Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Longitudinal influence of alcohol and marijuana use on academic performance in college students

  • Shashwath A. Meda ,

    shashwath.meda@hhchealth.org

    Affiliation Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Ralitza V. Gueorguieva,

    Affiliations Department of Biostatistics, Yale School of Public Health, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Brian Pittman,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Rivkah R. Rosen,

    Affiliation Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Farah Aslanzadeh,

    Affiliation Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Howard Tennen,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology and Neurosciences, Trinity College, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Samantha Leen,

    Affiliation Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Keith Hawkins,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Sarah Raskin,

    Affiliation Department of Community Medicine, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Rebecca M. Wood,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, New Britain, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Carol S. Austad,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, New Britain, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Alecia Dager,

    Affiliations Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Carolyn Fallahi,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Central Connecticut State University, New Britain, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Godfrey D. Pearlson

    Affiliations Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford HealthCare Corporation, Hartford, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Neurobiology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

Abstract

Background

Alcohol and marijuana are the two most abused substances in US colleges. However, research on the combined influence (cross sectional or longitudinal) of these substances on academic performance is currently scant.

Methods

Data were derived from the longitudinal 2-year Brain and Alcohol Research in College Students (BARCS) study including 1142 freshman students who completed monthly marijuana use and alcohol consumption surveys. Subjects were classified into data-driven groups based on their alcohol and marijuana consumption. A linear mixed-model (LMM) was employed using this grouping factor to predict grade point average (GPA), adjusted for a variety of socio-demographic and clinical factors.

Results

Three data-driven clusters emerged: 1) No/low users of both, 2) medium-high alcohol/no-low marijuana, and 3) medium-high users of both substances. Individual cluster derivations between consecutive semesters remained stable. No significant interaction between clusters and semester (time) was noted. Post-hoc analysis suggest that at the outset, compared to sober peers, students using moderate to high levels of alcohol and low marijuana demonstrate lower GPAs, but this difference becomes non-significant over time. In contrast, students consuming both substances at moderate-to-high levels score significantly lower at both the outset and across the 2-year investigation period. Our follow-up analysis also indicate that when students curtailed their substance use over time they had significantly higher academic GPA compared to those who remained stable in their substance use patterns over the two year period.

Conclusions

Overall, our study validates and extends the current literature by providing important implications of concurrent alcohol and marijuana use on academic achievement in college.

Introduction

Substance abuse (including alcohol) by students is prevalent at U.S. colleges and universities. About 4 of 5 college students drink alcohol and among these, half binge drink [13]. Following alcohol, marijuana (MJ) is the most frequent substance of choice among college students [4]. National surveys of individuals aged 18–25 indicate 52% report lifetime use, 31% report usage within the prior twelve months and 21% within the past month [13]. Further, 58% of alcohol drinking adolescents report using alcohol and marijuana simultaneously [5].

Recent research indicates that both alcohol and marijuana tend to alter brain structure/function, and are associated with impaired decision making, memory and impulsivity in young adults and adolescents [612]. Evidence suggests that adolescents who drink alcohol and also smoke tobacco cigarettes/marijuana are more likely to manifest alcohol and other substance use disorders as young adults compared to those who delay using these substances [13]. More recent evidence also points to the concomitant use of both alcohol and marijuana being associated with abnormal neural tissue development in young adults [14] and worse critical skills such as complex attention, memory, processing speed and visuospatial functioning in adolescents [15]. Despite these documented ill-effects of alcohol and marijuana there has been little change in substance use trends among college students in the past decade [4].

Substance use also has a larger adverse effect on cognitive functions when initiated earlier in life [7, 16, 17]. Early-onset marijuana users demonstrate poorer attention, cognitive inhibition and abstract reasoning, all of which are critical skills needed to function and succeed in a college environment [17]. A large longitudinal study confirmed that adolescent-onset marijuana users showed the largest full scale IQ drop between childhood and adulthood. Moreover, adolescent subjects who used marijuana regularly never reached their predicted IQ levels, even after sustained abstinence [18]. This study also documented that persistent cannabis use was associated with neuropsychological decline across broad functional domains, thus highlighting cannabis’ detrimental long term effects.

Relative to the larger costs to society raised by substance use, academic costs might appear to be minimal. However the impact on academic achievement due to substance use may meaningfully influence long-term student success. Most academic performance research with college students has focused on examining alcohol and marijuana effects separately, with little examination of their combined effects. Because substance use patterns tend to cluster among students, it is important to recognize that their influence on academic outcomes may have separable additive or synergistic effects. Substance use has been directly related to a variety of academic problems; substance-using college students skip more classes, spend less time studying, show decreased motivation and have disrupted sleep patterns [1924]. Arria et al. performed a large scale longitudinal study which indicated that college students who used marijuana very frequently during all four years were twice as likely as minimal users to experience discontinuous enrollment [20]. These intermediary processes ultimately cascade into more short- and long- term issues such as dropping out of college, losing potential in-college work opportunities, delayed graduation and unemployment [20, 25].

