Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 25 July 2017
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2017.00053

Sensory Processing in the Dorsolateral Striatum: The Contribution of Thalamostriatal Pathways

  • 1Neural and Behavioral Sciences, Center for Neural Engineering, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, United States
  • 2Molecular Neurobiology Laboratory, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, United States
  • 3Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, United States

The dorsal striatum has two functionally-defined subdivisions: a dorsomedial striatum (DMS) region involved in mediating goal-directed behaviors that require conscious effort, and a dorsolateral striatum (DLS) region involved in the execution of habitual behaviors in a familiar sensory context. Consistent with its presumed role in forming stimulus-response (S-R) associations, neurons in DLS receive massive inputs from sensorimotor cortex and are responsive to both active and passive sensory stimulation. While several studies have established that corticostriatal inputs contribute to the stimulus-induced responses observed in the DLS, there is growing awareness that the thalamus has a significant role in conveying sensory-related information to DLS and other parts of the striatum. The thalamostriatal projections to DLS originate mainly from the caudal intralaminar region, which contains the parafascicular (Pf) nucleus, and from higher-order thalamic nuclei such as the medial part of the posterior (POm) nucleus. Based on recent findings, we hypothesize that the thalamostriatal projections from these two regions exert opposing influences on the expression of behavioral habits. This article reviews the subcortical circuits that regulate the transmission of sensory information through these thalamostriatal projection systems, and describes the evidence that indicates these circuits could be manipulated to ameliorate the symptoms of Parkinson’s disease (PD) and related neurological disorders.

In the 19th century, William James wrote that “Any sequence of mental action which has been frequently repeated tends to perpetuate itself; so that we find ourselves automatically prompted to think, feel, or do what we have been before accustomed to think, feel, or do under like circumstances, without any consciously formed purpose, or anticipation of results” (James, 1887). This statement identifies the critical features that distinguish habitual behaviors from those that require conscious effort. While “consciously formed” actions are aimed at acquiring specific goals or “anticipated results”, habits are “automatically prompted” if they are “frequently repeated” in a similar context or “like circumstances”.

More than 125 years later, the neural basis for this behavioral dichotomy is only partially understood. Substantial evidence indicates that the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) is involved with encoding the relationship between conscious actions and the value of the outcomes produced by those actions, which are classified as goal-directed behaviors. By comparison, the dorsolateral striatum (DLS) is necessary for the expression of behavioral habits that are automatically evoked in a familiar context (Yin and Knowlton, 2006; Balleine et al., 2009; Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010; Devan et al., 2011; Seger and Spiering, 2011). In fact, the strong relationship between sensory context and habitual behavior has led to the view that DLS is needed to form stimulus-response (S-R) associations (Graybiel, 2008; Devan et al., 2011).

Substantial evidence indicates that DLS neurons respond to sensory stimulation, but the neural basis for these responses is in dispute. While some results suggest that DLS receives its sensory inputs entirely from sensorimotor cortex (Pidoux et al., 2011; Reig and Silberberg, 2014; Wilson, 2014), direct comparisons of stimulus-evoked discharges in DLS and primary somatosensory cortex (SI) indicate that other brain regions must be involved in shaping the sensory responses in DLS (Mowery et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014). In fact, the dorsal striatum is innervated by several thalamic nuclei that process sensory information, but the functions of most thalamostriatal systems remain unclear. While much is known about the centromedian-parafascicular (CM-Pf) complex and its functional role (Ding et al., 2010; Galvan and Smith, 2011; Smith et al., 2014), the potential influence exerted by posteromedial nucleus (POm) on striatal responsiveness is just beginning to be recognized (Watson et al., 2015).

