Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 02 April 2015
Sec. Multiple Sclerosis and Neuroimmunology
This article is part of the Research Topic Plasticity in Multiple Sclerosis: From Molecular to System Level, from Adaptation to Maladaptation View all 13 articles

Cognitive Rehabilitation in Multiple Sclerosis: The Role of Plasticity

  • 1Neuropsychology and Neuroscience Laboratory, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ, USA
  • 2Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, USA
  • 3Department of Neurology and Neurosciences, Rutgers New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, USA

Cognitive deficits are common in multiple sclerosis (MS), documented at many stages of the disease. Both structural and functional neuroimaging have demonstrated a relationship with cognitive abilities in MS. Significant neuroplasticity of cognitive functions in individuals with MS is evident. Homologous region adaptation, local activation expansion, and extra-region recruitment all occur in an effort to maintain cognitive functioning. While much of this neuroplasticity is adaptive, it may also be maladaptive, particularly in individuals that are demonstrating significant cognitive impairment and/or with disease progression. This maladaptive neuroplasticity may come at the cost of other cognitive functions. Studies of cognitive rehabilitation efficacy have also recently applied neuroimaging techniques to establish outcome. Researchers have successfully applied various neuroimaging techniques to study the effects of cognitive rehabilitation in MS including task-based fMRI and resting state functional connectivity across multiple realms of cognition including episodic memory, executive functioning, attention, and processing speed. These studies have demonstrated neuroplasticity in the brains of persons with MS through the documentation of changes at the level of the cerebral substrate from before to after non-invasive, non-pharmacological, behavioral treatment for deficits in cognition. Future research should seek to identify adaptive versus maladaptive neuroplasticity associated with specific cognitive rehabilitation programs within all MS phenotypes to foster the validation of the most effective cognitive rehabilitation interventions for persons with MS.

Cognitive Rehabilitation in Multiple Sclerosis: The Role of Plasticity

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a progressive neurological disease marked by the development of lesions, or plaques, throughout the brain and spinal cord. The disease has been shown to impact both the white and gray matter of the brain often resulting in permanent disability (13). A broad array of symptoms is common in persons with MS, including motor, psychiatric, and cognitive symptomatology (4).

Cognitive deficits are common in MS, with prevalence rates ranging from 43 to 70% (57). MS impacts multiple aspects of cognition and may appear either early or late in the disease process. Deficits in information processing speed represent the most common cognitive deficit in MS (813). Other prevalent areas of deficit include attention (13, 14), executive functioning (1517), working memory (18, 19), and long-term memory (4, 2023). Overall intellectual functioning generally remains intact (24), as do “simple” attention (i.e., repeating numbers) and basic verbal skills (i.e., word naming, comprehension)(25). The clinical presentation is thus typically one of cognitive deficits, sometimes mild to moderate in nature, impacting specific cognitive domains. Due to the fact that the cognitive profile in MS is generally not one of a generalized dementia, cognitive rehabilitation is particularly appropriate for persons with MS. Cognitive deficits can often be specifically identified through a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment and subsequent cognitive rehabilitation can target discrete areas of dysfunction in an effort to improve overall cognitive abilities and quality of life (QoL).

Cognitive dysfunction has been shown to exert a significant negative impact on the every day lives of persons with MS. Persons with MS with cognitive impairment participate in fewer social and vocational activities (25), have higher rates of unemployment or under employment (5, 2528) and show greater difficulties in doing routine household tasks (25, 29). Deficits in new learning and memory in particular have been shown to result in a reduced ability to make decisions that could affect functioning in everyday life (30) and negatively impact daily living (3133). Common resultant functional impairments include difficulty with household chores, shopping, completing home repairs, driving, and using public transportation (34, 35). Reduced QoL is often reported (36).

Given the significant impact of cognitive deficits on the everyday lives and overall QoL of persons with MS, it is imperative that we develop and validate mechanisms for effectively treating cognitive dysfunction in this population. Numerous studies have demonstrated cognitive rehabilitation to be effective across many domains of functioning in other neurological populations. For instance, recent systematic reviews have shown that cognitive interventions can significantly improve functioning in persons with TBI and stroke (3739). Cognitive rehabilitation has also led to significant gains in the aging population across both objective neuropsychological performance (40) and the performance of daily life activities (4143), with effects maintained up to 10 years post-treatment (44). There have, however, been considerably fewer studies on cognitive rehabilitation in MS, with many of these studies suffering from significant methodological difficulties (4548). More recent, well-designed studies have been more promising and have provided evidence of improved objective cognitive performance as well as improvements in everyday life activities following cognitive rehabilitation [e.g., (45, 4951)].

Cognitive Rehabilitation in MS: The Role of Neuroimaging

The term neuroplasticity refers to “the ability of the nervous system to respond to intrinsic and extrinsic stimuli by reorganizing its structure, function, and connections” [(52); p. 1591]. That is, the brain is able to reorganize its structural and functional connections in an effort to maximize functional capacity and “adjust” its resources to cope with cognitive impairments. Changes in functional activation in persons with MS have often been correlated with improved cognitive performance, such as following cognitive rehabilitation; authors have thus interpreted such neuroplasticity as having a positive or “adaptive” outcome (50, 53). However, it is important to recognize that such plasticity may also be “maladaptive.” The term “maladaptive plasticity” may be used to refer to cerebral inefficiency in situations in which such neuroplasticity is correlated with cognitive impairment or decline (54, 55).