It is important to note that alcohol and marijuana are not the only factors that contribute to poor academic achievement. A variety of other mediating and/or comorbid factors such as socioeconomic status, mental health (anxiety and depression in particular), cultural and family values, stress handling capability, peer pressure, intelligence quotient (IQ), tobacco smoking etc can play a major role in determining academic performance in college students [19, 22, 2629].

The aim of the current study was therefore to examine the effects of concurrent alcohol and marijuana use on academic performance in a large sample of college students over a two year period. We hypothesized that a) heavy users of either substance would have lower grades than minimal users of both substances and b) students who consumed both alcohol and marijuana heavily would experience a larger decline in GPA over time.

Methods and materials

A convenience sample of first-year students (age range 18–23 years) was recruited from two local colleges with diverse populations through e-mail, flyers and classroom visits to solicit participation in the Brain and Alcohol Research in College Students (BARCS) study [30]. The recruitment captured greater than 95% of possible participants. Given no minors were recruited; consent was obtained only from subjects and not their parents. All subjects provided written informed consent, including consent to obtain their GPAs each semester of the study. The study was approved by institutional review boards at Central Connecticut State University (CCSU), Trinity College, Hartford Hospital and Yale University. As part of the ongoing study, participants completed online demographic, clinical and substance use surveys every month for two years excluding summer months.

Variables acquired longitudinally

Grade Point Average (GPA).

Individual GPA data (up to 2 decimal places) were collected for each semester from college records.

Alcohol use.

Two different measures of alcohol use were recorded for each subject on the monthly substance use questionnaires a) the number of days an individual consumed alcohol in the past 30 days and b) the number of drinks he/she had on each occasion.

Marijuana use.

Students reported their monthly marijuana use in a scaled manner, ranging from 1–6. 1 was defined as having never used marijuana in the past 30 days, 2—having used 1–2 times, 3—having used 3–5 times, 4 –having used 6–9 times, 5 -having used 10–19 times and 6 having used 20+ times in the past month.

Variables acquired only at semester 1

Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT).

Individual SAT scores for math, verbal and writing were obtained from college records. SAT scores were explicitly included as they have been previously demonstrated to be good predictors of intelligence quotient (IQ) [31].

Socio-Economic Status (SES).

SES was calculated using Hollingshead’s four-factor index at the initial visit [32]. Occupational and educational scores were calculated for both parents and then summed to compute a total SES score. Although the direct impact of SES on academic achievement is debatable, we wanted to adjust for any overlapping variance it shared with substance abuse in our current study.

Tobacco (Cigarette) smoking.

Data on cigarette smoking status was used as a covariate in the current study as it has been shown to be linked with adverse health outcomes in college students [33], which might be an important mediator for academic achievement. Smoking status was assessed using the Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence [34]. Individual responses from item 4 from the Fagerstrom (How many cigarettes do you smoke per day) was used to identify cigarette-smokers and non-smokers, converted into a binary yes/no format and employed for further analyses.

Family History for Alcoholism (FHA).

Students were asked to self-report if they had at least one affected first-degree relative with alcoholism. The above ‘yes/no’ answer was binary coded and used for further analysis. Although studies examining FHA abuse among college students have been largely conflicting, there has been evidence showing FHA is associated with increased levels of alcohol use [28]. We therefore included the above variable as a means to control for such effects in the current analysis.

Lastly, given that anxiety disorders [29, 35, 36] and depression [26, 29] have a direct impact on academic success and often co-exist with substance abuse [37], we administered the mental health questionnaires below at study entry (semester 1) to capture and control such confounding aspects in analysis.

State Trait Anxiety Index (STAI).

Students completed the trait sections of the STAI questionnaire [38] at study entry. A total score was computed for each student by summing all these values.

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI).

Students were administered the BDI to assess depressive symptoms at study entry [39], all items from the inventory were summed to yield a total score.

Pre-Processing

Before conducting statistical analysis, alcohol use quantity and frequency were multiplied for each subject to derive total counts of alcoholic drinks consumed per month. Similarly, marijuana scale measures were transformed to continuous measures by taking their midpoint values for each monthly survey input. For example if a student reported consuming marijuana between 6–9 times in the past month, this was converted to a value of 7.5 (midpoint of 6–9). Given that our outcome variable (GPA) was based on a semester basis, we therefore averaged substance use across all months in a semester to derive use measures for that semester. Semester-wise substance use data were then log-transformed and used for further analysis.

Given that the MJ and alcohol use data were highly correlated and skewed (see S1 Fig) we chose to cluster substance use data into groups rather than retaining them as continuous independent predictors in the model in order to estimate valid and non-biased coefficients. A 2-step cluster classifying algorithm was implemented in SPSS based on both alcohol and marijuana consumption measures as input to estimate the underlying data clusters in each semester separately [40, 41]. This clustering algorithm as implemented in SPSS, first constructed a cluster feature tree by placing the first case at the root of the tree in a leaf node, then each successive case was added to an existing node or forms a new node based on its similarity to existing nodes using a distance measure as the similarity criterion. Finally, Individuals were grouped into data-driven clusters using an agglomerative clustering algorithm [39]. These data driven clusters were deemed time-varying and depending on each subjects’ combined substance use patterns for that particular semester, individuals could potentially be categorized under a different cluster membership across each of the four semesters. Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) was used to select the number of optimal data clusters across each semester. Resulting cluster assignments of individuals were then tested for their longitudinal inter-reliability using Cohen’s kappa statistic [42].

