed. More research is warranted to explore the inhibitory capacities of tobacco smokers and their future use as therapeutic targets.

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

All the authors wish to thank Miss Valerie Macioce for editing.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SG. Performed the experiments: JLC AS JP PC XQ. Analyzed the data: VF MCP VG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SG. Wrote the paper: VF JLC PML SG.

References

  1. 1. World Health Organization, Management of Substance Abuse Team. Global status report on alcohol and health, 2014. [Internet]. 2014. Available: http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/112736/1/9789240692763_eng.pdf?ua=1
  2. 2. Hugues J. Nicotine-related disorders. Kaplan & Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook of psychiatry. 8th ed. Philadelphia USA : Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2006. pp. 1257–1264.
  3. 3. Kandel D, Chen K, Warner LA, Kessler RC, Grant B. Prevalence and demographic correlates of symptoms of last year dependence on alcohol, nicotine, marijuana and cocaine in the U.S. population. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1997;44: 11–29. pmid:9031816
  4. 4. Killen JD, Fortmann SP. Craving is associated with smoking relapse: findings from three prospective studies. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5: 137–142. pmid:9234050
  5. 5. Fernández E, Schiaffino A, Borrell C, Benach J, Ariza C, Ramon JM, et al. Social class, education, and smoking cessation: Long-term follow-up of patients treated at a smoking cessation unit. Nicotine Tob Res Off J Soc Res Nicotine Tob. 2006;8: 29–36.
  6. 6. Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35: 377–406. pmid:20451551
  7. 7. Meil W, LaPorte D, Stewart P. Substance Dependence as a Neurological Disorder. Ken-Shiung Chen Dr, Advanced Topics in Neurological Disorders. 2012. Available: http://cdn.intechopen.com/pdfs/32484/InTech-Substance_dependence_as_a_neurological_disorder.pdf
  8. 8. Lubman DI, Yücel M, Pantelis C. Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction. 2004;99: 1491–1502. pmid:15585037
  9. 9. McNamee RL, Dunfee KL, Luna B, Clark DB, Eddy WF, Tarter RE. Brain Activation, Response Inhibition, and Increased Risk for Substance Use Disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32: 405–413. pmid:18302723
  10. 10. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41: 49–100. pmid:10945922
  11. 11. Jurado MB, Rosselli M. The Elusive Nature of Executive Functions: A Review of our Current Understanding. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17: 213–233. pmid:17786559
  12. 12. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16: 174–180. pmid:22336729
  13. 13. Heishman SJ, Kleykamp BA, Singleton EG. Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010; 210: 453–469.
  14. 14. Mendrek A, Monterosso J, Simon SL, Jarvik M, Brody A, Olmstead R, et al. Working memory in cigarette smokers: Comparison to non-smokers and effects of abstinence. Addict Behav. 2006;31: 833–844. pmid:16009504
  15. 15. Wagner M, Schulze-Rauschenbach S, Petrovsky N, Brinkmeyer J, von der Goltz C, Gründer G, et al. Neurocognitive impairments in non-deprived smokers-results from a population-based multi-center study on smoking-related behavior: Cognition in smokers. Addict Biol. 2013;18: 752–761. pmid:22339903
  16. 16. Scott WA. Cognitive Complexity and Cognitive Flexibility. Sociometry. 1962;25: 405.