After reviewing the evidence indicating that the striatum receives sensory inputs from both cortex and thalamus, we will describe the experimental results implicating the POm in transmitting sensory information to DLS. As part of this, we will describe how POm is regulated by a multi-synaptic circuit in which the superior colliculus activates the zona incerta (ZI), which sends GABAergic projections to POm. This feedforward inhibitory circuit is functionally significant because deep brain stimulation (DBS) in ZI produces beneficial effects in the treatment of Parkinson’s disease (PD) and related movement disorders (Sun et al., 2008; Caire et al., 2013; Garcia-Garcia et al., 2016). To the extent that the sensory information conveyed from POm facilitates the behavioral functions of the DLS, the therapeutic effects of ZI stimulation in humans could be mediated by disrupting the inhibitory influence exerted by ZI on the human homolog of POm.

Dual Control Systems in the Striatum

One of the first indications that the striatum processes sensory information came from studies that used a water maze to compare different neural systems in remembering visual stimuli and spatial locations. In one study (Packard and McGaugh, 1992), two partially-submerged balls labeled by horizontal or vertical strips were used to mark a hidden platform (correct choice) or a thin pedestal (incorrect choice). In the spatial task, the rat learned to find the escape platform by swimming to the same spatial location regardless of the pattern on the target ball. In the S-R version of the task, the platform’s location was varied but it could be located by swimming to the ball with the same pattern. Fornix lesions impaired performance on the spatial task, but did not impair the S-R habit. Conversely, striatal lesions impaired performance on the S-R task, but not on the spatial task.

Subsequent work showed that lesions of DLS alone were sufficient to establish its role in stimulus-controlled behavior. Using a similar water maze task, rats with bilateral DMS lesions still swam to different locations marked by the same visual cue. By contrast, rats with bilateral DLS lesions always swam to the same spatial location (Devan and White, 1999; Devan et al., 1999).

Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Controlled Behaviors

Studies that analyzed differences between goal-directed and stimulus-controlled behaviors have been instrumental in identifying the behavioral functions of the striatum. These behavioral categories are often distinguished by operant conditioning paradigms that vary either the value of a reward or its probability of delivery. If a rat is trained to press a lever for food that is subsequently paired with an aversive agent, this devaluation in reward reduces lever-pressing behavior (Adams and Dickinson, 1981; Dickinson, 1985). Consistent with this example, goal-directed behaviors are sensitive to changes in reward value. By contrast, the defining characteristic of a stimulus-controlled habit is its resistance to reward devaluation (Adams, 1982; Dickinson, 1985).

Most behaviors start as goal-directed, but they can be transformed into a behavioral habit by prolonged overtraining or by reducing the predictability of the reward (Dickinson et al., 1983). When goal-directed behaviors are repeatedly expressed in the same sensory context, their transformation into a behavioral habit has several adaptive advantages. In addition to increased efficiency and other improvements in skill that normally ensue with repeated performance, habitual expression of a behavior in a specific context allows valuable neural resources to be devoted to other cognitive tasks that are more demanding (Graybiel, 2008). Learning to drive a car, for example, requires focused attention during the initial training stages. With repeated practice, however, acquisition of good driving habits allows a driver to engage in thoughtful conversation while navigating a well-known route on “auto-pilot”.

Functional Roles of DMS and DLS

Selective inactivation of DMS or DLS indicates that these brain regions are differentially involved in the production of goal-directed and habitual behaviors (Redgrave et al., 2010). After a behavioral habit is acquired, DLS inactivation causes habitual activity to be replaced by goal-directed actions that are mediated by the intact DMS (Yin et al., 2004; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Conversely, when DMS is inactivated, goal-directed behaviors are replaced by motor habits because the DLS remains intact (Yin et al., 2005).