Statistical analysis

A longitudinal linear mixed model (LMM) was implemented to assess whether cluster membership predicted changes in GPA across the four semesters. In leiu of missing data the mixed effects approach is advantageous in a few ways over a conventional repeated measures ANOVA model a) it uses all available data on each subject despite missing data by using a maximum likelihood estimation process b) allows greater flexibility in modeling the correlation structure of repeated measures data [43] and c) it relaxes the randomness constraints of the missing data. In the current study, we first considered a full model which included main effects of cluster membership (within-subject, time-varying), sex, semester (within-subject), and the following covariates (captured at semester 1): age, SAT (math, verbal and writing) scores, tobacco smoking status, FHA status, SES, anxiety- and depression-related scores. All interactions between categorical predictors (i.e. sex, semester and cluster) were included in the full model. For parsimony, non-significant interactions, except for the cluster*semester term which interrogated GPA relationship with clusters over time, were dropped in the final model. Post-hoc analyses, including simple cluster effects within each semester, were performed and adjusted for Type I error using the Sidak method. In the above models, the correlation between repeated observations within each subject was modeled by including random subject effects and the best-fitting variance-covariance structure across time. The best fitting model was based on the smallest Akaike information criteria (AIC) [41]. As a secondary analysis, we fit a model including only the main and interactive effects of cluster and semester to examine the overall effects of covariates on the result. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v21 (http://www-01.ibm.com/software/analytics/spss/). A total of 1142 subjects were included in the final model; however the number of subjects with fully available data varied for each semester (see Fig 1). Differential data missingness across substance use clusters for semesters 2–4 in relation to semester 1 cluster assignment was assessed using a chi-square test.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Substance use patterns.

Scatter plots of alcohol and marijuana usage among students across semesters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.g001

In order to get a broad sense of how change in cluster membership (substance use patterns) over the two year period impacted academic GPA we conducted a secondary pseudo-trajectory analysis. In order to do this, each subjects’ data was coded into one of three groups in terms of their substance use as follows a) stable—those who remained in the same cluster at the start and end of the study b) declining—those who moved from a better to worse cluster (cluster 1 to 3, cluster 2 to 3 or cluster 1 to 2) and c) improving—those who moved from a worse to a better cluster (cluster 3 to 2, cluster 3 to 1 and cluster 2 to 1). For simplicity, we only used the starting and the ending semester’s information to decide the above transition groups. Given that there were missing data, the ending semesters were not the same for all subjects. We then performed a LMM similar to our primary analysis, however in this case we replaced the cluster membership predictor that we previously used with the newly coined cluster (group) transition variable and also included semester 1 cluster membership as an additional control variable.

Results

For subjects in the current study, there were no missing data for alcohol use variables across the four semesters (among subjects that provided data). However, the refusal and/or missing rates for marijuana data were as follows: semester 1: 0.4%, semester 2: 0%, semester 3: 22.3% and semester 4: 32.2% which therefore resulted in 22% of students in the 3rd semester and 32% of students in the 4th semester missing a cluster assignment. S1 Table provides details regarding frequency and missingness of all variables included in the LMM.

Substance use cluster (group) identification

Three data-driven clusters were identified individually for each of the four semesters. Based on substance use distribution, clusters were labeled as: No-low use of both substances (cluster 1), medium-high alcohol/No-low marijuana (cluster 2), and medium-high use of both substances (cluster 3; see Fig 1). The Silhouette measure of cohesion and separation for the data clusters was 0.7. The silhouette measure represents how close each point in one cluster is to points in the neighboring clusters and thus provides a way to assess parameters such as number of clusters. Silhouette coefficients (as these values are referred to as) near +1 indicates that the sample is far away from the neighboring clusters. A value of 0 indicates that the sample is on or very close to the decision boundary between two neighboring clusters and negative values indicates that those samples might have been assigned to the wrong cluster. In our study a value of 0.7 would refer to a good-very good separation of data. Demographics and clinical characteristics of the clusters at baseline (first semester) are reported in Table 1. Scatter plots of alcohol/marijuana consumption for clusters across semesters are also shown in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Sample demographics.

Demographics and clinical characteristics of study participants at semester 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.t001

Data missingness in semester 3 and 4 (in relation to semester 1 cluster assignment) was significantly different across the three substance use clusters. For semester 3, missingness statistics (chi-square 19.81; p<0.001) were as follows: 17.3% for cluster 1, 23.3% for cluster 2 and 32.9% of cluster 3. Similarly missingness for semester 4 (chi-square 36.83; p<0.001) data was as follows: 26.1% for cluster 1, 31.3% for cluster 2 and 50% of cluster 3.

Cluster coherence among individuals was fairly stable when assessed in relation to baseline (SEM1→SEM2: κ = 0.64; p<0.001, SEM1→SEM3: κ = 0.70; p<0.001, SEM1→SEM4: κ = 0.67; p<0.001). These kappa values suggested good-very good agreement of designated cluster assignment of subjects across semesters compared to semester 1 values. Comparable cluster coherence emerged when assessed on a semester-to-semester basis.