Neuroplasticity has recently been observed in numerous studies to explain treatment efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation. That is, both structural and functional neuroimaging have been shown to be related to improvements in cognitive abilities in MS following treatment. In studies of cognition in MS utilizing neuroimaging, cognitive impairments in MS have been related to various measures of cerebral integrity including, T2 lesion load (56), cerebral atrophy (57), third ventricular width (58), corpus callosum size (59), and cortical lesions (60). In addition, the wide application of functional neuroimaging techniques to the MS population has demonstrated alterations in patterns of cerebral activation and functional connectivity. Task-based fMRI is a widely used approach to understanding the cerebral resources involved in completing a specific cognitive task. This approach affords researchers the opportunity to examine levels of activation during task performance in specific brain regions. These altered patterns of cerebral activation have been documented during tasks involving attention (6163), working memory (54, 6366), episodic memory (64, 65, 67), and processing speed (68). That is, fMRI studies have noted changes in the functional organization of the brain in MS patients compared with healthy individuals. In addition, studies have even noted that patients in early stages of MS activate additional regions during task performance, prior to cognitive deficits being detectable on neuropsychological assessment [e.g., Ref. (69, 70)]. It has been proposed that this additional activation serves as a compensatory mechanism allowing the individual to maintain intact cognitive functioning for a period of time (69, 71, 72). In more severely impaired patients, however, the data are less consistent. Some groups have noted activation patterns to be comparable with controls (62), despite impairments in cognitive performance, with fewer areas of increased activation than is evident in patients in the earlier stages of the disease. This pattern of findings has been interpreted as an inability to access the additional cognitive resources needed to effectively perform the task (73). Others studies have noted increased activation on task-based fMRI in cognitively impaired patients with MS (54, 55), with this increase in activation correlated with worse performance on cognitive tasks. Due to its correlation with greater cognitive impairment, this increased activation is deemed maladaptive in nature and has been interpreted as neural inefficiency.

Resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC) studies have similarly noted increased activation to be interpreted as either adaptive or maladaptive in nature, depending on the progression of the disease. In contrast to task-based fMRI, rs-FC allows the examination of the communication between different brain regions within neural networks, while at “rest.” Increased connectivity during rs-FC is thought to serve as a compensatory mechanism for cognitive deficits early in the MS disease process (71, 7476). For example, early alterations in neuronal synchronization in rs-FC networks in clinically isolated syndrome (CIS) have been interpreted to be compensatory, indicating cortical reorganization. Such alterations may not be observed with increased brain damage, thought to indicate that such reorganization is finite and only evident early in the disease process (73). Interestingly, other work has noted that when increased rs-FC is found later in the disease process, this increase rs-FC appears to be maladaptive, similar to that which was found in some task-based fMRI studies [e.g., (54, 55)]. That is, increased rs-FC has been shown to be related to increased cognitive dysfunction in MS samples (77). Thus, early in the disease increased rs-FC appears to be adaptive, but later in the disease, these extra connections are associated with worse performance.

Functional neuroimaging techniques thus provide a means of understanding functional reorganization and neural plasticity in response to the disease process. Functional neuroimaging could similarly be used to observe neural plasticity following effective cognitive rehabilitation. The advantage of using functional neuroimaging in conjunction with traditional neuropsychological outcomes is that researchers can observe, not only the traditional behavioral improvements on cognitive tasks but also changes in the functional cerebral architecture underlying such cognitive improvements. Thus, several recent studies have utilized neuroimaging techniques to evaluate the neurofunctional and neuroanatomical changes associated with cognitive rehabilitation in MS samples. These studies have demonstrated neuroplasticity of the brain of the person with MS through the documentation of changes at the level of the cerebral substrate from before to after non-invasive, non-pharmacological, behavioral treatment for deficits in cognition.

Researchers have successfully applied various neuroimaging techniques to study the effects of cognitive rehabilitation in MS including task-based fMRI [e.g., (78)] and rs-FC [e.g., (79)] across multiple realms of cognition including episodic memory (78), executive functioning, attention, and processing speed (45, 50, 51). Studies have even begun to examine longer-term maintenance of such functional changes, documenting sustained plasticity over time (80).

Research conducted utilizing these neuroimaging techniques have consistently demonstrated significantly increased cerebral activity following cognitive rehabilitation [e.g., (50, 53, 78, 79, 8183)], with numerous researchers noting induced neural plasticity in response to cognitive rehabilitation. As expected, the specific brain regions in which changes in activation patterns are documented post-treatment varies with the treatment protocol investigated, the specific cognitive function targeted for treatment, as well as the imaging protocol applied. Studies also show differences in the documentation of a relationship between these changes in patterns of cerebral activation and changes in behavior documented via neuropsychological assessment. Specifically, some studies have found that the changes on fMRI to correlate with improvement on neuropsychological assessment in the targeted domain [e.g., (50, 53)], while others have failed to document such a relationship [e.g., (84)].

The majority of studies applying fMRI and rs-FC to the investigation of the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in MS have focused on the amelioration of attentional deficits. The attention/information processing modules of the cognitive therapy program, the RehaCom (85), have been far received the most attention. Filippi et al. (50) utilized both fMRI and rs-fMRI to examine the cerebral impact of cognitive retraining in 10 persons with MS that completed treatment and 10 that did not complete treatment. The treatment protocol examined consisted of a portion of the RehaCom addressing attention, information processing, and executive functioning. An improvement in cognitive functioning was noted on the Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST; a test of executive functioning), the paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT; a processing speed and working memory test), and controlled oral word association from pre to post-treatment. While no differences were noted on structural measures, the group who received the treatment showed significantly increased activation on fMRI in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/precuneus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) bilaterally compared to the placebo group (Figure 1). An increase in rs-FRI was also noted after the treatment period in the treatment group only in the right PCC and the IPL of the default mode network (DMN). The DMN is a cortical network that has been shown to be active when the individual is at rest and deactivated when the individual is actively engaged in a cognitive task (86). Finally, increased rs-FC was noted in the treatment group only in the executive functioning network (left DLPFC) as well, which is implicated in active cognitive control during task performance. The increased activity in these networks was interpreted to be indicative of compensatory activation due to treatment effects. These authors also noted positive correlations between changes in rs-FC and cognitive performance, as well as changes on fMRI and cognitive performance, such that increased activation and increased rs-FC were each associated with improved task performance. This was observed across all subjects and when examining the treatment group only. Importantly, regions showing post-treatment changes in activity are areas known to be active in cognitively demanding tasks (87). Further analyses of the same data revealed increased rs-FC of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as well as within the right middle frontal gyrus and the right IPL in the treatment group but not the control group (82). The control group showed decreased activation at follow-up in the ACC as well as the right cerebellum and the right inferior temporal lobule. In a follow-up investigation by the same group (88), rs-FC changes in the DMN following treatment predicted cognitive performance 6 months later. This indicates that the changes in patterns of cerebral activation and connectivity following cognitive rehabilitation can be maintained over time.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A,B) Statistical parametric mapping results (color-coded for t values) overlaid on high-spatial resolution T1-weighted MR images show changes in functional MR imaging activations during the Stroop interference condition in (A) control group (axial images) and (B) treatment group (p = 0.05, paired t test, family-wise corrected for multiple comparisons) (sagittal and axial images). (C) Statistical parametric mapping results (color-coded for t values) overlaid on high-spatial-resolution T1-weighted MR images show between group comparisons of functional MR imaging activations during the Stroop interference condition (analysis of variance, two-by-two factorial design; p = 0.05, family wise corrected for multiple comparisons) in treated group versus control group (sagittal and coronal images). Here and throughout, images are in neurologic convention (i.e., left side of the image shows left side of the brain, right side of the image shows right side of the brain). *Reprinted with permission from the Journal of Radiology.