Despite their differential involvement in goal-directed and stimulus-controlled behaviors, both DMS and DLS are considered to be cortically driven. As shown in Figure 1, goal-directed behaviors are usually associated with interactions between prefrontal cortex and DMS, while habitual behaviors are generally considered as being dependent on projections from sensorimotor cortex to the DLS (Yin et al., 2004, 2005; Yin and Knowlton, 2006; Redgrave et al., 2010; Hernández et al., 2015). In this scheme, the thalamus is part of a multi-synaptic loop that integrates output signals from both the dorsomedial and dorsolateral processing streams of the basal ganglia and, in turn, transmits this processed information to motor cortex.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Basal ganglia processing streams involved in the expression of goal-directed and stimulus-controlled behaviors. According to the prevailing view, virtually all sensory information is transmitted to the striatum by a series of thalamocortical and corticostriatal connections. The thalamus also sends direct projections to the striatum, but its functional influence is poorly understood.

The thalamus also sends projections to both the DMS and DLS. Thalamostriatal projections are known to be derived from a phylogenetically-old system that has a role in behavioral selection (McHaffie et al., 2005), but the precise nature of this role and the capacity of thalamostriatal systems for transmitting sensory-specific information has not been fully elucidated. A growing body of evidence, however, indicates that POm transmits sensory information to DLS, presumably to facilitate the expression of habitual behavior.

DLS Involvement in Sensorimotor Habits

Chronic recordings of DLS neurons across successive training sessions have led to several insights about the possible mechanisms by which goal-directed behaviors are transformed into behavioral habits that are encoded in DLS. In one study, DLS responses and forelimb EMG activity were recorded simultaneously as a rat learned to press a lever for a reward in a specific sensory context (Carelli et al., 1997). Initially, the forelimb-sensitive neurons in DLS were activated prior to each cued lever press, but these neuronal responses gradually subsided as training progressed. The neurons continued to respond, however, during other forelimb movements or in response to external cutaneous stimulation. The disparity between conditioned-induced suppression of DLS activity and the persistent responsiveness to other forms of active or passive sensory stimulation suggests that the emergence of habitual behavior is associated with neuroplasticity in specific neural circuits. Furthermore, the persistence of DLS responses to some forms of sensory stimulation suggests that sensory information can be transmitted to DLS by routes that are not altered by the acquisition of a behavioral habit.

Striatal Encoding of Sequence Learning

Several chronic recording studies indicate that DLS activity is altered during the acquisition of habits that consist of specific behavioral sequences. In two related studies, rats were trained to navigate a T-maze by turning right or left according to a cue that signaled the location of a reward (Jog et al., 1999; Barnes et al., 2005). Initially, DLS neurons discharged continuously as the rat proceeded through the maze, but with progressive training the DLS activity was transformed so that neurons discharged at the start and end of each trial but not during the middle part of the task. This “beginning-and-end bracketing pattern” in DLS could represent the neural correlate for “chunking” multiple behavioral elements into a specific sequence (Graybiel, 1998; Smith and Graybiel, 2014). In fact, behavioral lesion experiments have confirmed that DLS is necessary for encoding behavioral habits that consist of specific sequences of lever presses (Yin, 2010).

The expression of behavioral habits in a familiar context has prompted the belief that DLS is needed to form S-R associations. According to this view, habits that consist of a specific sequence of behavioral elements could be encoded by a chain of S-R associations in DLS, in which the sensorimotor elements of each behavioral action serve as stimuli for evoking the next behavioral response. But this possibility seems to be contradicted by the task-bracketing pattern of neural activity observed in DLS (Jog et al., 1999; Barnes et al., 2005; Smith and Graybiel, 2014).

Suppression of DLS activity during the interval between the start and end of a well-learned behavioral sequence suggests that each element in the behavioral sequence is not correlated with a specific S-R association in DLS. While stimulus-induced information might be transmitted faithfully to DLS during the initial stages of behavioral acquisition, repetitive training appears to recruit circuit mechanisms that alter DLS activity as a behavioral sequence becomes habitual, thereby gating certain sensory inputs so that only the start and end of the behavioral habit are correlated with DLS responsiveness (Smith and Graybiel, 2014). Relevant to this point, a similar task-bracketing pattern also appears in infralimbic cortex, which suggests that similar changes in striatal and cortical activity are essential for the consolidation of a behavioral habit (Smith and Graybiel, 2014).

