Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 03 November 2021
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.717291

A Review of the Potential of Virtual Walking Techniques for Gait Rehabilitation

Omar Janeh1* Frank Steinicke2
  • 1Department of Computer Engineering, University of Technology, Baghdad, Iraq
  • 2Human-Computer Interaction, Department of Informatics, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany

Virtual reality (VR) technology has emerged as a promising tool for studying and rehabilitating gait disturbances in different cohorts of patients (such as Parkinson's disease, post-stroke, or other neurological disorders) as it allows patients to be engaged in an immersive and artificial environment, which can be designed to address the particular needs of each individual. This review demonstrates the state of the art in applications of virtual walking techniques and related technologies for gait therapy and rehabilitation of people with movement disorders makes recommendations for future research and discusses the use of VR in the clinic. However, the potential for using these techniques in gait rehabilitation is to provide a more personalized approach by simulate the experience of natural walking, while patients with neurological disorders are maintained localized in the real world. The goal of our work is to investigate how the human nervous system controls movement in health and neurodegenerative disease.

1. Introduction

The emergence of virtual reality (VR) as a therapeutic tool has provided important insights for developing potential movement therapies for patients with neurological conditions, such as Parkinson's disease (Lei et al., 2019), stroke (Huygelier et al., 2021) or other nervous system iseases (Liu et al., 2018). Although, its research in rehabilitation is becoming more widespread as technology becomes more accessible and affordable, the utilization of VR is not yet regularly used in clinical rehabilitation settings. However, VR provides a novel platform for the development of unique and customizable interventions, which enables new interventions by manipulating training duration or intensity as well as multi-sensory feedback to satisfy clinical demands for intensive and repetitive patients training (Deutsch and Mirelman, 2007; Kiefer et al., 2013), and increase their interest in the rehabilitation process by letting patients experience immersion [e.g., using head mounted displays (HMD)] or non-immersion (e.g., using 2D displays with a limited field of view) virtual environments (VEs), so that patients' treatment compliance is effectively improved (Peñasco-Martín et al., 2010; Gallagher et al., 2016). Thus, more immersive displays have a higher opportunity to present a fully artificial digital environment that results in a high sense of presence (Milgram and Kishino, 1994). Slater (2003) has defined presence as the feeling of being in an environment even when the person is not physically present and leading to behavior that resembles the subject's situation in the environment.

Rehabilitation interventions in VEs can manipulate practice conditions to engage motivation, motor control, cognitive processes and sensory feedback-based learning mechanisms (Levin et al., 2015). Porras et al. (2018) suggested that implementation of patient-tailored motor learning strategies into the design and planning of VR interventions may enhance the efficiency and improve the therapeutic outcome. To this end, the general principles of motor learning can be well applied and integrated in VR training by providing goal-oriented, repetitive and varied practice that is adjusted to the abilities of the user (Deutsch and Mirelman, 2007; Langhorne et al., 2011). Therefore, when developing VR interventions, it is important to consider both the construction of the VE and the interfaces for measurement and feedback that accompany them (Weiss et al., 2006). Together, novel forms of therapeutic interventions can be used to evaluate and treat specific aspects of the human gait (Martens et al., 2017). Recent research has increasingly focused on the use of VR in rehabilitation, including to enhance walking (Mirelman et al., 2011, 2013; de Rooij et al., 2016, 2019).

Virtual walking (i.e., based on real walking) is considered the most intuitive way of navigation in VEs and is also found to be more presence-enhancing compared to other navigation techniques (Usoh et al., 1999). Furthermore, it is proven to be superior over other techniques across users' navigational tasks (Ruddle and Lessels, 2009), cognitive map buildings (Ruddle et al., 2011), and cognitive demands (Marsh et al., 2013). Therefore, a variety of virtual walking techniques have been proposed (see section 3), including walking in place (Slater et al., 1995a,b), redirected walking (Razzaque et al., 2001) or omnidirectional treadmill (Darken et al., 1997). However, there is an increased number of recent studies that use virtual walking techniques for medical and rehabilitative purposes, which provide insights into the future of gait rehabilitation in VR. For instance, Martelli et al. (2018), Janeh et al. (2019b), and Rockstroh et al. (2020) have used a real walking technique, which allows the user to walk about the space in a controlled VE. Other researchers have used new technologies of locomotion devices, such as Strider (Freiwald et al., 2020), 360° VR video-based immersive cycling training system (Lee et al., 2021) and KatWalk omnidirectional treadmill (Cherni et al., 2021). In addition, a recent study by Cai et al. (2021) has shown that WIP is feasible on gait rehabilitation of stroke patients, which translates the viewpoint when the user marches in a stationary location.

The goal of this review is to summarize insights from studies on locomotion techniques in VEs that illuminate the role of movement variability for gait therapy and discuss options for VEs to manipulate task attributes to provide novel forms of feedback and guidance. However, it can inform clinical decision-making and future practice about how to best apply virtual walking techniques in gait rehabilitation, and identify the walking task delivery under the different interface conditions, to demonstrate that the acquired skills from VE practice can be transferred to the real world. We summarize the state of the art of virtual walking techniques for gait rehabilitation in terms of technical, perceptual, cognitive aspects, as well as simulator sickness aspects that must be components of VEs for transfer to occur.

2. Human Gait

Human gait refers to the repetitive locomotion pattern of how a person walks. Although, the process appears automatic and easy, gait is actually a complex and high-level motor function (Mansfield and Neumann, 2009). In order to analyze and evaluate how a person walks, it is necessary to isolate the shortest, unique, repeatable task during gait. This task is called the (bipedal) gait cycle that requires movements from the right and left sides of the body. In normal gait, the average duration of a gait cycle will be very similar for the left and right sides. In pathological (i.e., abnormal) gait, there may be a pronounced difference between the two sides, leading to arrhythmic gait patterns (Uchytil et al., 2017).

2.1. Phases of the Gait Cycle

A gait cycle begins when the heel of one foot touches the ground and ends after the leg and body have advanced through space and time and the heel of that same foot hits the ground again. Realizing aspects of the gait cycle such as phasic, time, spatial and pressure measures, which can be measured an utilized to determine the quality of a person's gait. The cycle includes a period when the leg is in contact with the ground, which is followed by a period when it is advancing through space. Because of the dynamic and continuous nature of walking, the gait cycle is described as occurring between 0 and 100% (Figure 1). It can be distinguished into two primary phases: (i) the stance and (ii) swing phases, which alternate for right and left lower limbs.

Stance phase describes the portion of the gait cycle when the foot is in contact with the ground, which makes up to 60% of the gait cycle. Within a stance phase, the double support represents approximately 20% and single support represents approximately 40% of the gait cycle (Inman et al., 1981). Therefore, when a foot is in a swing phase the other foot should be in a single support phase. When a foot is in a stance phase, it goes through a double support phase 10% of the initial stance phase, a single support phase 40%, and another double support phase 10% of the end of stance.

Double support denotes the amount of time that a participant spends with both feet on the ground during one gait cycle.

Single support describes the time elapsed between the last contact of the current footfall to the first contact of the next footfall of the same foot. It is equivalent to the swing time.

Swing phase is the portion of the gait cycle when one foot is in the air. It is equivalent to the single support time of the opposite foot.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Phases of gait cycle defined for rhythmic natural walking and their proportions as percentages.

The phases of swing and stance are further divided into eight events during the gait cycle (Perry and Davids, 1992); five of which occur in the stance phase, when the foot is on the ground, and three in the swing phase, when the foot is moving forward through the air (Figure 1).

1. Heel contact: the heel or another part of the foot contacts the ground (at 0% of the gait cycle).

2. Foot flat the period that the entire plantar aspect of the foot is on the ground (at 8% of the gait cycle).

3. Mid stance is the point where the body weight passes directly over the supporting lower extremity (at 30% of the gait cycle).

4. Heel off describes the instant the heel leaves the ground (at 40% of the gait cycle).

5. Toe off describes the instant the toe leaves the ground (at 60% of the gait cycle).

6. Pre swing describes the period from toe off to mid swing (at 75% of the gait cycle).

7. Mid swing is the period when the foot of the swing leg passes next to the foot of the stance leg (at 85% of the gait cycle). This corresponds to the mid stance phase of the opposite lower extremity.

8. Late swing the period ranging from mid swing until heel contact (at 100% of the gait cycle).

2.2. Control of the Gait Cycle

Bipedal locomotion is accomplished through a complex and coordinated pattern of nerve signals, sent to the muscles, which in turn move the joints, the limbs and the remainder of the body (Duysens and Van de Crommert, 1998; Whittle, 2014). During walking in the real world, vestibular, proprioceptive, and efferent copy signals, as well as visual information create a consistent multi-sensory representation of a person's self-motion, i.e., acceleration, velocity and walking direction (Dietz, 2002; Takakusaki, 2013). Modifying the sensory information during the movement can come from either proprioceptive information or efference copies of the motor command during the preparation for motor output (Pynn and DeSouza, 2013). The control of locomotion involves the use of afferent information from a variety of sources in the visual, auditory, vestibular and proprioceptive systems (Dietz, 2002). Efference copies are those neural representations of motor outputs that predict reafferent sensory feedback and modulate the response of the corresponding sensory modalities. Also, accessing a copy of the efferent command allows the brain to prepare for the consequences of an intended motion before it has occurred (Harris et al., 2002).