Linear Mixed-Model (LMM) analysis

As expected, the main effect of cluster membership (F (2,2267.8) = 20.6; p<0.001) was a significant predictor of academic performance after controlling for clinical and demographic variables. Post-hoc analyses showed that compared to cluster 1 students (who consumed little-no alcohol/marijuana); those who consumed moderate-high level of alcohol (cluster 2) obtained lower average GPAs (averaged across semester). Further, students in cluster 3 (who consumed medium-high quantities of both substances) obtained lower grades compared to both cluster 1 and cluster 2 students. No significant higher-order interactions were observed. Although not significant, per our initial hypothesis, the cluster*semester term was retained. Simple effects of cluster within each semester (cluster*semester) revealed differences in GPA between clusters 1 and 2 were significant only at semester 1 (See Fig 2). However, significantly reduced GPA was noted in cluster 3 subjects compared to cluster 1 or 2 students across each of the four semesters. In addition to cluster membership, tobacco smokers (shown in Fig 3) had significantly lower GPA than non-smokers (F(1,873.2) = 4.16; p<0.05), males had significantly lower GPA compared to females (F(1,844.6) = 12.26; p<0.001), SAT scores positively predicted GPA (math (F(1,822.27) = 22.27; p<0.0001), writing (F(1,830.16) = 16.24; p<0.001)) and BDI was negatively associated with GPA (F(1,851.80) = 4.51; p = 0.03)). LMM evaluated without any covariates yielded exactly the same conclusions as above for the main effect of cluster, semester and their interaction.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Linear mixed model plot.

Bar plot of GPA (estimated marginal means) across semesters by cluster. Post-hoc between-group analysis (using a sidak correction for multiple comparisons) is also indicated. Between-cluster, post-hoc significances are marked as follows: ***p<0.001, **p<0.005,*p<0.01. Note: Error bars represent standard error of mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Mean plots of GPA (estimated marginal means) across gender and smoker groups.

Error bars across means represent 95% confidence interval limits.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.g003

Our secondary pseudo-trajectory analysis indicated a significant main effect of the cluster transition predictor. Post-hoc effects revealed that the improving group had significantly higher academic GPA compared to the stable group (see Fig 4). All other post-hoc comparisons were non-significant for this predictor. In addition we also noted semester 1 cluster membership, SAT math, SAT writing, sex and smoking status all to be significantly associated with academic GPA as shown in Table 2 below.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Bar plot of GPA (estimated marginal means) across cluster (group) transitioners from our pseudo trajectory analysis.

Post-hoc between-group analysis (using a Sidak correction for multiple comparisons) is also indicated. The Improver group represents students that went from a worse substance use cluster to a better one over the period of the study. Decliners were the opposite of improvers. Stable group participants started and ended the study in the same substance use cluster. Note: Error bars represent standard error of mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Fixed effects from the linear mixed model conducting the pseudo trajectory analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.t002

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first large-scale study to simultaneously integrate the effects of alcohol and marijuana use on college GPA using a longitudinal statistical model. Given the challenges presented by comorbid and collinear use of both alcohol and marijuana in college students, we performed a data-driven categorization of cumulative substance use and then used a LMM approach to evaluate the longitudinal effects of both alcohol and marijuana use on individual college GPA in a cohort of 1000+ college students. However, it is important to note that our study design or analysis was not geared to examine causation. Given the analysis presented in the paper, all we can infer is that academic performance and substance use was significantly associated. Our data-driven analysis revealed that roughly 40% of the students did not use, or consumed very light amounts of alcohol and MJ. Students who consumed moderate-to-large amounts of alcohol but only small-no MJ comprised another 40%, and the rest of the sample was heavy users of both substances. We also found almost no individuals with minimal alcohol and heavy MJ use. This is interesting given the popular belief that MJ-only users are much more common than individuals who use both substances together. However, the data driven patterns of substance use clusters we found was largely consistent with past reported data in college students [22].

The substance-use clusters identified across individuals in our study were relatively stable across semesters. Thus, heavy substance users during freshman year were more likely to maintain the same use pattern throughout the 2 study years. As hypothesized, even after controlling for a variety of demographic and clinical characteristics, cluster 3 students (who were moderate-heavy users of both substances) had poorer grades (from study outset to completion) than their peers who consumed little or no alcohol/marijuana. Overall, cluster 2 students (moderate-high alcohol + low-or-no marijuana) had lower GPA’s compared to their non/low substance use peers. However, contrary to our hypothesis we found no cluster by semester interaction, which suggests that the overall relationship between cluster and GPA is the same across each semester (as seen in Fig 2). Despite a non-significant interaction term, we looked at post-hoc simple effects to better interpret the data. These analyses revealed that the difference in GPA between cluster 1 and 2 diminished to trend-level significance during semester 2 and was non-significant for semesters 3 and 4. One possible explanation is that students consuming moderate-to-high levels of alcohol and only low levels or no marijuana (cluster 2), adapt over time, develop tolerance to substance effects and/or regulate their behavior to eventually catch up academically with their sober peers. Alternatively, it is possible that cluster 2 students chose to switch to less academically challenging courses. We were not able to distinguish among these possibilities.