Other chronic recording studies have confirmed that striatal neurons can encode the beginning, the end, or both the start and finish of a well-learned behavioral sequence (Jin and Costa, 2010). But these studies have also revealed additional types of striatal neurons, including those that show sustained activity throughout an entire behavioral sequence (Jin et al., 2014; Jin and Costa, 2015). In experiments in which mice habitually performed bar presses in rapid succession, many striatal neurons were continuously active throughout the entire bar-press sequence, and modulations in their rate of activity were correlated with the execution of individual bar presses (Jin et al., 2014). This suggests that subpopulations of striatal neurons encode sequences of individual motor acts by discharging at an increased rate as each motor element is expressed. By contrast, when primary motor cortex (MI) was recorded during these motor sequences, half of the neurons signaled the start or end of the sequence, but very few of them displayed activity changes that were correlated with individual motor elements (Jin et al., 2014). For those striatal neurons that encode each motor action, this pattern must depend on circuit mechanisms that extend beyond the corticostriatal signals sent from MI cortex.

While it is clear that sensorimotor information is transmitted to DLS during consolidation of a behavioral habit, there is considerable variation in the dynamics of the responses recorded in different subpopulations of DLS neurons during this learning process. While some changes undoubtedly involve neuroplasticity, the exact mechanisms are unclear. Dopamine signals have been identified as mediators in striatal learning (Surmeier et al., 2007; Shen et al., 2008), and some have suggested that the dopamine signals involved in reinforcing striatal detection of a sensory stimulus could selectively potentiate those corticostriatal inputs that transmit sensory-relevant signals (Donahue and Kreitzer, 2015). But the possibility that thalamostriatal inputs could contribute to the DLS responses that enable a behavioral habit has never been considered.

Innate Behaviors Regulated by DLS

Relevant to its role in mediating well-learned behaviors, the DLS is also involved in the expression of species-specific behaviors that are considered innate. In rodents, behaviors such as grooming and exploratory whisking do not depend on acquisition of a reward, but are characterized by highly-repetitive, sensory-guided stereotyped movements that are usually emitted in specific contexts.

Behavioral Grooming

Behavioral lesion experiments indicate that DLS has a role in grooming behavior, which consists of stereotyped head and limb movements that follow a predictable sequence known as a “syntactic chain”. Bilateral lesions at specific sites in the DLS disrupt grooming behavior by altering the sequence and duration of individual behavioral elements, or by omitting certain elements altogether even though the ability to make a specific grooming movement remains intact (Cromwell and Berridge, 1996).

The DLS lesions that affect grooming behavior are located at sites that receive dense inputs from the forelimb representations in the SI and MI cortical areas (Cromwell and Berridge, 1996; Brown et al., 1998; Hoffer and Alloway, 2001; Hoover et al., 2003). Neurons at these DLS sites are active during grooming behavior, and while some neuronal activity is correlated with specific grooming movements, most neurons appear to be encoding the repetition of a complete syntactic grooming chain (Aldridge and Berridge, 1998). Lesions in the MI regions that project to DLS have no effect on the pattern of grooming behavior (Berridge and Whishaw, 1992), and this suggests that another brain region sends DLS the information needed to maintain the syntactic chain. Likewise, MI areas that project to DLS are needed during the acquisition of a well-learned motor skill such as bar-pressing, but are not needed once the behavior has been consolidated (Kawai et al., 2015). This suggests that other inputs to the striatum are responsible for the expression of a well-learned motor habit once it has been consolidated.

Exploratory Whisking

Rats and mice actively sweep their whiskers to acquire sensory information about local surroundings and objects during behavioral exploration. Active whisking consists of highly-repetitive, rhythmic excursions of the whiskers, usually in the range of 5–15 Hz (Welker, 1964; Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003). Prior to contacting an object, whisking is bilaterally coordinated so that whisker movements on both sides of the head are relatively symmetric in terms of their frequency and amplitude (Gao et al., 2001; Mitchinson et al., 2007).