The voluntary control of movement and high-level modulation of gait patterns is originated at the supraspinal level. The latter regulates both the central pattern generator and reflex mechanisms (Dietz, 2002). Also at the supraspinal level, information from vestibular and visual systems are incorporated, which are crucial for the maintenance of balance, orientation, and control of precise movement (Dietz, 2002). Efferent stimulation is transmitted through motor neurons to individual muscle groups, which are recruited to affect the movement. Afferent feedback, including that from proprioceptors of the muscles and joints and mechanoreceptors of the skin, is used to directly modulate motor commands via mono- and polysynaptic reflex arcs, thus contributing to the efficiency of gait under normal conditions and stability of gait in the face of unexpected perturbations (Tucker et al., 2015).

2.3. Gait in Older Adults

The gait of the older adults is subject to two influences (Whittle, 2014): the effects of age itself and the effects of pathological conditions, such as osteoarthritis and parkinsonism, which become more common with advancing age. The gait of the older adults appears to be simply a slowed down version of the gait of younger adults. Furthermore, the differences between the gait of the younger and the older adults are described by Murray et al. (1969), which suggested that the purpose of gait changes in the elderly is characterized by a cautious attitude of walking, which is essentially an exaggeration of the gait changes which normally occur with age. For instance, decreasing the step length and increasing the step width make it easier to maintain balance while walking. Increasing the cycle time leads to a reduction in the percentage of the gait cycle for which there is only single support, since the increase in cycle length is largely achieved by lengthening the stance phase and hence the double support time. A comprehensive review of the changes in gait with advancing age was given by Prince et al. (1997).

Given biological aspects of walking, gait performance is determined by continuous, ongoing postural adjustments by several types of control mechanisms. Stereotypical patterns of synergic muscle group activation (Diener et al., 1988) need to be scaled appropriately by peripheral sensory feedback (Diener et al., 1988) and centrally generated, anticipatory motor programs (Horak and Macpherson, 1996). It has proposed that postural alignment requires three different processes (Horak et al., 1992): (i) sensory organization and weighting of the orientation senses such as somatosensory proprioceptive, visual and vestibular information, (ii) motor adjustment processes involved with executing coordinated and properly scaled neuromuscular responses and (iii) background tone of muscles through which balance changes are compensated. The process of sensory organization seems to be hierarchically organized at different levels, these systems should be coherent, any conflicting orientation inputs must be quickly suppressed in favor of those congruent with the internal reference, otherwise postural and gait performance worsens (Massion, 1998; Mergner and Rosemeier, 1998).

3. Virtual Walking Techniques

As in the real world, most immersive virtual environments are usually suitable to be explored by walking. However, allowing VR users unconstrained walking requires huge free-space areas in which the movements of the user can be tracked. In particular, in a VE the space may have infinite size and the user should be able to walk and explore that space freely. However, in real physical spaces users have constrained space. If the virtual space and the real space have similar sizes, a one-to-one mapping can be used for navigation, but if the virtual space is larger than the real space, the users may eventually walk outside the real tracking space. This interrupts the tracking and may breaks in presence and lead to reduce user experience. To overcome this limitation, some techniques have been developed to enable users to explore larger VEs with real walking. In this section, we summarize some of these most fundamental approaches:

3.1. Walking in Place

In walking-in-place (WIP) interfaces, users perform stepping-like movements without forward motion of the body, but a virtual forward motion is induced instead. The diagram in Figure 2 shows the gait cycle of WIP technique; the significant difference is that the single support periods of normal walking are replaced by foot off, maximum height and foot contact. In this technique, users make body gestures similar to real world walking, without actually moving with respect to the physical environment. This way, users can walk virtually and explore a larger virtual environment. Important advantages of WIP technique include: cost effectiveness (Feasel et al., 2008), naturalness (Usoh et al., 1999), stronger feeling of presence and easier to learn compared to other approaches (Slater et al., 1995b; Templeman et al., 1999), and proprioceptive feedback similar to real walking (Slater et al., 1994). However, since displacement in the real world is prevented with WIP technique, vestibular feedback as in real walking is not possible. One of the first scientific implementations of the walking in place technique was published by Slater et al. (1995b) and Slater et al. (1995a). In that work, head movements were analyzed while performing WIP gesture Figure 4A, and virtual walking was triggered by the movement of the head. The latency was substantial; the system required four steps in place to start the virtual walking, since false-positive steps (moving viewpoint when the user is not walking in place) were considered more confusing than a late start. Similarly, the system looked for no steps for two cycles to stop the virtual walking. Since then, different aspects of the walking in place technique have been examined, such as step detection, start and stop latency (Feasel et al., 2008), and smooth motion (Whitton and Peck, 2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Phases of gait cycle defined for rhythmic walking in place and their proportions as percentages.

Wendt et al. (2010) proposed system used a biomechanical state machine to control the virtual walking, and found more consistent output speeds compared to a study by Feasel et al. (2008). A similar study by Kim et al. (2012) have proposed a technique that triggers WIP technique using the inertial sensors embedded within two smart phones attached to the user's ankles in order to track leg movement in real time. Usually, most WIP techniques rely on gestures for walking input and control, for instance, a so-called stepping gesture, similar to soldiers marching in place. Nilsson et al. (2013a) performed a study comparing this gesture to two alternative gestural inputs: (i) a gesture where the user alternately bends each knee, thus moving the lower leg backwards, and (ii) a gesture where the user in turn taps each heel against the ground without breaking contact with the toes. Furthermore, the perceived required physical effort for the tapping gesture (Figure 4B) was closer to real walking. In another study by Nilsson et al. (2013b), some of those authors examined two more input gestures (i.e., hip movement and arm swinging). The results showed that arm swinging (Figure 4C) was perceived as natural as the original WIP technique. Moreover, Langbehn et al. (2015) have proposed WIP technique (Figure 4D) that involves a novel way of scaling the speed derived from the steps in place (i.e., the user is able to increase the speed by leaning the torso forward).

3.2. Redirected Walking

Redirected Walking (RDW) enables users to explore a virtual world that is considerably larger than the real world (Steinicke et al., 2009b). The idea is that users walk on different paths in the real world, which may vary from the paths they perceive in the VE (Bruder et al., 2013; Vasylevska and Kaufmann, 2017; Nilsson et al., 2018). For instance, using curvature gains the user effectively starts walking in small circles in the physical space while having the illusion of being able to walk straight in the VE (Razzaque et al., 2001). More particularly, (Figure 3) illustrates redirection of gait in a VE where the change of direction (i.e., redirected leg) is opposite to the contact leg, such as turning left while the right leg is in contact with the ground (Hase and Stein, 1999). This turning strategy is very similar to the one used in normal straight walking and tends to enlarge the step width, which minimizes the risk of falling, maximizes the possibility of fast change of directions, and ensures continuity of the walking path (Patla et al., 1999). However, redirection causes a sensory mismatch between the visual and bodily feedback elicited by the rotating VE during walking (Rothacher et al., 2018). It is found that, when only visual input is supplied, people can successfully estimate the amount of change in direction but not the path they followed (Lappe et al., 1999). This makes it possible to manipulate the visual flow to keep the users in the tracking area without being able to notice the manipulations if a physical space of at least 45m2 is available (Steinicke et al., 2009b). These experiments have been replicated with different settings and extended several times (Kopper et al., 2011; Bruder et al., 2012; Freitag et al., 2016). For instance, Grechkin et al. (2016) found that an area of approximately 25m2 can be sufficient for unlimited straight walking in a VE.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Phases of gait cycle defined for redirected walking and their proportions as percentages.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Images for WIP technique gestures: (A) traditional WIP, (B) tapping in place (C) arm swinging, and (D) forward leaning. Arrows illustrate the movement of body parts used to perform the gesture.

However, with RDW techniques large-scale VEs can be explored within a smaller tracking area. There are some variations of RDW techniques, and different taxonomies have been proposed. Steinicke et al. (2009b) proposed a classification based on the types of gains applied: translation (Figure 5A), rotation (Figure 5B) or curvature (Figure 5C). Suma et al. (2012) proposed a different classification based on the geometric flexibility, the detectability of the technique and the continuity. In this taxonomy, the repositioning and reorientation techniques can either be overt or subtle according to the detectability, and either continuous or discrete according to the gain application. Bruder et al. (2012) examined the limits of the gains for individuals using an electric wheelchair controlled by a joystick. The possible range for the gain values was found to be larger for such redirected driving. Recent work by Zhang et al. (2018) has examined motion detection thresholds in a large VE for the purposes of improving a 360° camera telepresence robot by real walking. They found that participants could not discriminate between real and telepresence movements (i.e., translation and rotation) when translation gains are down-scaled by 6% and up-scaled by 10%, and rotation gains are about 12% less or 9% more than the actual physical rotations. This indicates that observers in this particular setup were indeed sensitive to motion discrepancies.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Images for RDW technique manipulations: (A) translation gain, (B) rotation gain (C) curvature gain, and (D) bending gain. Lines indicate the real and virtual transformations.