The above was however not true for students who heavily used both MJ and alcohol (cluster 3). Despite the fact that cluster 3 students had similar SAT scores of academic achievement coming into college, compared to cluster 1 and 2, we noted that by the end of semester 1, they had significantly reduced GPAs. Our results therefore suggest that consuming moderate-to-high levels of both substances were associated with significantly lower GPA, which seemed to follow across subsequent semesters (see Fig 2). However, this level of consumption was not associated with a decline in GPA from semester 1, i.e. when we tracked cluster 3 academic performance over time, there was no significant change in GPA over the two-year period. Although partly consistent with our initial hypothesis, this particular finding was not entirely expected, as we had initially predicted that engaging in heavy substance use of both substances would also result in a faster decline in academic achievement over time (i.e. we would see a significant cluster by semester interaction). One simple possibility is that we had too few time points in the study in later semesters to detect a significant GPA decline over time. Also, larger attrition of subjects across substance use clusters over time could have resulted in a non-significant decline of GPA as well. Finally, it could be possible that heavy substance users are able to tolerate deleterious effects of those substances, due to perhaps a combination of genetic predisposition and adaptive cellular or systemic tolerance, thus preventing them from a continuing decline in academic performance. This hypothesis is supported in-part by evidence from both the animal and human literature. A rodent study demonstrated that exposures to large quantities of alcohol led to functional tolerance independent of environmental influences [44]. In humans, a study investigating alcohol tolerance in offspring of alcoholics and non-alcoholics, found that the former experience alcohol’s pleasurable effects at the start of drinking and are less affected by its subsequent impairing effects during the drinking session, that in turn might contribute to increased drinking behaviors [45, 46]. Similar animal-based evidence for tolerance effects at both the observational and neuronal levels has been noted for heavy marijuana use [47]. It is possible that a large number of students belonging to cluster 3 fit the above pattern and were thus able to maintain their grades over time due to adaptive tolerance to heavy substance use.

A key strength of the current study is its focus on a critical age group, college-attending late adolescents to early adults, when substance use and initiation is prevalent. Animal studies suggest that compared to adults, adolescents may be particularly sensitive and vulnerable to neurotoxic effects of substances including alcohol and marijuana [4850]. Preclinical studies also report increased cellular changes in adolescents compared to adults associated with exposure to THC (delta-9- tetrahydrocannabinol), a major psychoactive compound in marijuana [48, 49]. Human studies indicate that regular marijuana use starting before age 18 is associated with deficits in attention, visual search, executive functioning and IQ [7, 9, 1618]. Newer research seems to indicate that persistent cannabis use across four years of high school is associated with lower GPA, SAT scores and higher externalizing symptoms [51]. A recently-published large longitudinal cohort study that utilized trajectory analyses, seems to indicate that students who smoked cannabis (irrespective of use being infrequent, frequent, increasing or decreasing) during college had significantly lower college GPA and other related academic outcomes (even at study onset) [52]. Another study used structural equation modeling to conclude that MJ use during the first semester was directly related to lower GPA in college students. The study also found similar indirect effects of alcohol and other substances on academic outcomes such as GPA and graduation time [53]. Our results both confirm and extend previous reports by suggesting that students who consume moderate-large quantities of both alcohol and marijuana show significantly lowered college academic performance that is seen consistently over a two-year period. It was also encouraging to observe that students who in general were able to moderate or lower substance use patterns while in college over time performed significantly better academically compared to students who remained in the same substance use cluster. Our pseudo-trajectory analysis might therefore provide important clues in terms of how regulating, curtailing and modifying substance use behavior while in college could have a significant impact on academic GPA.

Our results suggest that early intervention to substance abuse in college students might be vital in shaping their academic success over time. Students who use alcohol and MJ (especially MJ) minimally during their incoming semester seem to do better academically over time. This might in-turn lead to significantly better life outcomes upon graduation. It was interesting to note that despite no differences in SAT scores between clusters, we begin to see a dramatic difference in GPA associated with heavy substance use by the end of the first semester. First-year students typically have exaggerated ideas about how much college students drink. Wanting to fit in and being free of parental control for the first time, these students can be led by this misperception into a pattern of heavy drinking that increases their risk of academic failure, serious injury, sexual assault, and even death. As stated by Moos et al 1976, “College health and academic counseling services may benefit from knowledge of individuals at high risk for poor physical health, academic dropout, and alcohol abuse.” This could translate into identification of heavy student alcohol and substance users through improved substance use education and readily available counseling programs, with the aim of reducing substance-associated morbidity. It is typical for freshman students to be subjected to peer pressure of drinking given the somewhat false sense of excessive drinking and substance use culture in colleges. One way to counteract this would be to roll out social norms marketing campaigns that try to convey accurate survey data about student drinking norms. The idea is that once students learn that far fewer students are drinking heavily than they once thought, they will feel less social pressure to drink and, therefore, moderate their alcohol use. In addition to enforcing strict rules and disseminating knowledge on alcohol and substance abuse during social events especially during the freshman year, schools could also take a more proactive role in creating a substance-free environment by promoting alcohol-free events for students and supporting student clubs and organizations that are substance-free and create and promote service learning and volunteer opportunities that are an integral part of the academic curriculum.