Substantial evidence implicates DLS with a role in exploratory whisking. The caudal DLS receives dense overlapping projections from several cortical whisker regions (Brown et al., 1998; Alloway et al., 1999, 2006, 2009; Hoffer and Alloway, 2001), and DLS neurons display rhythmic discharge patterns during exploratory whisking in the awake behaving rat (Carelli and West, 1991). Furthermore, in the lightly-anesthetized rat, neurons in caudal DLS display high-fidelity neuronal discharges in response to repetitive whisker deflections (Mowery et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012).

Consistent with other habitual behaviors, exploratory whisking does not depend on a rewarded outcome, and certain aspects of exploratory whisking are characterized by stereotyped patterns of whisker excursions that resemble S-R associations. For example, when whiskers on one side contact an external stimulus during active whisking, the contralateral whiskers automatically exhibit larger sweeping movements while the contacted whiskers display smaller excursions (Sachdev et al., 2003; Mitchinson et al., 2007).

Exploratory whisking is characterized by a series of epochs in which whisks are repetitively emitted at a specific frequency and amplitude for 1–2 s before another whisking epoch is emitted at a new frequency and amplitude (Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003). This pattern suggests that whisking behavior consists of a series of behavioral segments that are “chunked” together. Whether these frequency-defined epochs follow a specific syntactic chain is unknown because no study has examined whether DLS lesions alter either the kinematic or syntactic features of whisking behavior.

The repetitive stereotyped nature of whisking, along with the fact that DLS neurons respond to both active and passive whisker movements, has prompted several investigators to use the whisker system to analyze sensory processing in DLS (West, 1998; Mowery et al., 2011; Pidoux et al., 2011; Reig and Silberberg, 2014). The peripheral whiskers are densely innervated and, consequently, a major advantage of this model is that the whisker representations in thalamus, cortex and DLS are disproportionately larger than other somatic representations in these regions (Chapin and Lin, 1984; Fabri and Burton, 1991; Brown et al., 1998; Alloway et al., 2003, 2006).

Neural Circuits for Transmitting Sensory Information to DLS

Substantial effort has been devoted to elucidating the input-output connections of the striatum, including the neuronal routes that convey sensory information to DLS. While many studies have characterized the topography and other aspects of corticostriatal connectivity, far fewer have been devoted to elucidating the subcortical circuits that influence striatal processing. Nonetheless, as indicated by Figure 2, several neuronal circuits are capable of transmitting sensory-related information to the striatum without involving the cerebral cortex.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Circuit diagram illustrating multiple neuronal pathways for conveying sensory inputs to the striatum. The thick afferent and efferent connections of the POm indicate the most direct route for transmitting somesthetic information to the DLS. Abbreviations: DMS, dorsomedial striatum; DLS, dorsolateral striatum; MI, primary motor cortex; Pf, parafascicular nucleus; POm, posteromedial nucleus; SI, primary somatosensory cortex; SNc, substantia nigra pars compacta; VPM, ventroposteromedial nucleus. Adapted from Watson et al. (2015).

Corticostriatal Projections

The DLS receives a massive number of divergent and convergent projections from sensorimotor cortex (McGeorge and Faull, 1989; Brown et al., 1998; Wright et al., 1999; Hoffer and Alloway, 2001; Hintiryan et al., 2016). The combined influence of multiple divergent projections from SI, SII, MI and related cortical regions is assumed to be responsible for the fact that DLS has a discernible, but relatively crude somatotopic organization (Carelli and West, 1991; Brown et al., 1998).