Redirection algorithms can also be altered to involve passive haptic feedback objects (Steinicke et al., 2008, 2009a). A proxy object in the real environment representing virtual objects with similar size, shape and surface structure can support passive haptic feedback to the users. Although more difficult to utilize, such passive haptic feedback improve the VR experience significantly (Insko et al., 2001). Other RDW techniques use a visuo-haptic interaction to modify the human spatial perception, such as Suma et al. (2011) and Matsumoto et al. (2016), to provide a sensation of walking in unlimited VR space in spite of walking in a limited real space. In these systems, since the users actually move their bodies in space, both motor commands and proprioceptive as well as vestibular information from the body movements can be utilized. Another technique for exploring architectural 3D models scales the virtual room to fit into the real room, so that users can feel the real walls when they reach to the virtual walls (Bruder et al., 2009). In this study, an intense redirection was used to force users go through a virtual door in a virtual wall, so that they did not collide with the real walls.

Recently, novel RDW techniques consider perceptual masking effects like saccades, blinks, and other perceptual suppressions. In this context, Sun et al. (2018) enhance redirected interaction by detecting saccades and amplifying redirection during the events without introducing virtual scene warping. Another work by Langbehn et al. (2018) conducts perceptual experiments to measure translation and rotation thresholds during eye blinks to facilitate RDW.

3.3. Locomotion Devices

Treadmills are allowing navigation of large-scale VEs via walking movements made within a limited space. While it is supposed to biomechanically identical to normal walking, it alters users' perception of motion due to missing vestibular feedback and alters the user's gait cycle (Durgin et al., 2007). Seminal work in this field was reported by the Walkthrough project (Brooks, 1987), which supported unidirectional movement, and the user could rotate by using a steering bar similar to a bicycle. It allows for walking in one direction, but severely restricting the possibilities for navigation through VEs (Souman et al., 2010). Three generations of locomotion devices were developed for the U.S. Army's Dismounted Infantry Training Program (Darken et al., 1997). The Uniport was the first seated treadmill (Figure 6A) built for lower body locomotion and exertion, which did not feel natural and did not allow for making sidesteps. The second, Treadport is based on a standard unidirectional treadmill (Figure 6B) with the user being monitored and constrained from behind via a mechanical attachment to the user's waist. It was better compared to the first generation in which allowed for more natural locomotion, but was still limited to one direction of movement. The third generation system was the omnidirectional treadmill (Figure 6C) that enables locomotion in any direction of travel. The system consisted of 2D rotary motors that moved the treadmill belts to keep the user in the same place. The study showed that accurate user tracking and precise control over the speed of the belts were critical for usability of the system. Otherwise users experienced uncomfortable sudden movements. A similar system was developed in later studies and compared a 3DOF motion platform with controller-based locomotion (Darken et al., 1997; Iwata, 1999). In more recent studies, an improved omnidirectional treadmill so-called CyberWalk was compared with real walking (Schwaiger et al., 2007; Souman et al., 2011), which allows for natural walking in any direction through arbitrarily large-scale VEs. The CyberWalk needed to ideally be large enough to accommodate a gradual accelerations on the motion platform to keep the user at its center. Although the system was found to be effective in locomotion in VEs, it is extremely expensive to maintain and difficult to adjust in the real space (Frissen et al., 2013).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Images for treadmill locomotion interfaces: (A) seated, (B) unidirectional, and (C) omnidirectional treadmills.

Furthermore, there are some atypical approaches to locomotion in this category. One of these studies was so-called Cybersphere (Fernandes et al., 2003). The authors used a large sphere in which the user could walk, run, jump or crawl freely in any direction to explore an infinite VE. Another similar product, which was commercialized, is called VirtuSphere (Medina et al., 2008). The VirtuSphere was designed to work with HMDs that enables users to walk in all directions by placing them inside a large, rotatable, hollow sphere. Due to the sphere having its own large mass, it will not stop, start, or change directions with a high degree of responsiveness, and users must essentially re-calibrate their movements to adjust for the movement of the surface under their feet. Another interesting approach to locomotion in VEs called String Walker (Iwata et al., 2007). In this approach, each foot was attached to four motor pulleys with strings. Once a forward motion was detected, the strings pulled the user to the center. This information was gathered with a touch sensor placed on each foot. It detected stance phase and swing phase of walking. The tension was only applied when the foot was on the ground. The motor-pulley mechanisms are mounted on a turntable driven by a motor when the walker changes direction of walking, the turntable is activated to follow the direction of the walker.

3.4. Controller-Based Virtual Walking

Manual devices such as joysticks, keyboards and VR controllers are widely available, which allow to perform walking inside the VE by involve user's hands and arms (Darken and Sibert, 1996; Marchal et al., 2011). Such joystick-based walking was compared with real walking using different display types (CAVE vs. HMD) (Grechkin et al., 2014). In this study, users performed perceptual-motor coordination tasks with different locomotion techniques. The results show that different velocity controls of each locomotion technique affect the timing and success rate of actions. In real walking, the speed can be controlled easily whereas with a joystick an almost constant speed is provided. Another study by Peck et al. (2011b) and Peck et al. (2011a) compared joysticks with other locomotion techniques in a virtual maze environment. They found that participants, who used joystick-based walking performed significantly worse than participants who used RDW or WIP. Furthermore, joystick- and keyboard-like devices were inferior for controlling spatial orientation compared to RDW techniques (Ruddle and Lessels, 2006). Riecke et al. (2010) compared real walking and joystick locomotion with an additional alternative of real rotation with joystick-based walking. They found that combining real rotation with joystick-based walking produce similar task performance scores as real walking. The results show that large tracked areas are not required for reasonable navigation performance in VR. On the other hand, Nabiyouni et al. (2015) compared joystick to a real walking and VirtuSphere; joystick received better results than VirtuSphere in terms of fatigue, ease of learning, ease of walking and precision. The authors concluded that well designed low fidelity locomotion techniques such as joysticks often give better results compared to designs with moderate interaction fidelity like VirtuSphere.

Alternative locomotion techniques have been developed using VR controllers such as teleportation (Bozgeyikli et al., 2016). With teleportation the user's virtual viewpoint is moved while the user itself stays at the same position and orientation in the physical space. Bolte et al. (2011) developed the so-called jumper metaphor that uses the head direction to select the destination and a physical jump of the user to trigger the teleportation. Another work by Bozgeyikli et al. (2016), utilizes gesture-based interaction to point to where the user wants to go, and the main motion takes place through teleportation. In their work, teleportation was compared to WIP and joystick regarding usability. Results show that teleportation is subjectively preferred as a user friendly locomotion technique. However, an extended version of this teleportation technique for which it was possible to set a certain target direction into which the user should face after the teleportation, showed a decrease of the user experience. Bolte et al. (2011) compared teleportation to real walking and to the jumper metaphor. The result shows that teleportation and jumper metaphor are more effective techniques than real walking. Furthermore, in a CAVE setup, Freitag et al. (2014) compared teleportation to joystick and real walking with portals that were used to reorient the user in the tracking space. Teleportation was faster than real walking, but led to an increased loss of orientation compared to joystick. They could not find any differences between teleportation and real walking concerning motion sickness.

Overall virtual walking techniques may be a practical and useful tool to target sensory and cognitive deficits that contribute to gait impairments, and thus provide new opportunities to improve gait, mobility, and ultimately quality of life in those living with neurological and neurodegenerative diseases. In the following section we will discuss research utilizing virtual reality as a method for therapeutic intervention for gait impairments in different cohorts of neurological patients.

4. Virtual Walking Techniques for Gait Rehabilitation

More recent reviews by Canning et al. (2020), Huygelier et al. (2021), and Keshner and Lamontagne (2021) highlighted the concrete contributions of VR to rehabilitation of balance and gait; suggesting that the most promising effects of VR are the ability to multitask in a VE that can replicate the demands of a physical space. There is indeed already a promising body of evidence for effective virtual walking techniques in populations such as stroke (Mirelman et al., 2010; Cai et al., 2021), multiple sclerosis (Samaraweera et al., 2013; Winter et al., 2021), Parkinson's disease (Janeh et al., 2019a; Quek et al., 2021), and Alzheimer's disease (White and Moussavi, 2016). In this section, we summarize the different VR-based gait rehabilitation approaches:

4.1. Treadmills

A commonly used virtual walking technique for gait rehabilitation is unidirectional treadmills (Yang et al., 2008; Mirelman et al., 2011; Peruzzi et al., 2017; Richards et al., 2018). Such non-immersive VR-based training hold promise for fully immersive VR, such as using an HMD in combination with treadmill walking (Luque-Moreno et al., 2015; Roeles et al., 2018), which provided motor cognitive challenges in a simulated, real life but safe environment, compared with the same dose of treadmill training alone (Canning et al., 2020). To date, a small number of studies have investigated gait training using an HMD (Parijat et al., 2015; Peterson et al., 2018; Chan et al., 2019), and a recent study showed that both young and older adults were able to use HMD during walking without adverse effects (Kim et al., 2017). More recently, research groups have also investigated the use of omnidirectional treadmills to walk through virtual environments (Lamontagne et al., 2019; Soni and Lamontagne, 2020), which allow changes in direction while accommodating gait speed changes that observed during overground locomotion.