  17. 17. Dinn WM, Aycicegi A, Harris CL. Cigarette smoking in a student sample: Neurocognitive and clinical correlates. Addict Behav. 2004;29: 107–126. pmid:14667424
  18. 18. Monterosso JR, Aron AR, Cordova X, Xu J, London ED. Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79: 273–277. pmid:15967595
  19. 19. Reynolds B, Patak M, Shroff P, Penfold RB, Melanko S, Duhig AM. Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behavior in adolescent daily smokers and nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2007;15: 264–271. pmid:17563213
  20. 20. Wegmann L, Bühler A, Strunk M, Lang P, Nowak D. Smoking cessation with teenagers: The relationship between impulsivity, emotional problems, program retention and effectiveness. Addict Behav. 2012;37: 463–468. pmid:22273584
  21. 21. Krishnan-Sarin S, Reynolds B, Duhig AM, Smith A, Liss T, McFetridge A, et al. Behavioral Impulsivity Predicts Treatment Outcome in a Smoking Cessation Program for Adolescent Smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88: 79–82. pmid:17049754
  22. 22. Billieux J, Gay P, Rochat L, Khazaal Y, Zullino D, Van der Linden M. Lack of inhibitory control predicts cigarette smoking dependence: Evidence from a non-deprived sample of light to moderate smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;112: 164–167. pmid:20667667
  23. 23. Sacco KA, Bannon KL, George TP. Nicotinic receptor mechanisms and cognition in normal states and neuropsychiatric disorders. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2004;18: 457–474.
  24. 24. Fond G, Guillaume S, Artero S, Bernard P, Ninot G, Courtet P, et al. Self-reported major depressive symptoms at baseline impact abstinence prognosis in smoking cessation program. A one-year prospective study. J Affect Disord. 2013;149: 418–421. pmid:23265988
  25. 25. Craig TJ, Van Natta PA. The association of smoking and drinking habits in a community sample. J Stud Alcohol. 1977;38: 1434–1439. pmid:895152
  26. 26. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20: 22–33;quiz 34–57. pmid:9881538
  27. 27. Ewing JA. Detecting alcoholism: The cage questionnaire. JAMA. 1984;252: 1905–1907. pmid:6471323
  28. 28. Pellet J, Decrat M, Lang F, Chazot L, Tatu P, Blanchon Y, et al. [Description of a sample of 300 MADRS scales on depressive patients]. Ann Méd-Psychol. 1987;145: 170–175.
  29. 29. Berlin I, Singleton EG, Heishman SJ. Validity of the 12-item French version of the Tobacco Craving Questionnaire in treatment-seeking smokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2010;12: 500–507. pmid:20335281
  30. 30. Fagerstrom K-O, Schneider NG. Measuring nicotine dependence: A review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. J Behav Med. 1989;12: 159–182. pmid:2668531
  31. 31. Etter J-F, Duc TV, Perneger TV. Validity of the Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence and of the Heaviness of Smoking Index among relatively light smokers. Addiction. 1999;94: 269–281. pmid:10396794
  32. 32. Underner M, Le Houezec J, Perriot J, Peiffer G. Tests for evaluating tobacco dependence. [Les tests d’évaluation de la dépendance tabagique]. Rev Mal Respir. 2012;29: 462–474. pmid:22542405
  33. 33. Mackinnon A, Ritchie K, Mulligan R. The measurement properties of a French language adaptation of the National Adult Reading Test. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1999;8: 27–38.
  34. 34. Nelson HE, O’Connell A. Dementia: The Estimation of Premorbid Intelligence Levels Using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex. 1978;14: 234–244. pmid:679704
  35. 35. Kirchner WK. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol. 1958;55: 352–358. pmid:13539317
  36. 36. Vickers D, Vincent N, Medvedev A. The geometric structure, construction, and interpretation of path-following (trail-making) tests. J Clin Psychol. 1996;52: 651–661. pmid:8912108
  37. 37. Burgess PW, Shallice T. Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34: 263–272. pmid:8657357
  38. 38. Golden CJ. Identification of brain disorders by the stroop color and word test. J Clin Psychol. 1976;32: 654–658. pmid:956433
  39. 39. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1935;121: 15.
  40. 40. Pattij T, De Vries TJ. The role of impulsivity in relapse vulnerability. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23: 700–705. pmid:23462336
  41. 41. Field M, Cox W. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;97: 1–20. pmid:18479844
  42. 42. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108: 44–79. pmid:23856628
  43. 43. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personal Individ Differ. 2001;30: 669–689.