Several anatomical studies indicate that corticostriatal projections are characterized by substantial amounts of divergence. Tracer deposits in the SI and MI limb of primates have consistently revealed a “one-to-many” corticostriatal projection pattern (Flaherty and Graybiel, 1991, 1994). Similarly, in rats, focal tracer deposits in a single cortical column of the SI barrel field produced separate and distinct patches of labeled terminals in the DLS neuropil (Alloway et al., 1998).

Corticostriatal divergence has also been demonstrated with the 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) labeling technique (Brown, 1992). Stimulation of different body parts (e.g., trunk or limbs) reveals a crude somatotopic map that resembles the overlying cortical topography, but 2-DG patches representing different body sites are often juxtaposed in different combinations. This “combinatorial map” suggests that every DLS site could receive converging inputs from separate cortical regions that are activated during a behavioral activity. Relevant to this result, DLS recordings have shown that adjacent neurons often respond to stimulation of separate body parts (Carelli and West, 1991).

The complexity of the functional topography of DLS is further emphasized by the presence of substantial corticostriatal convergence. Corresponding somatotopic representations in SI, SII and MI project to overlapping parts of DLS (Flaherty and Graybiel, 1993, 1995; Alloway et al., 2000; Hoffer and Alloway, 2001), and ultrastructural analysis indicates that projections from rat SI and MI whisker regions converge on individual DLS neurons (Ramanathan et al., 2002). This is significant because corticostriatal axons have few collaterals, they traverse the striatal neuropil along a predominantly straight path, and each axon contributes few synapses to each medium spiny neuron (MSN) that it contacts (Wilson, 1995; Kincaid et al., 1998). These findings suggest that DLS discharges must depend on synchronous activation of multiple cortical areas that send convergent projections to the DLS.

The DLS also receives inputs from sensorimotor cortex in the contralateral hemisphere (Wilson, 1987; Wright et al., 2001; Reiner et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2009). Consistent with these intra-telencephalic projections (Reiner et al., 2003), small unilateral tracer deposits in DLS have revealed mirror-image distributions of neuronal labeling throughout multiple sensorimotor cortical areas in both hemispheres (Alloway et al., 2006). Hence, the large number of cortical sites in both hemispheres that send converging projections to DLS underscores the number of different combinations of cortical neurons that can cooperate with each other to activate DLS.

Thalamostriatal Projections

Thalamostriatal projections originate from several nuclei, but not all of them process sensory information. The thalamic nuclei that are principally involved in conveying sensory-related information to the striatum include POm, the CM/Pf complex and other intralaminar nuclei, and the lateral posterior (LP) and lateral dorsal (LD) nuclei. While the lateral Pf nucleus in rodents represents the homolog of the primate CM nucleus (Galvan and Smith, 2011), the rodent POm is considered to be the homolog of the anterior part of the primate pulvinar (Butler and Hodos, 2005). Both Pf and POm send projections to the sensorimotor striatum (i.e., DLS in rats and putamen in primates).

Most thalamostriatal research has focused on the CM/Pf complex because it contains more thalamostriatal neurons than any other region. Consistent with evidence suggesting that tecto-thalamic pathways represent a phylogenetically-old system for transmitting sensory information to the basal ganglia (McHaffie et al., 2005), the CM/Pf complex receives a large number of multimodal sensory inputs from the intermediate and deep layers of the superior colliculus (Krout et al., 2001). The CM/Pf complex is also notable for having relatively weak connections with the cerebral cortex (Galvan and Smith, 2011).

The LP and LD nuclei receive sensory inputs that are largely visual in nature. The LP nucleus receives inputs from both the upper collicular layers and from several visual cortical areas (Donnelly et al., 1983; Sugita et al., 1983; Abramson and Chalupa, 1988; Lane et al., 1993, 1997; Roth et al., 2016). By comparison, the LD nucleus receives visual-related input from the pretectal area, the intermediate layers of the superior colliculus, and the occipital cortex (Arango and Scalia, 1978; Robertson, 1983; Thompson and Robertson, 1987).