As mentioned previously mental illness is an important piece of the picture, given that psychiatric disorders and substance abuse are often co-morbid; we tried to control for these important factors. However, these measures were only collected at study onset, future studies might therefore benefit by collecting history of tobacco use, depression and anxiety measures longitudinally to better account for variability in these potentially important mediating factors. In the current study however, exploratory analyses performed within the LMM framework found no interaction between depression or anxiety scores with cluster definition.

Other strengths of the current study included being among the first to assess simultaneous longitudinal influences of both alcohol and marijuana on academic performance. Further, we controlled for a variety of demographic and clinical variables that might otherwise be comorbid with substance use. The study also had several limitations. We were not able to detect a meaningful group of individuals with low alcohol and moderate-high marijuana use, which would have allowed us to better parse effects of substance use on academic performance. Unfortunately we did not capture a variety of other interesting mediating factors such as “pre-college” drinking or marijuana use patterns, high school GPA, skipping classes, course difficulty or degree of college engagement which could have enabled us to draw better inferences. There was a significant degree of non-reporting of marijuana use in later semesters, in subjects who volunteered other types of data; we do not have a ready explanation for the specific failure to report these substance data. However, given that missingness seemed to increase from cluster 1 to cluster 3 in a step-wise manner, one might speculate that participants who frequently abused substances were less motivated to provide survey data as time progressed. The above missing data was also significantly different across substance use clusters which could have possibly impacted our findings.

Conclusions

Overall, our study adds to the growing body of adolescent/early adulthood substance use literature by showing that combined alcohol and marijuana effects can pose a threat to college GPA. Although we detected significant differences in academic GPA across different substance use groups, the association between substance use (clusters) and GPA did not change over the 2-year follow up period. Students consuming moderate-heavy alcohol and little-no marijuana seemed to show significantly lower GPA only at study onset. However the impact on GPA in students who consumed moderate-heavy doses of both alcohol and marijuana seemed to be more severe as these students had a significantly lowered GPA at both outset and throughout the 2-year study period, suggesting serious implications towards academic achievement when both substances are prevalently used while attending college. Supplementary results also suggest that moderating or curtailing substance use while in college might help improving academic GPA.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Frequency and missingness of key variables included in the linear mixed model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Alcohol and marijuana use across each semester.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172213.s002

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible by grant support from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA016599 and AA19036, Pearlson).

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: HT KH SR RMW CSA CF SAM GDP.
  2. Data curation: SAM RVG BP.
  3. Formal analysis: SAM RVG BP.
  4. Funding acquisition: GDP.
  5. Investigation: RRR FA SL KH SR RMW CSA AD CF GDP.
  6. Methodology: HT KH SR RMW CSA CF SAM GDP.
  7. Project administration: GDP CSA SR KH.
  8. Resources: RRR FA SL KH SR RMW CSA AD CF GDP.
  9. Software: SAM RVG BP.
  10. Supervision: GDP CSA SR KH.
  11. Validation: HT KH SR RMW CSA CF SAM GDP.
  12. Visualization: SAM.
  13. Writing – original draft: SAM RVG BP HT GDP.
  14. Writing – review & editing: SAM RVG BP HT GDP KH SR RMR CSA AD.