  44. 44. Wilson SJ, MacLean RR. Associations between self-control and dimensions of nicotine dependence: A preliminary report. Addict Behav. 2013;38: 1812–1815. pmid:23254232
  45. 45. Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, Derbyshire KL, Leppink EW, Grant JE. Impaired response inhibition and excess cortical thickness as candidate endophenotypes for trichotillomania. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;59: 167–173. pmid:25223951
  46. 46. Pauli-Pott U, Roller A, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner M, Mingebach T, Dalir S, Becker K. Inhibitory control and delay aversion in unaffected preschoolers with a positive family history of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55: 1117–1124. pmid:24673598
  47. 47. Hıdıroğlu C, Torres IJ, Er A, Işık G, Yalın N, Yatham LN, et al. Response inhibition and interference control in patients with bipolar I disorder and first-degree relatives. Bipolar Disord. 2015;17: 781–794. pmid:26415581
  48. 48. Nigg JT, Glass JM, Wong MM, Poon E, Jester JM, Fitzgerald HE, et al. Neuropsychological Executive Functioning in Children at Elevated Risk for Alcoholism: Findings in Early Adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113: 302–314. pmid:15122950
  49. 49. Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, et al. Poor Response Inhibition as a Predictor of Problem Drinking and Illicit Drug Use in Adolescents at Risk for Alcoholism and Other Substance Use Disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45: 468–475. pmid:16601652
  50. 50. Yakir A, Rigbi A, Kanyas K, Pollak Y, Kahana G, Karni O, et al. Why do young women smoke? III. Attention and impulsivity as neurocognitive predisposing factors. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17: 339–351. pmid:17141485
  51. 51. Di Chiara G. Role of dopamine in the behavioural actions of nicotine related to addiction. Eur J Pharmacol. 2000;393: 295–314. pmid:10771025
  52. 52. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12: 652–669. pmid:22011681
  53. 53. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, London ED, Childress AR, Lee GS, Bota RG, et al. Brain metabolic changes during cigarette craving. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59: 1162–1172. pmid:12470133
  54. 54. McBride D, Barrett SP, Kelly JT, Aw A, Dagher A. Effects of Expectancy and Abstinence on the Neural Response to Smoking Cues in Cigarette Smokers: an fMRI Study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31: 2728–2738. pmid:16598192
  55. 55. Baddeley AD. Fractionating the central executive. In Struss D, & Knight R T (Eds) Principles of frontal lobe function. Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 246–260.
  56. 56. Nathaniel-James DA, Frith CD. The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Evidence from the Effects of Contextual Constraint in a Sentence Completion Task. NeuroImage. 2002;16: 1094–1102. pmid:12202096
  57. 57. Barch DM, Sheline YI, Csernansky JG, Snyder AZ. Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: specificity to schizophrenia compared with major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53: 376–384. pmid:12614990
  58. 58. Stein EA, Pankiewicz J, Harsch HH, Cho J-K, Fuller SA, Hoffmann RG, et al. Nicotine-Induced Limbic Cortical Activation in the Human Brain: A Functional MRI Study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155: 1009–1015. pmid:9699686
  59. 59. Lawrence NS, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Cognitive Mechanisms of Nicotine on Visual Attention. Neuron. 2002;36: 539–548. pmid:12408855
  60. 60. Fadardi JS, Cox WM. Reversing the sequence: Reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;101: 137–145. pmid:19193499
  61. 61. Li X, Hartwell KJ, Owens M, LeMatty T, Borckardt JJ, Hanlon CA, et al. Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Reduces Nicotine Cue Craving. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73: 714–720. pmid:23485014
  62. 62. Fecteau S, Fregni F, Boggio PS, Camprodon JA, Pascual-Leone A. Neuromodulation of Decision-Making in the Addictive Brain. Subst Use Misuse. 2010;45: 1766–1786. pmid:20590399
  63. 63. Fregni F, Liguori P, Fecteau S, Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Boggio PS. Cortical stimulation of the prefrontal cortex with transcranial direct current stimulation reduces cue-provoked smoking craving: a randomized, sham-controlled study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69: 32–40.
  64. 64. Collette F, Van der Linden M. Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26: 105–125. pmid:11856556