Also examining the RehaCom, Bonavita et al. (84) investigated changes in functional connectivity from before to after 8 weeks of cognitive rehabilitation with specific sections of the RehaCom program, namely, Attention and Concentration, Plan a Day, Divided Attention, Reaction Behavior, and Logical Thinking. They contrasted this treatment with a control group that received a placebo intervention. Post-treatment cognitive gains were noted in only the treatment group in processing speed abilities [symbol digit modalities test (SDMT) and PASAT] and verbal and visual learning and memory [(selective reminding test (SRT) and the spatial recall test (SPART-10/36)]. Changes were also noted in the DMN post-treatment, specifically increased FC in the PCC and IPC. In contrast to Filippi et al. (50), these authors failed to find a correlation between changes in FC and improvement in neuropsychological functioning.

Specifically focused on the neuroplasticity of the cerebellum, Cerasa et al. (53) demonstrated that specific, computer-based training for attention deficits results in adaptive neural plasticity of the neural network involved in attention. Specifically, they found increased activity in the posterior cerebral lobule (lobule IV) and the superior parietal lobule following RehaCom in the treatment group only. A significant relationship was noted between behavioral gains post-treatment and increased activation in these brain regions, similar to others (50). Interestingly, lobule VI of the cerebellum is active in the articulatory control system; the authors thus concluded the increased activation noted in this region post-treatment to represent an increased effort to subvocally refresh subvocal stimuli in this system.

Sastre-Garriga et al. (83) found increased brain activity in the cerebellum following a treatment designed to target attention, speed of information processing, executive functions, memory, and higher level language processes. Participants who completed treatment demonstrated improvement in cognitive performance as well as increased brain activity in the anterior and posterior lobes of the right cerebellum. Although suffering from some methodological limitations, the authors were able to conclude that the positive impact of the cognitive rehabilitation on cognitive performance may, in fact, be mediated by increased activity within the cerebellum, a finding further supported by Cerasa et al. (53). Although the cerebellum is a largely understudied brain region as it relates to cognition and cognitive rehabilitation, it is important to note that two existing studies on cognitive rehabilitation in MS highlight the adaptive neuroplasticity of the cerebellum in response to a treatment for attention deficits. This is clearly a region ripe for future investigation.

Penner et al. (89) examined the effect of a 3- to 4-week computerized training program targeting selective attention in 11 patients with MS on patterns of cerebral activation on fMRI. Increased activation was seen post-treatment in MS patients with both mild and severe cognitive impairment in brain regions involved in attention, namely, the PCC, the precuneus, and the dorsal frontal cortex. Behavioral improvement correlated with the increased activation noted in these regions post-treatment. Although the lack of a control group in the study design was a limiting factor of this study, these data indicate that persons with MS can benefit from cognitive rehabilitation across the range of severity of cognitive impairment and neuroplasticity can be induced by cognitive rehabilitation procedures. Penner et al. (90) concluded that cognitive rehabilitation may enhance neuroplasticity in persons with MS and encourages the use of fMRI to enhance our understanding of the induced plasticity in persons with MS, as well as identify effective cognitive rehabilitation protocols.

Although limited in number, the two existing studies examining the cognitive rehabilitation of memory functioning in MS via neuroimaging techniques, also support the existence of induced neural plasticity in response to treatment. Chiaravalloti and colleagues utilized both fMRI (78) and FC (79) to evaluate a 10-session cognitive rehabilitation protocol specifically targeting new learning and memory abilities through a randomized clinical trial. After treatment, greater activation was evident only in the treatment group during performance of a memory task within a widespread cortical network involving frontal, parietal, precuneus, and parahippocampal regions (Figure 2). In a separate analysis by the same group (79), a significant increase in FC was noted in the treatment group post-treatment between the left hippocampus and cortical regions involved in memory functions, namely, the left insula, right parahippcampal gyrus, right insula, precentral gyrus, and post-central gyrus (Figure 3). These changes were not seen in the placebo-control group. These results demonstrate the neuroplasticity of the memory network in response to cognitive rehabilitation targeting learning and memory deficits in MS.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the 2 × 2 ANOVA with factors of time and group. Following treatment, significant increases in activation were seen in the treatment group relative to the control group in regions including frontal lobe, parietal lobe, and cerebellum. All comparisons are significant at p < 0.01 (minimum cluster size = 10 voxels). (A) Bold activation change from pre- to post-treatment in parahippocampal gyrus. Control group represented by blue line; treatment group represented by red line. All interactions shown are significant at p < 0.01. (B) Bold activation change from pre- to post-treatment in superior temporal gyrus. (C) Bold activation change from pre- to post-treatment in middle frontal gyrus. (D) Bold activation change from pre- to post-treatment in precuneus. *Reprinted with permission from the Journal of Neurology.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. LHIPP seed: increased connectivity between from LHIPP to left and right insula in the treatment group at post-treatment. Interaction plot displays increased connectivity to left insula. R-values are plotted on the ordinate; time is plotted on the abscissa. Red line indicates treatment subjects, blue line indicates controls. *Reprinted with permission from Brain Imaging and Behavior.