References

  1. 1. Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. J Stud Alcohol Drugs Suppl. 2009;(16):12–20. Epub 2009/06/30. pmid:19538908
  2. 2. Thombs DL, Olds RS, Bondy SJ, Winchell J, Baliunas D, Rehm J. Undergraduate drinking and academic performance: a prospective investigation with objective measures. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009;70(5):776–85. Epub 2009/09/10. pmid:19737503
  3. 3. Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Maenner G, Gledhill-Hoyt J, Lee H. Changes in binge drinking and related problems among American college students between 1993 and 1997. Results of the Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study. J Am Coll Health. 1998;47(2):57–68. Epub 1998/10/23. pmid:9782661
  4. 4. Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future: National survey results on drug use, 1975–2011. Volume II: College students and adults ages 19–50 Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan. 2012.
  5. 5. Agosti V, Nunes E, Levin F. Rates of psychiatric comorbidity among U.S. residents with lifetime cannabis dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2002;28(4):643–52. Epub 2002/12/21. pmid:12492261
  6. 6. Hall W, Degenhardt L. Adverse health effects of non-medical cannabis use. Lancet. 2009;374(9698):1383–91. Epub 2009/10/20. pmid:19837255
  7. 7. Pope HG Jr., Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Cohane G, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Early-onset cannabis use and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of the association? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;69(3):303–10. Epub 2003/03/14. pmid:12633916
  8. 8. Schweinsburg AD, Brown SA, Tapert SF. The influence of marijuana use on neurocognitive functioning in adolescents. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2008;1(1):99–111. Epub 2008/01/01. pmid:19630709
  9. 9. Solowij N, Jones KA, Rozman ME, Davis SM, Ciarrochi J, Heaven PC, et al. Verbal learning and memory in adolescent cannabis users, alcohol users and non-users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;216(1):131–44. Epub 2011/02/18.
  10. 10. Takagi M, Yucel M, Cotton SM, Baliz Y, Tucker A, Elkins K, et al. Verbal memory, learning, and executive functioning among adolescent inhalant and cannabis users. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72(1):96–105. Epub 2010/12/09. pmid:21138716
  11. 11. White AM, Swartzwelder HS. Age-related effects of alcohol on memory and memory-related brain function in adolescents and adults. Recent Dev Alcohol. 2005;17:161–76. Epub 2005/03/26. pmid:15789865
  12. 12. Zeigler DW, Wang CC, Yoast RA, Dickinson BD, McCaffree MA, Robinowitz CB, et al. The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Prev Med. 2005;40(1):23–32. Epub 2004/11/09. pmid:15530577
  13. 13. Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi HY. Early adolescent patterns of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana polysubstance use and young adult substance use outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;136:51–62. Epub 2014/01/18. pmid:24434016
  14. 14. Jacobus J, Squeglia LM, Meruelo AD, Castro N, Brumback T, Giedd JN, et al. Cortical thickness in adolescent marijuana and alcohol users: A three-year prospective study from adolescence to young adulthood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;16:101–9. Epub 2015/05/09. pmid:25953106
  15. 15. Jacobus J, Squeglia LM, Infante MA, Castro N, Brumback T, Meruelo AD, et al. Neuropsychological performance in adolescent marijuana users with co-occurring alcohol use: A three-year longitudinal study. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(6):829–43. Epub 2015/05/06. pmid:25938918
  16. 16. Ehrenreich H, Rinn T, Kunert HJ, Moeller MR, Poser W, Schilling L, et al. Specific attentional dysfunction in adults following early start of cannabis use. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;142(3):295–301. Epub 1999/04/20.
  17. 17. Fontes MA, Bolla KI, Cunha PJ, Almeida PP, Jungerman F, Laranjeira RR, et al. Cannabis use before age 15 and subsequent executive functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198(6):442–7. Epub 2011/06/02. pmid:21628706
  18. 18. Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(40):E2657–64. Epub 2012/08/29. pmid:22927402
  19. 19. Arria AM. Nonmedical Use of Prescription Stimulants and Analgesics: Associations with Social and Academic Behaviors among College Students. J Drug Issues. 2008;38(4):1045–60. Epub 2008/10/01. pmid:20414361
  20. 20. Arria AM, Garnier-Dykstra LM, Caldeira KM, Vincent KB, Winick ER, O'Grady KE. Drug use patterns and continuous enrollment in college: results from a longitudinal study. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74(1):71–83. Epub 2012/12/04. pmid:23200152
  21. 21. Arria AM, Wilcox HC, Caldeira KM, Vincent KB, Garnier-Dykstra LM, O'Grady KE. Dispelling the myth of "smart drugs": cannabis and alcohol use problems predict nonmedical use of prescription stimulants for studying. Addict Behav. 2013;38(3):1643–50. Epub 2012/12/21. pmid:23254212
  22. 22. Williams J, Liccardo Pacula R, Chaloupka FJ, Wechsler H. Alcohol and marijuana use among college students: economic complements or substitutes? Health Econ. 2004;13(9):825–43. Epub 2004/09/14. pmid:15362176
  23. 23. Wolaver AM. Effects of heavy drinking in college on study effort, grade point average, and major choice. Contemp Econ Policy. 2002;20(4):415–28.
  24. 24. Singleton RA Jr., Wolfson AR. Alcohol consumption, sleep, and academic performance among college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009;70(3):355–63. Epub 2009/04/18. pmid:19371486
  25. 25. Martinez JA, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Is heavy drinking really associated with attrition from college? The alcohol-attrition paradox. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22(3):450–6. Epub 2008/09/10. pmid:18778140
  26. 26. Frojd SA, Nissinen ES, Pelkonen MU, Marttunen MJ, Koivisto AM, Kaltiala-Heino R. Depression and school performance in middle adolescent boys and girls. J Adolesc. 2008;31(4):485–98. Epub 2007/10/24. pmid:17949806