In parallel to those clinical investigations, other studies have demonstrated VR foot pedals combined with neuroimaging techniques (functional MRI) or DBS surgery to investigate the pathophysiology underlying gait deficits in Parkinson's disease with freezing of gait; which in turn allowed the patients to navigate forward or turning through the virtual environment (Shine et al., 2013; van der Hoorn et al., 2014; Gilat et al., 2015; Georgiades et al., 2016; Ehgoetz Martens et al., 2018; Matar et al., 2019). Forward progression was only achieved when patients alternately depressed the pedals (i.e., left-right-left). Along the same lines, several studies already adopted a VR cycling training for the motor rehabilitation of old adults or stroke patients (Deutsch et al., 2013; Yin et al., 2016; Pedroli et al., 2018). Although, it seems that treadmills walking may lead to similar kinematic data to ground walking, but further studies will be necessary to ensure that the acquired skills from VE practice can be transferred to the real world (Lohse et al., 2014; de Rooij et al., 2016; Palma et al., 2017; Porras et al., 2018; Levac et al., 2019).

4.2. Virtual Stepping

Among the most promising one that requires bilateral limb coordination, Killane et al. (2015) investigated the effects of the addition of a non-immersive VR component to stepping in place on a balance board with cognitive loading aimed at reducing the number of FoG episodes in PD. These technologies, which allow stepping-in-place on a balance board, have been utilized previously in literature to mimic gait (Nantel et al., 2011). Accordingly, a virtual teacher has effectively instructed while healthy adults were stepping in place (Koritnik et al., 2008, 2010), and others have successfully been applied in rehabilitation (Duschau-Wicke et al., 2009). More recently, there has been an emphasis on using stepping over virtual obstacles placed on the path of walking, either projected onto the floor (Geerse et al., 2018, 2020b) or treadmill (Heeren et al., 2013; van Ooijen et al., 2016) or 3D holographic cues seen through Microsoft HoloLens (Coolen et al., 2020; Geerse et al., 2020a; Miyake et al., 2021), or displayed on the floor of a virtual environment (Gómez-Jordana et al., 2018; Janeh et al., 2019a), which can be used for advance planning and real-time modification of the obstacle avoidance behavior (Edd et al., 2020). As such, the possible applications for this gait retraining paradigm are widespread, especially when combined with measures of gait biomechanics alterations (Sveistrup, 2004; Martens et al., 2014; Cano Porras et al., 2019). It has been previously shown that individuals are able to follow floor-projected foot placement visual cues aimed to modify gait parameters with an accuracy that is sufficient for the most common therapeutic applications (Bennour et al., 2018). Therefore, VR has the potential to present novel classes of stimuli, such as virtual humans and avatars that provide continuous information (Kiefer et al., 2013). It is therefore possible to imagine a number of ways that continuous information about the desired gait pattern could be presented to a patient. Liu et al. (2020) leverage embodiment in a virtual environment to help with rehabilitation from gait asymmetry, allowing the patient to see their own gait. Moreover, studies have shown that WIP is feasible on gait rehabilitation of stroke patients in which intensity, frequency, motion amplitude, and feedback can be manipulated to provide tailored motor training (Cai et al., 2021). Future studies might focus on identifying which control strategies can best facilitate stepping performance in patients at varying degrees of recovery following neurological injury.

4.3. Virtual Manipulations

One of the unique capabilities of VR is that visual information can be enhanced or manipulated during ongoing walking in a manner that is not possible in the real world, e.g., VEs can be used to manipulate visual cues to modulate the gait characteristics of patients with PD that provoke FoG and other impairments contributing to fall risk (Schubert et al., 2005). For instance, Janeh et al. (2019a) found that PD patients overcame the spatial asymmetry and exhibited a comparable step length by enlarging the step length of the short side, an adapted step time, and a swing time variability of both sides during the manipulation of visual-proprioceptive cues. Another example is by Barton et al. (2014) that has investigated the possibility of using the manipulation of visual cues with a time delay in a VE to alter gait using a Virtual Mirror Box. In their study, movements kinematics of the unimpaired leg were combined with the movement timing of the impaired leg to model a realistic avatar with a symmetric gait pattern. In addition, an extensive body of literature has examined the role of visual self-motion in the control of locomotion by selectively manipulating the direction or speed of the visual flow provided through the VE (Lamontagne et al., 2007, 2010). VR can also be used to manipulate the locomotor trajectory of patients during overground walking that varied the path's radius of curvature, to assess the impact of an emulated knee disability on the locomotor trajectory. Gérin-Lajoie et al. (2010). In many studies (Chou et al., 2009; Janeh et al., 2017a,b, 2018), where they manipulated the translation gain of walking in healthy younger and older adults, so that one step forward in the physical world corresponds to several steps forward in the VE. In contrast, Matsumoto et al. (2018) examined the effect of curvature and bending gains (Figure 5d) on walking biomechanics, which occurs when the curvature of the walking path in the VE was manipulated, while the actual walking path remains constant. Therefore, using VR to manipulate visual flow thus has the potential to alter the interaction space and provide notable information about locomotion speed and heading to the patient (Warren et al., 2001; Turano et al., 2005). Walking trajectory was shown to be affected when healthy young subjects were exposed to rotational, translational or a combination of both, demonstrating the importance of visual flow on steering behavior during locomotion (Sarre et al., 2008). Additionally, if the same rotational optic flow is generated via a simulated camera rotation in VE against an actual head rotation, a different locomotor behavior also emerges, whereby the simulated but not the actual head rotation results in a trajectory deviation (Hanna et al., 2017). Such findings support the potential contribution of the motor command in heading estimation (Banks et al., 1996; Crowell et al., 1998). These findings also corroborate the presence of multisensory integration of both visual and non-visual information (i.e., vestibular, proprioceptive, and somatosensory) to generate a single representation of self-motion and orientation in space (Karthik et al., 2014; Acerbi et al., 2018).

4.4. Controllers

Another technique was also employed using controller-based virtual walking, where participants were asked to walk around a VE and remembering objects and rooms that they had viewed in order to estimate cognition (Albani et al., 2002; Klinger et al., 2006; Cipresso et al., 2014). The authors have focused on motor control aspects related to action and navigation as well as performing activities of daily living (i.e., even though they were not actually walking). However, this basic research has implications for practice; suggesting that VEs can be used for the examination of cognitive deficits that may interfere with mobility. Moreover, the use of VR hand-held controllers allows users to interact with virtual elements using their hands as they do real-life, allowing exercise repetition, intensity variation, and task-oriented training (Cortés-Pérez et al., 2020). Although these studies have great potential in improving the assessment of cognition in a more ecological manner, more research studies are needed to know whether this will be useful, reliable, and clinically meaningful. Once this is established it would be useful to use these cognitive tasks to assess and quantify changes in gait in order to understand gait disorders.

5. Conclusion

As such, this review emphasized the importance of employing virtual walking techniques in rehabilitation, and thereby it is a promising approach and possibly effective for improving the gait of people with neurological diseases, suggesting that the severity of the disease can influence the effect of the use of VR during rehabilitation. Moreover, to determine the role of VR-based gait rehabilitation, further research is needed to investigate the characteristics of each patient and his disorder to develop personalized techniques. Thus, potential changes in gait characteristics should be taken into consideration when designing virtual walking techniques.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Acerbi, L., Dokka, K., Angelaki, D. E., and Ma, W. J. (2018). Bayesian comparison of explicit and implicit causal inference strategies in multisensory heading perception. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14:e1006110. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albani, G., Pignatti, R., Bertella, L., Priano, L., Semenza, C., Molinari, E., et al. (2002). Common daily activities in the virtual environment: a preliminary study in parkinsonian patients. Neurol. Sci. 23, s49–s50. doi: 10.1007/s100720200064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., and Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements. Vision Res. 36, 431–443. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00122-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, G. J., De Asha, A. R., van Loon, E. C., Geijtenbeek, T., and Robinson, M. A. (2014). Manipulation of visual biofeedback during gait with a time delayed adaptive virtual mirror box. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennour, S., Ulrich, B., Legrand, T., Jolles, B. M., and Favre, J. (2018). A gait retraining system using augmented-reality to modify footprint parameters: effects on lower-limb sagittal-plane kinematics. J. Biomech. 66, 26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolte, B., Steinicke, F., and Bruder, G. (2011). “The jumper metaphor: an effective navigation technique for immersive display setups,” in Proceedings of Virtual Reality International Conference (Laval), 1–7.

Google Scholar

Bozgeyikli, E., Raij, A., Katkoori, S., and Dubey, R. (2016). “Point &teleport locomotion technique for virtual reality,” in Proceedings of the 2016 Annual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in Play (Austin, TX: ACM), 205–216.

Google Scholar

Brooks, F. P. Jr (1987). “Walkthrough a dynamic graphics system for simulating virtual buildings,” in Proceedings of the 1986 Workshop on Interactive 3D Graphics (Chapel Hill, NC: ACM), 9–21.

Google Scholar

Bruder, G., Interrante, V., Phillips, L., and Steinicke, F. (2012). Redirecting walking and driving for natural navigation in immersive virtual environments. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 18, 538–545. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2012.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G., Steinicke, F., Bolte, B., Wieland, P., Frenz, H., and Lappe, M. (2013). Exploiting perceptual limitations and illusions to support walking through virtual environments in confined physical spaces. Displays 34, 132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.displa.2012.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G., Steinicke, F., and Hinrichs, K. H. (2009). “Arch-explore: a natural user interface for immersive architectural walkthroughs,” in 2009 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (Lafayette, LA: IEEE), 75–82.