Ernst et al. (81) also examined the neuroplasticity associated with memory abilities, examining changes on fMRI following treatment focusing on autobiographical memories in an MS sample. The authors define autobiographical memory as the “capacity to relive detailed events, evoking spatiotempoal context, in which they were encountered as they are remembered.” The authors noted that following an intervention program for autobiographical memory, patients showed greater recruitment of the right cuneous, the left inferior and superior occipital gyri, the left precuneus and part of the lateral temporal cortex, largely on the left side, as compared with before treatment. These regions were consistent with regions known to be involved in the trained constructs. That is, changes were noted in posterior cerebral regions, known to be associated with the mental visual imagery trained in the training protocol applied. Significant improvement was noted in autobiographical memory, although the relationship between the behavioral and neuroimaging changes was not examined. Taken together, the two existing studies examining the efficacy of memory interventions in MS with neuroimaging both demonstrate increased activation in similar brain networks known to be integral to the trained function.

fMRI is thus a valuable tool to identify areas of dysfunction, and provides substantial evidence of both natural and induced neuroplasticity in persons with MS. Neuroplasticity in MS appears to largely be adaptive in nature when in response to rehabilitation, minimizing the clinical consequences of the neurological injury. It seems that a positive outcome of cognitive rehabilitation is likely the presence of post-treatment changes in fMRI, indicating the strengthening of existing regions and pathways associated with the treated domain. The application of neuroimaging measures to examine the functional and structural basis of changes in cognitive performance following cognitive rehabilitation will enhance our ability to identify the most effective treatments for persons with MS and modify such treatment to achieve maximal efficacy (9193).

Increased Activation/Connectivity: Adaptive or Maladaptive?

Increases in cerebral activation, as well as increased functional connectivity can occur in persons with MS under varying conditions [see (94) for a complete discussion]. The first condition involves “local expansion.” The term “local expansion” refers to an increase in activation in the region immediately surrounding the lesioned area or area affected by the disease (95, 96). Specifically, persons in the early stages of MS have been shown to demonstrate increases in activation and connectivity, as compared with healthy controls, in the absence of cognitive impairment. Such changes in brain function are often associated with intact cognitive functioning and interpreted as adaptive neuroplasticity. For example, Forn et al. (70) demonstrated increased cortical recruitment during fMRI, reflecting the local expansion of activation, in cognitively preserved CIS patients (70) suggesting that early cortical changes may, in fact, limit the clinical expression of neuronal damage resulting from MS. Audoin et al. (69) similarly showed that CIS patients exhibited significantly greater activation in the regions normally involved in executive functioning: orbitofrontal regions, right cerebellum, and bilateral lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) region during the PASAT, as compared with healthy controls, suggesting this activation to be adaptive. Amann et al. (97) examined the cerebral activation patterns associated with a working memory fMRI task in RRMS subjects with mild cognitive impairment and healthy controls and found that the overall pattern of brain activation was similar between the two groups. However, in the Anmann et al. study, persons with MS showed local expansion of cerebral activation during task performance within regions typically associated with working memory (i.e., anterior frontal and inferior parietal cortex). Similar findings were observed by Forn et al. (72) in an early RRMS sample. Taken together, these studies indicate that the maintenance of cognitive performance was due to the local cerebral expansion, interpreted as adaptive plasticity. Thus, in both CIS patients (69, 70) and patients with early RRMS (72, 97), there are indications of early plasticity of cognitive processes. While task performance is intact, the minimal additional recruitment typically seen in local expansion of activation appears to be an active and effective compensatory mechanism occurring early in the disease process.

A second condition is one in which we observe “homologous area adaptation.” Homologous area adaptation involves activation in areas in homologous regions of the contralateral hemisphere to the area impacted by disease (98, 99). In these instances, increases in cerebral activation/connectivity are correlated with impaired cognition. Chiaravalloti et al. (54) examined a modified PASAT administered via fMRI in three groups: MS with working memory impairment, MS without working memory impairment, and healthy controls. The healthy control group and MS group without working memory impairment showed a comparable activation pattern, i.e., primarily left hemisphere activation during working memory performance. However, in those MS individuals with working memory impairment, significantly more activation was noted bilaterally in the parietal and frontal regions in the MS group (indicative of both local expansion and homologous area adaptation). Further, the degree of extension of activation into the homologous right frontal region was correlated with worse cognitive performance, indicative of maladaptive neuroplasticity. This same pattern of results was also observed by Hillary et al. (55) on a different working memory task, with these authors interpreting their findings as indicative of neural inefficiency. Loitfelder et al. (100) compared HC with subjects with CIS, RRMS, and SPMS on a Go/No-Go task using fMRI and demonstrated that SPMS subjects showed activation in regions other than the task-related network observed in healthy controls (i.e., terms “extra-region” recruitment). The authors interpreted the observed extensive activation as neural inefficiency (100). Taken together, these several studies demonstrate cortical recruitment of “extra-regions” to support task completion, but this “extra-region” activation is associated with poorer cognitive functioning, and thus reflects maladaptive plasticity.

A final condition is one in which one observes increased activation in regions associated with the cognitive constructs being addressed within the treatment. This is precisely what is observed following cognitive rehabilitation. Multiple authors have shown increased activation of existing networks underlying trained functions in person with MS following treatment (78, 81). However, these areas are those known to underlie the performance of the skills taught during the active interventions. It thus appears that cognitive rehabilitation may not entail a traditional expansion of active brain regions into local or distal regions. In contrast, what appears to be occurring is increased activation of brain regions engaged by the techniques taught in treatment. This may be a strengthening of existing areas of activation or, in some cases, may involve newly activated regions. As an example, in Chiaravalloti et al. (78), increased activation was observed in the parietal regions during a verbal learning task. However, this activation is directly related to the techniques taught in treatment – visualization. This is thus activation supporting newly engaged cognitive processes that were shown to support the successful completion of the task.