  27. 27. Humensky J, Kuwabara SA, Fogel J, Wells C, Goodwin B, Van Voorhees BW. Adolescents with depressive symptoms and their challenges with learning in school. J Sch Nurs. 2010;26(5):377–92. Epub 2010/07/08. pmid:20606058
  28. 28. Labrie JW, Migliuri S, Kenney SR, Lac A. Family history of alcohol abuse associated with problematic drinking among college students. Addict Behav. 2010;35(7):721–5. Epub 2010/04/03. pmid:20359831
  29. 29. Reisbig AM, Danielson JA, Wu TF, Hafen M Jr., Krienert A, Girard D, et al. A study of depression and anxiety, general health, and academic performance in three cohorts of veterinary medical students across the first three semesters of veterinary school. J Vet Med Educ. 2012;39(4):341–58. Epub 2012/11/29. pmid:23187027
  30. 30. Dager AD, Anderson BM, Stevens MC, Pulido C, Rosen R, Jiantonio-Kelly RE, et al. Influence of alcohol use and family history of alcoholism on neural response to alcohol cues in college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37 Suppl 1:E161–71. Epub 2012/10/20.
  31. 31. Frey MC, Detterman DK. Scholastic Assessment or g? The relationship between the Scholastic Assessment Test and general cognitive ability. Psychol Sci. 2004;15(6):373–8. Epub 2004/05/19. pmid:15147489
  32. 32. Hollingshead ADB. Four factor index of social status. New Haven, Conn: Yale University, Dept of Sociology. 1975;(1975:18–4).
  33. 33. Caldeira KM, O'Grady KE, Garnier-Dykstra LM, Vincent KB, Pickworth WB, Arria AM. Cigarette smoking among college students: longitudinal trajectories and health outcomes. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14(7):777–85. Epub 2012/05/17. pmid:22589418
  34. 34. Fagerstrom KO, Schneider NG. Measuring nicotine dependence: a review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. J Behav Med. 1989;12(2):159–82. Epub 1989/04/01. pmid:2668531
  35. 35. Kessler RC. The impairments caused by social phobia in the general population: implications for intervention. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2003;(417):19–27. Epub 2003/09/03. pmid:12950433
  36. 36. Stein MB, Kean YM. Disability and quality of life in social phobia: epidemiologic findings. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(10):1606–13. Epub 2000/09/29. pmid:11007714
  37. 37. Regier DA, Farmer ME, Rae DS, Locke BZ, Keith SJ, Judd LL, et al. Comorbidity of mental disorders with alcohol and other drug abuse. Results from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) Study. JAMA. 1990;264(19):2511–8. Epub 1990/11/21. pmid:2232018
  38. 38. Spielberger CD, Gorssuch RL, Lushene PR, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc. 1983.
  39. 39. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–71. Epub 1961/06/01. pmid:13688369
  40. 40. Bacher J, Wnzig K, Vogler M. SPSS TwoStep Cluster—A First Evaluation. available at http://wwwstatisticalinnovationscom/products/twosteppdf, [accessed on 23 July 2013]. 2013.
  41. 41. Akaike H. [Data analysis by statistical models]. No To Hattatsu. 1992;24(2):127–33. Epub 1992/03/01. pmid:1567644
  42. 42. Carletta J. Assesing agreement on classification tasks: The kappa statistic. Computational Linguistics. 1996;22(2):249–54.
  43. 43. Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH. Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the Archives of General Psychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(3):310–7. Epub 2004/03/03. pmid:14993119
  44. 44. Melchior CL, Tabakoff B. Modification of environmentally cued tolerance to ethanol in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1981;219(1):175–80. Epub 1981/10/01. pmid:7288607
  45. 45. Newlin DB, Thomson JB. Alcohol challenge with sons of alcoholics: a critical review and analysis. Psychol Bull. 1990;108(3):383–402. Epub 1990/11/01. pmid:2270234
  46. 46. Schuckit MA, Gold EO. A simultaneous evaluation of multiple markers of ethanol/placebo challenges in sons of alcoholics and controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45(3):211–6. Epub 1988/03/01. pmid:3422553
  47. 47. Oviedo A, Glowa J, Herkenham M. Chronic cannabinoid administration alters cannabinoid receptor binding in rat brain: a quantitative autoradiographic study. Brain Res. 1993;616(1–2):293–302. Epub 1993/07/09. pmid:8395305
  48. 48. Rubino T, Vigano D, Realini N, Guidali C, Braida D, Capurro V, et al. Chronic delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol during adolescence provokes sex-dependent changes in the emotional profile in adult rats: behavioral and biochemical correlates. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(11):2760–71. Epub 2008/01/04. pmid:18172430
  49. 49. Schneider M, Koch M. Chronic pubertal, but not adult chronic cannabinoid treatment impairs sensorimotor gating, recognition memory, and the performance in a progressive ratio task in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(10):1760–9. Epub 2003/07/31. pmid:12888772
  50. 50. Barron S, White A, Swartzwelder HS, Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Slawecki CJ, et al. Adolescent vulnerabilities to chronic alcohol or nicotine exposure: findings from rodent models. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29(9):1720–5. Epub 2005/10/06. pmid:16205372
  51. 51. Meier MH, Hill ML, Small PJ, Luthar SS. Associations of adolescent cannabis use with academic performance and mental health: A longitudinal study of upper middle class youth. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;156:207–12. Epub 2015/09/28. pmid:26409752
  52. 52. Suerken CK, Reboussin BA, Egan KL, Sutfin EL, Wagoner KG, Spangler J, et al. Marijuana use trajectories and academic outcomes among college students. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;162:137–45. Epub 2016/03/30. pmid:27020322
  53. 53. Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Bugbee BA, Vincent KB, O'Grady KE. The academic consequences of marijuana use during college. Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(3):564–75. Epub 2015/08/04. pmid:26237288