Google Scholar

Cai, H., Lin, T., Chen, L., Weng, H., Zhu, R., Chen, Y., et al. (2021). Evaluating the effect of immersive virtual reality technology on gait rehabilitation in stroke patients: a study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 22, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s13063-021-05031-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canning, C. G., Allen, N. E., Nackaerts, E., Paul, S. S., Nieuwboer, A., and Gilat, M. (2020). Virtual reality in research and rehabilitation of gait and balance in parkinson disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 16, 409–425. doi: 10.1038/s41582-020-0370-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cano Porras, D., Sharon, H., Inzelberg, R., Ziv-Ner, Y., Zeilig, G., and Plotnik, M. (2019). Advanced virtual reality-based rehabilitation of balance and gait in clinical practice. Ther. Adv. Chronic Dis. 10:2040622319868379. doi: 10.1177/2040622319868379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, Z. Y., MacPhail, A. J., Au, I. P., Zhang, J. H., Lam, B. M., Ferber, R., et al. (2019). Walking with head-mounted virtual and augmented reality devices: effects on position control and gait biomechanics. PLoS ONE 14:e0225972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherni, H., Nicolas, S., and Metayer, N. (2021). Using virtual reality treadmill as a locomotion technique in a navigation task: impact on user experience-case of the katwalk. Int. J. Virtual Real. 21, 1–14. doi: 10.20870/IJVR.2021.21.1.3046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, Y.-H., Wagenaar, R. C., Saltzman, E., Giphart, J. E., Young, D., Davidsdottir, R., et al. (2009). Effects of optic flow speed and lateral flow asymmetry on locomotion in younger and older adults: a virtual reality study. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64, 222–231. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipresso, P., Albani, G., Serino, S., Pedroli, E., Pallavicini, F., Mauro, A., et al. (2014). Virtual multiple errands test (vmet): a virtual reality-based tool to detect early executive functions deficit in parkinson's disease. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:405. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coolen, B., Beek, P. J., Geerse, D. J., and Roerdink, M. (2020). Avoiding 3d obstacles in mixed reality: does it differ from negotiating real obstacles? Sensors 20, 1095. doi: 10.3390/s20041095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortés-Pérez, I., Nieto-Escamez, F. A., and Obrero-Gaitán, E. (2020). Immersive virtual reality in stroke patients as a new approach for reducing postural disabilities and falls risk: a case series. Brain Sci. 10, 296. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowell, J. A., Banks, M. S., Shenoy, K. V., and Andersen, R. A. (1998). Visual self-motion perception during head turns. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 732–737. doi: 10.1038/3732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darken, R. P., Cockayne, W. R., and Carmein, D. (1997). “The omni-directional treadmill: a locomotion device for virtual worlds,” in Proceedings of the 10th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology (Banff, AB: ACM), 213–221.

Google Scholar

Darken, R. P., and Sibert, J. L. (1996). Navigating large virtual spaces. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 8, 49–71. doi: 10.1080/10447319609526140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Rooij, I. J., van de Port, I. G., and Meijer, J.-W. G. (2016). Effect of virtual reality training on balance and gait ability in patients with stroke: systematic review and meta-analysis. Phys. Ther. 96, 1905–1918. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20160054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Rooij, I. J., van de Port, I. G., Visser-Meily, J. M., and Meijer, J.-W. G. (2019). Virtual reality gait training versus non-virtual reality gait training for improving participation in subacute stroke survivors: study protocol of the virtas randomized controlled trial. Trials 20, 89. doi: 10.1186/s13063-018-3165-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, J., and Mirelman, A. (2007). Virtual reality-based approaches to enable walking for people poststroke. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 14, 45–53. doi: 10.1310/tsr1406-45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, J. E., Myslinski, M. J., Kafri, M., Ranky, R., Sivak, M., Mavroidis, C., et al. (2013). Feasibility of virtual reality augmented cycling for health promotion of people post-stroke. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 37, 118–124. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0b013e3182a0a078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, H., Horak, F., and Nashner, L. (1988). Influence of stimulus parameters on human postural responses. J. Neurophysiol. 59, 1888–1905. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.6.1888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, V. (2002). Proprioception and locomotor disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 781. doi: 10.1038/nrn939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durgin, F. H., Reed, C., and Tigue, C. (2007). Step frequency and perceived self-motion. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 4, 5. doi: 10.1145/1227134.1227139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duschau-Wicke, A., Von Zitzewitz, J., Caprez, A., Lunenburger, L., and Riener, R. (2009). Path control: a method for patient-cooperative robot-aided gait rehabilitation. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 18, 38–48. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2009.2033061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duysens, J., and Van de Crommert, H. W. (1998). Neural control of locomotion; part 1: the central pattern generator from cats to humans. Gait Posture 7, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(97)00042-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edd, S., Martins, N. V., Bennour, S., Ulrich, B., Jolles, B., and Favre, J. (2020). Changes in lower limb biomechanics when following floor-projected foot placement visual cues for gait rehabilitation. Gait Posture 77:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehgoetz Martens, K. A., Hall, J. M., Georgiades, M. J., Gilat, M., Walton, C. C., Matar, E., et al. (2018). The functional network signature of heterogeneity in freezing of gait. Brain 141, 1145–1160. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feasel, J., Whitton, M. C., and Wendt, J. D. (2008). “Llcm-wip: Low-latency, continuous-motion walking-in-place,” in 2008 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (Reno, NV: IEEE), 97–104.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fernandes, K. J., Raja, V., and Eyre, J. (2003). Cybersphere: the fully immersive spherical projection system. Commun. ACM 46, 141–146. doi: 10.1145/903893.903929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitag, S., Rausch, D., and Kuhlen, T. (2014). “Reorientation in virtual environments using interactive portals,” in 2014 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI) (Minneapolis, MN: IEEE), 119–122.

Google Scholar

Freitag, S., Weyers, B., and Kuhlen, T. W. (2016). Examining rotation gain in cave-like virtual environments. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 22, 1462–1471. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2016.2518298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freiwald, J. P., Ariza, O., Janeh, O., and Steinicke, F. (2020). “Walking by cycling: a novel in-place locomotion user interface for seated virtual reality experiences,” in Proceedings of the 2020 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Honolulu, HI), 1–12.

Google Scholar

Frissen, I., Campos, J. L., Sreenivasa, M., and Ernst, M. O. (2013). “Enabling unconstrained omnidirectional walking through virtual environments: an overview of the cyberwalk project,” in Human Walking in Virtual Environments (Springer), 113–144.

Google Scholar

Gallagher, R., Damodaran, H., Werner, W. G., Powell, W., and Deutsch, J. E. (2016). Auditory and visual cueing modulate cycling speed of older adults and persons with parkinson's disease in a virtual cycling (v-cycle) system. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 13, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0184-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerse, D. J., Coolen, B., and Roerdink, M. (2020a). Quantifying spatiotemporal gait parameters with hololens in healthy adults and people with parkinson's disease: Test-retest reliability, concurrent validity, and face validity. Sensors 20, 3216. doi: 10.3390/s20113216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerse, D. J., Roerdink, M., Marinus, J., and van Hilten, J. J. (2018). Assessing walking adaptability in parkinson's disease: “the interactive walkway.” Front. Neurol. 9:1096. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.01096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerse, D. J., Roerdink, M., Marinus, J., and van Hilten, J. J. (2020b). Assessing walking adaptability in stroke patients. Disabil. Rehabil. 1–9. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2020.1731852 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiades, M. J., Gilat, M., Martens, K. A. E., Walton, C. C., Bissett, P. G., Shine, J. M., et al. (2016). Investigating motor initiation and inhibition deficits in patients with parkinson's disease and freezing of gait using a virtual reality paradigm. Neuroscience 337, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gérin-Lajoie, M., Ciombor, D. M., Warren, W. H., and Aaron, R. K. (2010). Using ambulatory virtual environments for the assessment of functional gait impairment: a proof-of-concept study. Gait Posture 31, 533–536. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilat, M., Shine, J. M., Walton, C. C., O'Callaghan, C., Hall, J. M., and Lewis, S. J. (2015). Brain activation underlying turning in parkinson's disease patients with and without freezing of gait: a virtual reality fmri study. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 1, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/npjparkd.2015.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez-Jordana, L. I., Stafford, J., Peper, C. L. E., and Craig, C. M. (2018). Virtual footprints can improve walking performance in people with parkinson's disease. Front. Neurol. 9:681. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grechkin, T., Thomas, J., Azmandian, M., Bolas, M., and Suma, E. (2016). “Revisiting detection thresholds for redirected walking: combining translation and curvature gains,” in Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Applied Perception (Anaheim, CA: ACM), 113–120.