In conclusion, increases in activation/connectivity are seen in persons with MS following cognitive rehabilitation and these increases are often associated with improvement in the targeted cognitive domain. While increased activation has been found with increased cognitive decline and disease progression in studies of the natural progression of MS, it is important to note that this increase in activation due to disease progression is distinct from the activation observed following cognitive rehabilitation. Thus, there are situations in which increased activation and/or connectivity is a negative consequence of the disease. In these situations, the activation we are observing might be best termed “maladaptive compensation.” That is, these extra areas of activation (or connectivity) are actually associated with worse performance and are therefore maladaptive (54, 55, 77). However, there are also situations in which such increases in activation and connectivity are positive, such as following effective cognitive rehabilitation. In these cases, the increased activation is associated with improvement in cognitive functioning and can thus be concluded to be adaptive. It is important to note that adaptive and maladaptive cerebral activation have been shown in the various disease stages (CIS, RR, SPMS). However, the documentation of cerebral reorganization via fMRI following cognitive rehabilitation has largely focused on RRMS patients to date. Thus, additional research is needed on cerebral reorganization after cognitive training, focusing on all MS phenotypes.

In reviewing the existing research, it is clear that there is significant neuroplasticity of cognitive functions in individuals with MS. Homologous region adaptation, local activation expansion, and extra-region recruitment all occur in an effort to maintain cognitive functioning. While much of this neuroplasticity is adaptive, it is important to note that in many situations, such neuroplasticity may be maladaptive, particularly in individuals that are demonstrating significant cognitive impairment and/or with disease progression. This maladaptive neuroplasticity (e.g., extra-region recruitment) may come at the cost of other cognitive functions for which the new areas now being utilized were crucial, such as processing speed. It is encouraging that such neuroplasticity can be induced through treatment such as cognitive rehabilitation, in an effort to “normalize” brain function and behavioral output. Moving forward, a focus on identifying adaptive versus maladaptive neuroplasticity associated with specific cognitive rehabilitation programs within all MS phenotypes would aide in the validation of the most effective cognitive rehabilitation interventions for persons with MS.

Author Contributions

Dr. NC led the literature review, synthesis of the literature, and drafted the manuscript. Dr. HG assisted with synthesis of the literature and edited the manuscript several times. Dr. JD assisted the literature review, synthesis of the literature, and editing of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

Dr. Nancy D. Chiaravalloti reports no disclosures. Dr. Helen M. Genova reports no disclosures. Dr. John DeLuca has served as a consultant for Biogen IDEC and Novartis Pharmaceuticals. He has received grant funding from Biogen IDEC. He also is a journal club speaker for EMD Serono.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge grant support from the NIH R01HD045798 (NC) and 1R01HD045798 S1 (NC), NMSS CA 1069-A-7 (JD), and Kessler Foundation.

References

1. Chelune GJ, Stott H, Pinkston J. Multiple Sclerosis Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis (2008).

Google Scholar

2. Trapp BD, Peterson J, Ransohoff RM, Rudick R, Mork S, Bo L. Axonal transection in the lesions of multiple sclerosis. N Engl J Med (1998) 338(5):278–85. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801293380502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Trapp BD, Ransohoff RM, Fisher E, Rudick RA. Neurodegeneration in multiple sclerosis: relationship to neurological disability. Neuroscientist (1999) 5(1):48–57. doi:10.1177/107385849900500107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Brassington JC, Marsh NV. Neuropsychological aspects of multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychol Rev (1998) 8(2):43–77. doi:10.1023/A:1025621700003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Benedict RH, Cookfair D, Gavett R, Gunther M, Munschauer F, Garg N, et al. Validity of the minimal assessment of cognitive function in multiple sclerosis (MACFIMS). J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2006) 12(4):549–58. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060723

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Peyser JM, Rao SM, LaRocca NG, Kaplan E. Guidelines for neuropsychological research in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol (1990) 47(1):94–7. doi:10.1001/archneur.1990.00530010120030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rao SM, Leo GJ, Bernardin L, Unverzagt F. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurology (1991) 41(5):685–91. doi:10.1212/WNL.41.5.685

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bergendal G, Fredrikson S, Almkvist O. Selective decline in information processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Neurol (2007) 57(4):193–202. doi:10.1159/000099158

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. DeLuca J, Chelune GJ, Tulsky DS, Lengenfelder J, Chiaravalloti ND. Is speed of processing or working memory the primary information processing deficit in multiple sclerosis? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2004) 26(4):550–62. doi:10.1080/13803390490496641

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Diamond BJ, DeLuca J, Kim H, Kelley SM. The question of disproportionate impairments in visual and auditory information processing in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (1997) 19(1):34–42. doi:10.1080/01688639708403834

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Grafman J, Rao S, Bernardin L, Leo GJ. Automatic memory processes in patients with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol (1991) 48(10):1072–5. doi:10.1001/archneur.1991.00530220094025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Janculjak D, Mubrin Z, Brinar V, Spilich G. Changes of attention and memory in a group of patients with multiple sclerosis. Clin Neurol Neurosurg (2002) 104(3):221–7. doi:10.1016/S0303-8467(02)00042-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Litvan I, Grafman J, Vendrell P, Martinez JM. Slowed information processing in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol (1988) 45(3):281–5. doi:10.1001/archneur.1988.00520270059021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Beatty WW, Wilbanks SL, Blanco CR, Hames KA, Tivis R, Paul RH. Memory disturbance in multiple sclerosis: reconsideration of patterns of performance on the selective reminding test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (1996) 18(1):56–62. doi:10.1080/01688639608408262

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Denney DR, Sworowski LA, Lynch SG. Cognitive impairment in three subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2005) 20(8):967–81. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Foong J, Rozewicz L, Quaghebeur G, Davie CA, Kartsounis LD, Thompson AJ, et al. Executive function in multiple sclerosis. The role of frontal lobe pathology. Brain (1997) 120(Pt 1):15–26. doi:10.1093/brain/120.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lazeron RH, Rombouts SA, Scheltens P, Polman CH, Barkhof F. An fMRI study of planning-related brain activity in patients with moderately advanced multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2004) 10(5):549–55. doi:10.1191/1352458504ms1072oa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Audoin B, Au Duong MV, Ranjeva JP, Ibarrola D, Malikova I, Confort-Gouny S, et al. Magnetic resonance study of the influence of tissue damage and cortical reorganization on PASAT performance at the earliest stage of multiple sclerosis. Hum Brain Mapp (2005) 24(3):216–28. doi:10.1002/hbm.20083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Grigsby J, Ayarbe SD, Kravcisin N, Busenbark D. Working memory impairment among persons with chronic progressive multiple sclerosis. J Neurol (1994) 241(3):125–31. doi:10.1007/BF00868338