Google Scholar

Grechkin, T. Y., Plumert, J. M., and Kearney, J. K. (2014). Dynamic affordances in embodied interactive systems: the role of display and mode of locomotion. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 20, 596–605. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2014.18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanna, M., Fung, J., and Lamontagne, A. (2017). Multisensory control of a straight locomotor trajectory. J. Vestib. Res. 27, 17–25. doi: 10.3233/VES-170603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, L. R., Jenkin, M. R., Zikovitz, D., Redlick, F., Jaekl, P., Jasiobedzka, U., et al. (2002). Simulating self-motion i: cues for the perception of motion. Virtual Real. 6, 75–85. doi: 10.1007/s100550200008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hase, K., and Stein, R. (1999). Turning strategies during human walking. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2914–2922. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.6.2914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeren, A., van Ooijen, M. W., Geurts, A. C., Day, B. L., Janssen, T. W., Beek, P. J., et al. (2013). Step by step: a proof of concept study of c-mill gait adaptability training in the chronic phase after stroke. J. Rehabil. Med. 45, 616–622. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, F., Nutt, J., and Nashner, L. (1992). Postural inflexibility in parkinsonian subjects. J. Neurol. Sci. 111, 46–58. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(92)90111-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, F. B., and Macpherson, J. M. (1996). Postural orientation and equilibrium. Handbook Physiol. 1, 255–292. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp120107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huygelier, H., Mattheus, E., Abeele, V. V., van Ee, R., and Gillebert, C. R. (2021). The use of the term virtual reality in post-stroke rehabilitation: a scoping review and commentary. Psychol. Belg. 61, 145. doi: 10.5334/pb.1033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inman, V. T., Ralston, H. J., and Todd, F. (1981). Human Walking. Williams & Wilkins.

Insko, B. E., Meehan, M., Whitton, M., and Brooks, F. (2001). Passive haptics significantly enhances virtual environments (Ph.D. thesis). University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Google Scholar

Iwata, H. (1999). The torus treadmill: Realizing locomotion in ves. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 19, 30–35. doi: 10.1109/38.799737

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwata, H., Yano, H., and Tomiyoshi, M. (2007). “String walker,” in ACM SIGGRAPH 2007 Emerging Technologies (ACM), 20.

Google Scholar

Janeh, O., Bruder, G., Steinicke, F., Gulberti, A., and Poetter-Nerger, M. (2018). Analyses of gait parameters of younger and older adults during (non-) isometric virtual walking. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 24, 2663–2674. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2017.2771520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janeh, O., Fründt, O., Schönwald, B., Gulberti, A., Buhmann, C., Gerloff, C., et al. (2019a). Gait training in virtual reality: Short-term effects of different virtual manipulation techniques in parkinson's disease. Cells 8, 419. doi: 10.3390/cells8050419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janeh, O., Katzakis, N., Tong, J., and Steinicke, F. (2019b). “Infinity walk in vr: Effects of cognitive load on velocity during continuous long-distance walking,” in ACM Symposium on Applied Perception 2019 (ACM), 9.

Google Scholar

Janeh, O., Langbehn, E., Steinicke, F., Bruder, G., Gulberti, A., and Poetter-Nerger, M. (2017a). “Biomechanical analysis of (non-) isometric virtual walking of older adults,” in 2017 IEEE Virtual Reality (VR) (Los Angeles, CA: IEEE), 217–218.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Janeh, O., Langbehn, E., Steinicke, F., Bruder, G., Gulberti, A., and Poetter-Nerger, M. (2017b). Walking in virtual reality: effects of manipulated visual self-motion on walking biomechanics. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 14, 12. doi: 10.1145/3022731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karthik, L., Kumar, G., Keswani, T., Bhattacharyya, A., Chandar, S. S., and Rao, K. B. (2014). Protease inhibitors from marine actinobacteria as a potential source for antimalarial compound. PLoS ONE 9:e90972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keshner, E., and Lamontagne, A. (2021). The untapped potential of virtual reality in rehabilitation of balance and gait in neurological disorders. Front. Virtual Real. 2:641650. doi: 10.3389/frvir.2021.641650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, A. W., Rhea, C. K., and Warren, W. H. (2013). “Vr-based assessment and rehabilitation of functional mobility,” in Human Walking in Virtual Environments (Springer), 333–350.

Google Scholar

Killane, I., Fearon, C., Newman, L., McDonnell, C., Waechter, S. M., Sons, K., et al. (2015). Dual motor-cognitive virtual reality training impacts dual-task performance in freezing of gait. IEEE J. Biomed. Health Inform. 19, 1855–1861. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2015.2479625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, A., Darakjian, N., and Finley, J. M. (2017). Walking in fully immersive virtual environments: an evaluation of potential adverse effects in older adults and individuals with parkinson's disease. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14, 16. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0225-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J.-S., Gračanin, D., and Quek, F. (2012). “Sensor-fusion walking-in-place interaction technique using mobile devices,” in 2012 IEEE Virtual Reality Workshops (VRW) (Costa Mesa, CA: IEEE), 39–42.

Google Scholar

Klinger, E., Chemin, I., Lebreton, S., and Marié, R.-M. (2006). Virtual action planning in parkinson's disease: acontrol study. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 342–347. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopper, R., Stinson, C., and Bowman, D. (2011). “Towards an understanding of the effects of amplified head rotations,” in The 3rd IEEE VR Workshop on Perceptual Illusions in Virtual Environments, Vol. 2.

Google Scholar

Koritnik, T., Bajd, T., and Munih, M. (2008). Virtual environment for lower-extremities training. Gait Posture 27, 323–330. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koritnik, T., Koenig, A., Bajd, T., Riener, R., and Munih, M. (2010). Comparison of visual and haptic feedback during training of lower extremities. Gait Posture 32, 540–546. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamontagne, A., Blanchette, A. K., Fung, J., McFadyen, B. J., Sangani, S., Robitaille, N., et al. (2019). “Development of a virtual reality toolkit to enhance community walking after stroke,” in 2019 International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation (ICVR) (Tel Aviv: IEEE), 1–3.

Google Scholar

Lamontagne, A., Fung, J., McFadyen, B., Faubert, J., and Paquette, C. (2010). Stroke affects locomotor steering responses to changing optic flow directions. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 24, 457–468. doi: 10.1177/1545968309355985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamontagne, A., Fung, J., McFadyen, B. J., and Faubert, J. (2007). Modulation of walking speed by changing optic flow in persons with stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 4, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-4-22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langbehn, E., Eichler, T., Ghose, S., von Luck, K., Bruder, G., and Steinicke, F. (2015). “Evaluation of an omnidirectional walking-in-place user interface with virtual locomotion speed scaled by forward leaning angle,” in Proceedings of the GI Workshop on Virtual and Augmented Reality (GI VR/AR), 149–160.

Google Scholar

Langbehn, E., Steinicke, F., Lappe, M., Welch, G. F., and Bruder, G. (2018). In the blink of an eye: leveraging blink-induced suppression for imperceptible position and orientation redirection in virtual reality. ACM Trans. Graph. 37, 66. doi: 10.1145/3197517.3201335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langhorne, P., Bernhardt, J., and Kwakkel, G. (2011). Stroke rehabilitation. Lancet 377, 1693–1702. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60325-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lappe, M., Bremmer, F., and Van den Berg, A. (1999). Perception of self-motion from visual flow. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 329–336. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01364-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N., Choi, W., and Lee, S. (2021). Development of an 360-degree virtual reality video-based immersive cycle training system for physical enhancement in older adults: a feasibility study. BMC Geriatr. 21, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12877-021-02263-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, C., Sunzi, K., Dai, F., Liu, X., Wang, Y., Zhang, B., et al. (2019). Effects of virtual reality rehabilitation training on gait and balance in patients with parkinson's disease: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 14:e0224819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levac, D. E., Huber, M. E., and Sternad, D. (2019). Learning and transfer of complex motor skills in virtual reality: a perspective review. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 16, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/s12984-019-0587-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, M. F., Weiss, P. L., and Keshner, E. A. (2015). Emergence of virtual reality as a tool for upper limb rehabilitation: incorporation of motor control and motor learning principles. Phys. Ther. 95, 415–425. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, B.-B., Ding, Q.-N., and Zhu, W.-S. (2018). Research progress on the role of virtual reality technology in rehabilitation of nervous system diseases. Chin. J. Contemp. Neurol. Neurosurg. 64, 53–63. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1672-6731.2018.03.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Sangani, S., Patterson, K., Fung, J., and Lamontagne, A. (2020). Real-time avatar-based feedback to enhance the symmetry of spatiotemporal parameters after stroke: Instantaneous effects of different avatar views. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 28, 878. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.2979830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohse, K. R., Hilderman, C. G., Cheung, K. L., Tatla, S., and Van der Loos, H. M. (2014). Virtual reality therapy for adults post-stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis exploring virtual environments and commercial games in therapy. PLoS ONE 9:e93318. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque-Moreno, C., Ferragut-Garcías, A., Rodríguez-Blanco, C., Heredia-Rizo, A. M., Oliva-Pascual-Vaca, J., Kiper, P., et al. (2015). A decade of progress using virtual reality for poststroke lower extremity rehabilitation: systematic review of the intervention methods. Biomed. Res. Int. 2015:342529. doi: 10.1155/2015/342529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansfield, P. J., and Neumann, D. A. (2009). Essentials of Kinesiology for the Physical Therapist Assistant. Mosby Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Marchal, M., Pettré, J., and Lécuyer, A. (2011). “Joyman: a human-scale joystick for navigating in virtual worlds,” in 2011 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI) (Singapore: IEEE), 19–26.

Google Scholar

Marsh, W. E., Kelly, J. W., Dark, V. J., and Oliver, J. H. (2013). Cognitive demands of semi-natural virtual locomotion. Presence 22, 216–234. doi: 10.1162/PRES_a_00152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martelli, D., Xia, B., Prado, A., and Agrawal, S. K. (2018). Gait adaptations during overground walking and multidirectional oscillations of the visual field in a virtual reality headset. Gait Posture. 67, 251–256. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, K. A. E., Ellard, C. G., and Almeida, Q. J. (2014). Does anxiety cause freezing of gait in parkinson's disease? PLoS ONE 9:e106561. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, K. A. E., Shine, J. M., and Lewis, S. J. (2017). “Using virtual reality to advance the understanding and rehabilitation of gait impairments in parkinson's disease,” in Locomotion and Posture in Older Adults (Springer), 397–416.