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. DeLuca J, Barbieri-Berger S, Johnson SK. The nature of memory impairments in multiple sclerosis: acquisition versus retrieval. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (1994) 16(2):183–9. doi:10.1080/01688639408402629

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. DeLuca J, Gaudino EA, Diamond BJ, Christodoulou C, Engel RA. Acquisition and storage deficits in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (1998) 20(3):376–90. doi:10.1076/jcen.20.3.376.819

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Rao SM, Grafman J, DiGuilio D, Mittenberg W, Bernardin L, Leo GJ, et al. Memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: its relation to working memory, semantic encoding and implicit learning. Neuropsychology (1993) 7(3):364–74. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Thornton AE, Raz N, Tucke KA. Memory in multiple sclerosis: contextual encoding deficits. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2002) 8(3):395–409. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813200

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Macniven JAB, Davis C, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Constantinescu CS. Stroop performance in multiple sclerosis: information processing, selective attention, or executive functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2008) 14:805–14. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080946

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Rao SM, Leo GJ, Ellington L, Nauertz T, Bernardin L, Unverzagt F. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. II. Impact on employment and social functioning. Neurology (1991) 41(5):692–6. doi:10.1212/WNL.41.5.692

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Beatty WW, Blanco CR, Wilbanks SL, Paul RH, Hames KA. Demographic, clinical, and cognitive characteristics of multiple sclerosis patients who continue to work. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (1995) 9(3):167–73. doi:10.1177/154596839500900306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Edgley K, Sullivan MJ, Dehoux E. A survey of multiple sclerosis. Part 2: determinants of employment status. Can J Rehabil (1991) 4:127–32.

Google Scholar

28. Parmenter JL, Shucard JL, Schucard DW. Information processing deficits in multiple sclerosis: a matter of complexity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2007) 13:417–23. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070580

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kalmar JH, Gaudino EA, Moore NB, Halper J, Deluca J. The relationship between cognitive deficits and everyday functional activities in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology (2008) 22(4):442–9. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.442

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Nagy H, Bencsik K, Rajda C, Benedek K, Beniczky S, Keri S, et al. The effects of reward and punishment contingencies on decision-making in multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2006) 12(4):559–65. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060644

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Goverover Y, O’Brien AR, Moore NB, DeLuca J. Actual reality: a new approach to functional assessment in persons with multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2010) 91(2):252–60. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2009.09.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kessler HR, Cohen RA, Lauer K, Kausch DF. The relationship between disability and memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Int J Neurosci (1992) 62(1–2):17–34. doi:10.3109/00207459108999754

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rendell PG, Jensen F, Henry JD. Prospective memory in multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2007) 13(3):410–6.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Goverover Y, Genova HM, Hillary FG, DeLuca J. The relationship between neuropsychological measures and the timed instrumental activities of daily living task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2007) 13(5):636–44. doi:10.1177/1352458506072984

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Staples D, Lincoln NB. Intellectual impairment in multiple sclerosis and its relation to functional abilities. Rheumatol Rehabil (1979) 18(3):153–60. doi:10.1093/rheumatology/18.3.153

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Grima DT, Torrance GW, Francis G, Rice G, Rosner AJ, Lafortune L. Cost and health related quality of life consequences of multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2000) 6(2):91–8. doi:10.1191/135245800678827545

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, Langenbahn DM, Malec JF, Bergquist TF, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2000) 81(12):1596–615. doi:10.1053/apmr.2000.19240

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, Langenbahn DM, Felicetti T, Kneipp S, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2005) 86(8):1681–92. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, Malec JF, Kalmar K, Fraas M, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2011) 92(4):519–30. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2010.11.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA (2002) 288(18):2271–81. doi:10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ball K, Edwards JD, Ross LA. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci (2007) 62(Spec No 1):19–31. doi:10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.19

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology (2002) 48(5):329–40. doi:10.1159/000065259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Vance DE, Wood K, Roenker DL, Ball KK. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health (2005) 9(3):262–71. doi:10.1080/13607860412331336788

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rebok GW, Ball K, Guey LT, Jones RN, Kim HY, King JW, et al. Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc (2014) 62(1):16–24. doi:10.1111/jgs.12607

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Amato MP, Goretti B, Viterbo RG, Portaccio E, Niccolai C, Hakiki B, et al. Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in patients with multiple sclerosis: results of a randomized, double-blind trial. Mult Scler (2014) 20(1):91–8. doi:10.1177/1352458513501571

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Amato MP, Langdon D, Montalban X, Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Krupp LB, et al. Treatment of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: position paper. J Neurol (2013) 260(6):1452–68. doi:10.1007/s00415-012-6678-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. O’Brien AR, Chiaravalloti N, Goverover Y, Deluca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: a review of the literature. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2008) 89(4):761–9. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2007.10.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Rosti-Ostajarvi EM, Hamalainen PI. Neuropsychological rehabilitation for multiple sclerosis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2011) (11):CD009131. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Chiaravalloti ND, Moore NB, Nikelshpur OM, Deluca J. An RCT to treat learning impairment in multiple sclerosis: the MEMREHAB trial. Neurology (2013) 81(24):2066–72. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000437295.97946.a8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Filippi M, Riccitelli G, Mattioli F, Capra R, Stampatori C, Pagani E, et al. Multiple sclerosis: effects of cognitive rehabilitation on structural and functional MR imaging measures – an explorative study. Radiology (2012) 262(3):932–40. doi:10.1148/radiol.11111299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Mattioli F, Stampatori C, Zanotti D, Parrinello G, Capra R. Efficacy and specificity of intensive cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci (2010) 288(1–2):101–5. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.09.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cramer SC, Sur M, Dobkin BH, O’Brien C, Sanger TD, Trojanowski JQ, et al. Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications. Brain (2011) 134(Pt 6):1591–609. doi:10.1093/brain/awr039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Valentino P, Nistico R, Chiriaco C, Pirritano D, et al. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation of attention deficits for multiple sclerosis: a randomized trial with fMRI correlates. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2013) 27(4):284–95. doi:10.1177/1545968312465194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chiaravalloti ND, Hillary F, Ricker J, Christodoulou C, Kalnin A, Liu WC, et al. Cerebral activation patterns during working memory performance in multiple sclerosis using fMRI. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2005) 27(1):33–54. doi:10.1080/138033990513609

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hillary FG, Chiaravalloti ND, Ricker JH, Steffener J, Bly BM, Lange G, et al. An investigation of working memory rehearsal in multiple sclerosis using fMRI. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2003) 25(7):965–78. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.7.965.16490

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Benedict RH, Weinstock-Guttman B, Fishman I, Sharma J, Tjoa CW, Bakshi R. Prediction of neuropsychological impairment in multiple sclerosis: comparison of conventional magnetic resonance imaging measures of atrophy and lesion burden. Arch Neurol (2004) 61(2):226–30. doi:10.1001/archneur.61.2.226

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Zivadinov R, Sepcic J, Nasuelli D, De Masi R, Bragadin LM, Tommasi MA, et al. A longitudinal study of brain atrophy and cognitive disturbances in the early phase of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2001) 70(6):773–80. doi:10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Benedict RH, Bruce JM, Dwyer MG, Abdelrahman N, Hussein S, Weinstock-Guttman B, et al. Neocortical atrophy, third ventricular width, and cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol (2006) 63(9):1301–6. doi:10.1001/archneur.63.9.1301

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Pelletier J, Suchet I, Witias T. A longitudinal study of callosal atrophy and interhemispheric dysfunction in relapse-remitting multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol (2001) 58:105–11. doi:10.1001/archneur.58.1.105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Roosendaal SD, Moraal B, Pouwels PJ, Vrenken H, Castelijns JA, Barkhof F, et al. Accumulation of cortical lesions in MS: relation with cognitive impairment. Mult Scler (2009) 15(6):708–14. doi:10.1177/1352458509102907

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Nebel K, Wiese H, Seyfarth J, Gizewski ER, Stude P, Diener HC, et al. Activity of attention related structures in multiple sclerosis patients. Brain Res (2007) 1151:150–60. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Penner IK, Rausch M, Kappos L, Opwis K, Radu EW. Analysis of impairment related functional architecture in MS patients during performance of different attention tasks. J Neurol (2003) 250(4):461–72. doi:10.1007/s00415-003-1025-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Staffen W, Mair A, Zauner H, Unterrainer J, Niederhofer H, Kutzelnigg A, et al. Cognitive function and fMRI in patients with multiple sclerosis: evidence for compensatory cortical activation during an attention task. Brain (2002) 125(Pt 6):1275–82. doi:10.1093/brain/awf125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Mainero C, Caramia F, Pozzilli C, Pisani A, Pestalozza I, Borriello G, et al. fMRI evidence of brain reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage (2004) 21(3):858–67. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Mainero C, Pantano P, Caramia F, Pozzilli C. Brain reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple sclerosis: insights from functional MRI studies. J Neurol Sci (2006) 245(1–2):93–8. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Sweet LH, Rao SM, Primeau M, Mayer AR, Cohen RA. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory among multiple sclerosis patients. J Neuroimaging (2004) 14(2):150–7. doi:10.1177/1051228403262695

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bobholz JA, Rao SM, Lobeck L, Elsinger C, Gleason A, Kanz J, et al. fMRI study of episodic memory in relapsing-remitting MS: correlation with T2 lesion volume. Neurology (2006) 67(9):1640–5. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000242885.71725.76

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Genova HM, Hillary FG, Wylie G, Rypma B, DeLuca J. Examination of processing speed deficits in multiple sclerosis using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2009) 15(3):383–93. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090535

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Audoin B, Ibarrola D, Ranjeva JP, Confort-Gouny S, Malikova I, Ali-Cherif A, et al. Compensatory cortical activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of MS. Hum Brain Mapp (2003) 20(2):51–8. doi:10.1002/hbm.10128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Forn C, Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Bosca I, Casanova B, Sanjuan A, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of cognitive performance in patients with a clinically isolated syndrome suggestive of multiple sclerosis at presentation: an activation and connectivity study. Mult Scler (2012) 18(2):153–63. doi:10.1177/1352458511417744

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Forn C, Barros-Loscertales A, Escudero J, Belloch V, Campos S, Parcet MA, et al. Cortical reorganization during PASAT task in MS patients with preserved working memory functions. Neuroimage (2006) 31(2):686–91. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Forn C, Barros-Loscertales A, Escudero J, Benlloch V, Campos S, Antonia Parcet M, et al. Compensatory activations in patients with multiple sclerosis during preserved performance on the auditory N-back task. Hum Brain Mapp (2007) 28(5):424–30. doi:10.1002/hbm.20284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Filippi M, Rocca MA. Present and future of fMRI in multiple sclerosis. Expert Rev Neurother (2013) 13(12s):27–31. doi:10.1586/14737175.2013.865871

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Au Duong MV, Boulanouar K, Audoin B, Treseras S, Ibarrola D, Malikova I, et al. Modulation of effective connectivity inside the working memory network in patients at the earliest stage of multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage (2005) 24(2):533–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Bonnet MC, Allard M, Dilharreguy B, Deloire M, Petry KG, Brochet B. Cognitive compensation failure in multiple sclerosis. Neurology (2010) 75(14):1241–8. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f612e3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Cader S, Cifelli A, Abu-Omar Y, Palace J, Matthews PM. Reduced brain functional reserve and altered functional connectivity in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain (2006) 129(Pt 2):527–37. doi:10.1093/brain/awh670

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Leavitt VM, Wylie G, Genova HM, Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Altered effective connectivity during performance of an information processing speed task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2012) 18(4):409–17. doi:10.1177/1352458511423651