Google Scholar

Massion, J. (1998). Postural control systems in developmental perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 465–472. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00031-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matar, E., Shine, J. M., Gilat, M., Ehgoetz Martens, K. A., Ward, P. B., Frank, M. J., et al. (2019). Identifying the neural correlates of doorway freezing in parkinson's disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2055–2064. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, K., Ban, Y., Narumi, T., Yanase, Y., Tanikawa, T., and Hirose, M. (2016). “Unlimited corridor: redirected walking techniques using visuo haptic interaction,” in ACM SIGGRAPH 2016 Emerging Technologies (Anaheim, CA: ACM), 20.

Matsumoto, K., Yamada, A., Nakamura, A., Uchmura, Y., Kawai, K., and Tanikawa, T. (2018). “Biomechanical parameters under curvature gains and bending gains in redirected walking,” in 2018 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR) (Tuebingen; Reutlingen,: IEEE), 631–632.

Google Scholar

Medina, E., Fruland, R., and Weghorst, S. (2008). “Virtusphere: Walking in a human size vr “hamster ball,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 52 (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications Sage CA), 2102–2106.

Google Scholar

Mergner, T., and Rosemeier, T. (1998). Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and visual signals for postural control and motion perception under terrestrial and microgravity conditions a conceptual model. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 118–135. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00032-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milgram, P., and Kishino, F. (1994). “A taxonomy of mixed reality visual displays,” in IEICE Transactions on Information and Systems, Special issue on Networked Reality.

Google Scholar

Mirelman, A., Maidan, I., Herman, T., Deutsch, J. E., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2011). Virtual reality for gait training: can it induce motor learning to enhance complex walking and reduce fall risk in patients with parkinson's disease? J. Gerontol. A 66, 234–240. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirelman, A., Patritti, B. L., Bonato, P., and Deutsch, J. E. (2010). Effects of virtual reality training on gait biomechanics of individuals post-stroke. Gait Posture 31, 433–437. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirelman, A., Rochester, L., Reelick, M., Nieuwhof, F., Pelosin, E., Abbruzzese, G., et al. (2013). V-time: a treadmill training program augmented by virtual reality to decrease fall risk in older adults: study design of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Neurol. 13:15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, T., Al-Sada, M., Zhong, T., Wang, W., and Sugano, S. (2021). “Feasibility evaluation of mixed reality obstacles on treadmill using hololens to elicit real obstacle negotiation,” in 2021 IEEE/SICE International Symposium on System Integration (SII) (Iwaki; Fukushima: IEEE), 756–761.

Google Scholar

Murray, M. P., Kory, R. C., and Clarkson, B. H. (1969). Walking patterns in healthy old men. J. Gerontol. 24, 169–178. doi: 10.1093/geronj/24.2.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nabiyouni, M., Saktheeswaran, A., Bowman, D. A., and Karanth, A. (2015). “Comparing the performance of natural, semi-natural, and non-natural locomotion techniques in virtual reality,” in 2015 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI) (Arles: IEEE), 3–10.

Google Scholar

Nantel, J., de Solages, C., and Bronte-Stewart, H. (2011). Repetitive stepping in place identifies and measures freezing episodes in subjects with parkinson's disease. Gait Posture 34, 329–333. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, N. C., Peck, T., Bruder, G., Hodgson, E., Serafin, S., Whitton, M., et al. (2018). 15 years of research on redirected walking in immersive virtual environments. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 38, 44–56. doi: 10.1109/MCG.2018.111125628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, N. C., Serafin, S., Laursen, M. H., Pedersen, K. S., Sikström, E., and Nordahl, R. (2013a). “Tapping-in-place: Increasing the naturalness of immersive walking-in-place locomotion through novel gestural input,” in 2013 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI) (Orlando, FL: IEEE), 31–38.

Google Scholar

Nilsson, N. C., Serafin, S., and Nordahl, R. (2013b). “The perceived naturalness of virtual locomotion methods devoid of explicit leg movements,” in Proceedings of Motion on Games (Dublin: ACM), 155–164.

Google Scholar

Palma, G. C. d. S., Freitas, T. B., Bonuzzi, G. M. G., Soares, M. A. A., Leite, P. H. W., Mazzini, N. A., et al. (2017). Effects of virtual reality for stroke individuals based on the international classification of functioning and health: a systematic review. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 24, 269–278. doi: 10.1080/10749357.2016.1250373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parijat, P., Lockhart, T. E., and Liu, J. (2015). Effects of perturbation-based slip training using a virtual reality environment on slip-induced falls. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 43, 958–967. doi: 10.1007/s10439-014-1128-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patla, A., Prentice, S., Rietdyk, S., Allard, F., and Martin, C. (1999). What guides the selection of alternate foot placement during locomotion in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 128, 441–450. doi: 10.1007/s002210050867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, T. C., Fuchs, H., and Whitton, M. C. (2011a). The design and evaluation of a large-scale real-walking locomotion interface. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 18, 1053–1067. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2011.289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, T. C., Fuchs, H., and Whitton, M. C. (2011b). “An evaluation of navigational ability comparing redirected free exploration with distractors to walking-in-place and joystick locomotio interfaces,” in 2011 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference (Singapore: IEEE), 55–62.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pedroli, E., Greci, L., Colombo, D., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Arlati, S., et al. (2018). Characteristics, usability, and users experience of a system combining cognitive and physical therapy in a virtual environment: positive bike. Sensors 18, 2343. doi: 10.3390/s18072343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peñasco-Martín, B., de los Reyes-Guzmán, A., Gil-Agudo, Á., Bernal-Sahún, A., Pérez-Aguilar, B., and de la Peña-González, A. I. (2010). Application of virtual reality in the motor aspects of neurorehabilitation. Rev. Neurol. 51, 481–488. doi: 10.33588/rn.5108.2009665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, J., and Davids, J. R. (1992). Gait analysis: normal and pathological function. J. Pediatr. Orthop. 12, 815. doi: 10.1097/01241398-199211000-00023

CrossRef Full Text

Peruzzi, A., Zarbo, I. R., Cereatti, A., Della Croce, U., and Mirelman, A. (2017). An innovative training program based on virtual reality and treadmill: effects on gait of persons with multiple sclerosis. Disabil. Rehabil. 39, 1557–1563. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2016.1224935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, S. M., Rios, E., and Ferris, D. P. (2018). Transient visual perturbations boost short-term balance learning in virtual reality by modulating electrocortical activity. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 1998–2010. doi: 10.1152/jn.00292.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porras, D. C., Siemonsma, P., Inzelberg, R., Zeilig, G., and Plotnik, M. (2018). Advantages of virtual reality in the rehabilitation of balance and gait: Systematic review. Neurology 90, 1017–1025. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, F., Corriveau, H., Hebert, R., and Winter, D. A. (1997). Gait in the elderly. Gait Posture 5, 128–135. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(97)01118-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pynn, L. K., and DeSouza, J. F. (2013). The function of efference copy signals: implications for symptoms of schizophrenia. Vision Res. 76, 124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quek, D. Y., Economou, K., MacDougall, H., Lewis, S. J., and Ehgoetz Martens, K. A. (2021). Validating a seated virtual reality threat paradigm for inducing anxiety and freezing of gait in parkinson's disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 11, 1443–1454. doi: 10.3233/JPD-212619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Razzaque, S., Kohn, Z., and Whitton, M. C. (2001). Redirected walking. Proc. Eurograph. 9, 105–106. doi: 10.2312/egs.20011036

CrossRef Full Text

Richards, C. L., Malouin, F., Lamontagne, A., McFadyen, B. J., Dumas, F., Comeau, F., et al. (2018). Gait training after stroke on a self-paced treadmill with and without virtual environment scenarios: a proof-of-principle study. Physiother. Can. 70, 221–230. doi: 10.3138/ptc.2016-97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riecke, B. E., Bodenheimer, B., McNamara, T. P., Williams, B., Peng, P., and Feuereissen, D. (2010). “Do we need to walk for effective virtual reality navigation? physical rotations alone may suffice,” in International Conference on Spatial Cognition (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer), 234–247.

Google Scholar

Rockstroh, C., Blum, J., Hardt, V., and Gritz, A. S. (2020). Design and evaluation of a virtual restorative walk with room-scale virtual reality and impossible spaces. Front. Virtual Real. 1:29. doi: 10.3389/frvir.2020.598282

CrossRef Full Text

Roeles, S., Rowe, P., Bruijn, S., Childs, C., Tarfali, G., Steenbrink, F., and Pijnappels, M. (2018). Gait stability in response to platform, belt, and sensory perturbations in young and older adults. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 56, 2325–2335. doi: 10.1007/s11517-018-1855-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothacher, Y., Nguyen, A., Lenggenhager, B., Kunz, A., and Brugger, P. (2018). Visual capture of gait during redirected walking. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36035-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddle, R., and Lessels, S. (2009). The benefits of using a walking interface to navigate virtual environments. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 16, 1–5. doi: 10.1145/1502800.1502805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddle, R. A., and Lessels, S. (2006). For efficient navigational search, humans require full physical movement, but not a rich visual scene. Psychol. Sci. 17, 460–465. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01728.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddle, R. A., Volkova, E., and Bülthoff, H. H. (2011). Walking improves your cognitive map in environments that are large-scale and large in extent. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 18, 10. doi: 10.1145/1970378.1970384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samaraweera, G., Guo, R., and Quarles, J. (2013). “Latency and avatars in virtual environments and the effects on gait for persons with mobility impairments,” in 2013 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI) (Orlando, FL: IEEE), 23–30.