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Chiaravalloti ND, Wylie G, Leavitt V, Deluca J. Increased cerebral activation after behavioral treatment for memory deficits in MS. J Neurol (2012) 259(7):1337–46. doi:10.1007/s00415-011-6353-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Leavitt VM, Wylie GR, Girgis PA, DeLuca J, Chiaravalloti ND. Increased functional connectivity within memory networks following memory rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. Brain Imaging Behav (2014) 8(3):394–402. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9183-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Dobryakova E, Wylie GR, DeLuca J, Chiaravalloti ND. A pilot study examining functional brain activity 6 months after memory retraining in MS: the MEMREHAB trial. Brain Imaging Behav (2014) 8(3):403–6. doi:10.1007/s11682-014-9309-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Ernst A, Botzung A, Gounot D, Sellal F, Blanc F, de Seze J, et al. Induced brain plasticity after a facilitation programme for autobiographical memory in multiple sclerosis: a preliminary study. Mult Scler Int (2012) 2012:820240. doi:10.1155/2012/820240

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Parisi L, Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Panicari L, Mattioli F, Filippi M. Cognitive rehabilitation correlates with the functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain Imaging Behav (2014) 8(3):387–93. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9160-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Sastre-Garriga J, Alonso J, Renom M, Arevalo MJ, Gonzalez I, Galan I, et al. A functional magnetic resonance proof of concept pilot trial of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2011) 17(4):457–67. doi:10.1177/1352458510389219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Bonavita S, Sacco R, Della Corte M, Esposito S, Sparaco M, d’Ambrosio A, et al. Computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation improves cognitive performances and induces brain functional connectivity changes in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients: an exploratory study. J Neurol (2015) 262(1):91–100. doi:10.1007/s00415-014-7528-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Hasomed Rehacom Cognitive Therapy. (2015). Available from: www.rehacom.us/index.php/aboutrehacom

Google Scholar

86. Caramia F, Tinelli E, Francia A, Pozzilli C. Cognitive deficits in multiple sclerosis: a review of functional MRI studies. Neurol Sci (2010) 31(Suppl 2):S239–43. doi:10.1007/s10072-010-0379-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci (2007) 27(9):2349–56. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Parisi L, Rocca MA, Mattioli F, Copetti M, Capra R, Valsasina P, et al. Changes of brain resting state functional connectivity predict the persistence of cognitive rehabilitation effects in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2014) 20(6):686–94. doi:10.1177/1352458513505692

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Penner I, Kappos L, Opwis K. Induced changes in brain activation using a computerized attention training in patients with multiple sclerosis (MS). Paper Presented at the Proceedings of KogWis05. The German Cognitive Science Conference. Basel: Schwabe (2005).

Google Scholar

90. Penner IK, Opwis K, Kappos L. Relation between functional brain imaging, cognitive impairment and cognitive rehabilitation in patients with multiple sclerosis. J Neurol (2007) 254(Suppl 2):II53–7. doi:10.1007/s00415-007-2013-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Filippi M, Rocca MA, Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Geurts JJ, Rombouts SA, et al. The contribution of MRI in assessing cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology (2010) 75(23):2121–8. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d768

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Penner IK, Sastre-Garriga J. One step forward in the quest for evidence of the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2014) 20(1):2. doi:10.1177/1352458513510982

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Penner IK, Kappos L, Rausch M, Opwis K, Radu EW. Therapy-induced plasticity of cognitive functions in MS patients: insights from fMRI. J Physiol Paris (2006) 99(4–6):455–62. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Stojanovic-Radic J, DeLuca J. Neuroplasticity in MS. “Plasticity of Cognition in Neurologic Disorders”, Drs. Tracy, Hampstead, and Sathian (eds) (Forthcoming).

Google Scholar

95. Merzenich MM, Nelson RJ, Stryker MP, Cynader MS, Schoppmann A, Zook JM. Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. J Comp Neurol (1984) 224(4):591–605. doi:10.1002/cne.902240408

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Pantano P, Iannetti GD, Caramia F, Mainero C, Di Legge S, Bozzao L, et al. Cortical motor reorganization after a single clinical attack of multiple sclerosis. Brain (2002) 125(Pt 7):1607–15. doi:10.1093/brain/awf164

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Amann M, Dossegger LS, Penner IK, Hirsch JG, Raselli C, Calabrese P, et al. Altered functional adaptation to attention and working memory tasks with increasing complexity in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients. Hum Brain Mapp (2011) 32(10):1704–19. doi:10.1002/hbm.21142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Grafman J, Christen Y. Neuronal Plasticity: Building a Bridge from the Laboratory of the Clinic. New York: Springer Verlag (1999).

Google Scholar

99. Grafman J, Litvan I. Evidence for Four Forms of Neuroplasticity Neuronal Plasticity: Building a Bridge from the Laboratory to the Clinic. New York: Springer (1999). p. 131–9.

Google Scholar

100. Loitfelder M, Fazekas F, Petrovic K, Fuchs S, Ropele S, Wallner-Blazek M, et al. Reorganization in cognitive networks with progression of multiple sclerosis: insights from fMRI. Neurology (2011) 76(6):526–33. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820b75cf

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multiple sclerosis, cognitive rehabilitation, neuroimaging, fMRI, cognitive remediation, cognition

Citation: Chiaravalloti ND, Genova HM and DeLuca J (2015) Cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: the role of plasticity. Front. Neurol. 6:67. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00067

Received: 31 January 2015; Paper pending published: 03 March 2015;
Accepted: 12 March 2015; Published online: 02 April 2015.

Edited by:

Maria Assunta Rocca, Università Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Italy

Reviewed by:

Flavia Mattioli, Spedali Civili, Italy
Laura Vacchi, Università Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Italy

Copyright: © 2015 Chiaravalloti, Genova and DeLuca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nancy D. Chiaravalloti, Neuropsychology and Neuroscience Laboratory, Kessler Foundation, 300 Executive Drive, Suite 70, West Orange, NJ 07052, USA e-mail: nchiaravalloti@kesslerfoundation.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.