Google Scholar

Sarre, G., Berard, J., Fung, J., and Lamontagne, A. (2008). Steering behaviour can be modulated by different optic flows during walking. Neurosci. Lett. 436, 96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, M., Prokop, T., Brocke, F., and Berger, W. (2005). Visual kinesthesia and locomotion in parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 20, 141–150. doi: 10.1002/mds.20281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwaiger, M., Thuimmel, T., and Ulbrich, H. (2007). “Cyberwalk: an advanced prototype of a belt array platform,” in 2007 IEEE International Workshop on Haptic, Audio and Visual Environments and Games (Ottawa, ON: IEEE), 50–55.

Google Scholar

Shine, J. M., Matar, E., Ward, P. B., Bolitho, S. J., Pearson, M., Naismith, S. L., et al. (2013). Differential neural activation patterns in patients with parkinson's disease and freezing of gait in response to concurrent cognitive and motor load. PLoS ONE 8:e52602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, M. (2003). A note on presence terminology. Presence Connect 3, 1–5.

Google Scholar

Slater, M., Steed, A., and Usoh, M. (1995a). “The virtual treadmill: a naturalistic metaphor for navigation in immersive virtual environments,” in Virtual Environments 95 (Vienna: Springer), 135–148.

Google Scholar

Slater, M., Usoh, M., and Steed, A. (1994). “Steps and ladders in virtual reality,” in Virtual Reality Software and Technology (Singapore: World Scientific), 45–54.

Google Scholar

Slater, M., Usoh, M., and Steed, A. (1995b). Taking steps: the influence of a walking technique on presence in virtual reality. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 2, 201–219. doi: 10.1145/210079.210084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soni, S., and Lamontagne, A. (2020). Characterization of speed adaptation while walking on an omnidirectional treadmill. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 17, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12984-020-00787-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souman, J. L., Giordano, P. R., Frissen, I., Luca, A. D., and Ernst, M. O. (2010). Making virtual walking real: Perceptual evaluation of a new treadmill control algorithm. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 7, 11. doi: 10.1145/1670671.1670675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souman, J. L., Giordano, P. R., Schwaiger, M., Frissen, I., Thümmel, T., Ulbrich, H., et al. (2011). Cyberwalk: Enabling unconstrained omnidirectional walking through virtual environments. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 8, 25. doi: 10.1145/2043603.2043607

CrossRef Full Text

Steinicke, F., Bruder, G., Hinrichs, K., Jerald, J., Frenz, H., and Lappe, M. (2009a). Real walking through virtual environments by redirection techniques. J. Virtual Real. Broadcast. 6. doi: 10.20385/1860-2037/6.2009.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinicke, F., Bruder, G., Jerald, J., Frenz, H., and Lappe, M. (2009b). Estimation of detection thresholds for redirected walking techniques. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 16, 17–27. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2009.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinicke, F., Bruder, G., Kohli, L., Jerald, J., and Hinrichs, K. (2008). “Taxonomy and implementation of redirection techniques for ubiquitous passive haptic feedback,” in 2008 International Conference on Cyberworlds (Hanzhou: IEEE), 217–223.

Google Scholar

Suma, E. A., Bruder, G., Steinicke, F., Krum, D. M., and Bolas, M. (2012). “A taxonomy for deploying redirection techniques in immersive virtual environments,” in Virtual Reality Short Papers and Posters (VRW) (Costa Mesa, CA: IEEE), 43–46.

Google Scholar

Suma, E. A., Clark, S., Krum, D., Finkelstein, S., Bolas, M., and Warte, Z. (2011). “Leveraging change blindness for redirection in virtual environments,” in 2011 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference (Singapore: IEEE), 159–166.

Google Scholar

Sun, Q., Patney, A., Wei, L.-Y., Shapira, O., Lu, J., Asente, P., et al. (2018). Towards virtual reality infinite walking: dynamic saccadic redirection. ACM Trans. Graph. 37, 67. doi: 10.1145/3197517.3201294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sveistrup, H. (2004). Motor rehabilitation using virtual reality. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 1, 1. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-1-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takakusaki, K. (2013). Neurophysiology of gait: from the spinal cord to the frontal lobe. Mov. Disord. 28, 1483–1491. doi: 10.1002/mds.25669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Templeman, J. N., Denbrook, P. S., and Sibert, L. E. (1999). Virtual locomotion: Walking in place through virtual environments. Presence 8, 598–617. doi: 10.1162/105474699566512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, M. R., Olivier, J., Pagel, A., Bleuler, H., Bouri, M., Lambercy, O., et al. (2015). Control strategies for active lower extremity prosthetics and orthotics: a review. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 12, 1. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-12-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turano, K. A., Yu, D., Hao, L., and Hicks, J. C. (2005). Optic-flow and egocentric-direction strategies in walking: central vs peripheral visual field. Vision Res. 45, 3117–3132. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uchytil, J., Jandačka, D., Farana, R., Zahradník, D., Rosicky, J., Janura, M., et al. (2017). Kinematics of gait using bionic and hydraulic knee joints in transfemoral amputees. Acta Gymnica 47, 130–137. doi: 10.5507/ag.2017.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Usoh, M., Arthur, K., Whitton, M. C., Bastos, R., Steed, A., Slater, M., et al. (1999). “Walking > walking-in-place > flying, in virtual environments,” in Proceedings of the Annual Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques (Los Angeles, CA: ACM Press; Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.), 359–364.

Google Scholar

van der Hoorn, A., Renken, R. J., Leenders, K. L., and de Jong, B. M. (2014). Parkinson-related changes of activation in visuomotor brain regions during perceived forward self-motion. PLoS ONE 9:e95861. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ooijen, M. W., Roerdink, M., Trekop, M., Janssen, T. W., and Beek, P. J. (2016). The efficacy of treadmill training with and without projected visual context for improving walking ability and reducing fall incidence and fear of falling in older adults with fall-related hip fracture: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 16:215. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0388-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasylevska, K., and Kaufmann, H. (2017). Compressing VR: Fitting large virtual environments within limited physical space. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 37, 85–91. doi: 10.1109/MCG.2017.3621226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., and Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 213–216. doi: 10.1038/84054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, P. L., Kizony, R., Feintuch, U., and Katz, N. (2006). Virtual reality in neurorehabilitation. Textbook Neural Repair Rehabil. 51, 182–197. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511545078.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, J. D., Whitton, M. C., and Brooks, F. P. (2010). “Gud wip: Gait-understanding-driven walking-in-place,” in 2010 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference (VR) (Boston, MA: IEEE), 51–58.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

White, P. J., and Moussavi, Z. (2016). Neurocognitive treatment for a patient with alzheimer's disease using a virtual reality navigational environment. J. Exp. Neurosci. 10:JEN-S40827. doi: 10.4137/JEN.S40827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittle, M. W. (2014). Gait Analysis: An Introduction. Butterworth-Heinemann.

Google Scholar

Whitton, M. C., and Peck, T. C. (2013). “Stepping-driven locomotion interfaces,” in Human Walking in Virtual Environments (Springer), 241–262

Google Scholar

Winter, C., Kern, F., Gall, D., Latoschik, M. E., Pauli, P., and Käthner, I. (2021). Immersive virtual reality during gait rehabilitation increases walking speed and motivation: a usability evaluation with healthy participants and patients with multiple sclerosis and stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 18, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12984-021-00848-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y.-R., Tsai, M.-P., Chuang, T.-Y., Sung, W.-H., and Wang, R.-Y. (2008). Virtual reality-based training improves community ambulation in individuals with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Gait Posture 28, 201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, C., Hsueh, Y.-H., Yeh, C.-Y., Lo, H.-C., and Lan, Y.-T. (2016). A virtual reality-cycling training system for lower limb balance improvement. Biomed. Res. Int. 2016:9276508. doi: 10.1155/2016/9276508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Langbehn, E., Krupke, D., Katzakis, N., and Steinicke, F. (2018). Detection thresholds for rotation and translation gains in 360 video-based telepresence systems. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 24, 1671–1680. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2018.2793679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual locomotion techniques, virtual reality, gait disorders, therapeutic advances, rehabilitation

Citation: Janeh O and Steinicke F (2021) A Review of the Potential of Virtual Walking Techniques for Gait Rehabilitation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:717291. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.717291

Received: 30 May 2021; Accepted: 06 October 2021;
Published: 03 November 2021.

Edited by:

Ioannis Ugo Isaias, Julius Maximilian University of Worzburg, Germany

Reviewed by:

Kaviraja Udupa, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, India
Augusto Fusco, Fondazione Policlinico Universitario A. Gemelli IRCCS, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Janeh and Steinicke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Omar Janeh, 120049@uotechnology.edu.iq

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download