The POm nucleus is distinct from LP, LD and the CM/Pf complex in that it receives very few inputs from the superior colliculus (Roger and Cadusseau, 1984). Instead, POm is dominated by ascending and descending somesthetic inputs that originate, respectively, from the trigeminal nuclei and from multiple sensorimotor cortical areas (Chiaia et al., 1991a,b; Diamond et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1994; Veinante et al., 2000; Alloway et al., 2003; Killackey and Sherman, 2003).

In contrast to the CM/Pf complex, which has sparse connections with cortex, the POm, LP and LD nuclei are strongly connected with specific cortical areas (Galvan and Smith, 2011; Smith et al., 2014). Furthermore, these thalamic nuclei and their respective cortical targets send converging projections to specific striatal regions (Groenewegen and Berendse, 1994). The convergence of thalamostriatal and corticostriatal projections from sites that are reciprocally-connected suggests an obvious circuit mechanism for promoting cooperative activation of their common targets in the striatum.

Rodent studies indicate that thalamostriatal projections from LP, LD, POm and Pf differ significantly in terms of the striatal regions that they innervate. While Pf projections terminate throughout the dorsal striatum (Alloway et al., 2014), the LD, LP and POm nuclei innervate more restricted striatal territories. The LD nucleus is reciprocally connected with the agranular motor and posterior parietal cortices (Chandler et al., 1992; Reep and Corwin, 1999), and it projects densely to the dorsal periphery of the rat striatum (Kamishina et al., 2008). The LP nucleus is connected with the medial agranular and posterior parietal cortices (Reep and Corwin, 2009), and it innervates the dorsal central striatum and a narrow part of the rostral DLS (Kamishina et al., 2008). In contrast to these thalamic nuclei, POm is the only thalamic nucleus that innervates DLS without projecting to any other striatal region (Deschênes et al., 1995, 1996; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014).

Comparisons of Pf and POm Neurons and their Connections

The DLS receives projections from both Pf and POm, but these nuclei differ substantially in terms of their neuronal morphology. Neurons in Pf have an oval soma from which a few poorly-branched dendrites extend over distances ranging between 0.5 mm and 1.5 mm (Hazlett et al., 1976; Deschênes et al., 1996; Lacey et al., 2007). These relatively long, poorly-branched dendrites are considered ideal for integrating different types of information from a variety of sources (Deschênes et al., 1995).

By contrast, POm neurons have highly-branched dendritic arbors that resemble the classic description of bushy relay neurons in ventral posteromedial nucleus (VPM) and other thalamic relay nuclei (Deschênes et al., 1995; Ohno et al., 2012). These bushy dendrites extend 300–400 μm from the soma in all directions, thereby endowing POm neurons with a large surface area for receiving many synaptic contacts within a confined space. In addition to receiving ascending projections from the spinal trigeminal nucleus (Chiaia et al., 1991a; Williams et al., 1994; Veinante et al., 2000), POm also receives descending corticothalamic projections from SI cortex that form large synaptic terminals known as “drivers”, as well as smaller synaptic terminals from MI (Sherman and Guillery, 1998; Alloway et al., 2008). Ultrastructural analysis demonstrates that these trigeminothalamic and corticothalamic projections converge synaptically onto common neuronal targets in POm (Groh et al., 2013). Consistent with this finding, POm neurons are most likely to discharge when they receive near-coincident inputs from these converging projections (Groh et al., 2013).

Differential Projection Patterns of Pf and POm Neurons

Axonal projections from Pf and POm differ in several important respects. Axons emerging from Pf pass through the reticular nucleus, where they emit a few sparsely-branched collaterals before proceeding towards the basal ganglia to provide collateral innervation to the globus pallidus (GP), subthalamic nucleus (STN) and entopeduncular nucleus before diverging into multiple branches that terminate throughout the dorsal striatum, including its medial and lateral sectors (Deschênes et al., 1996; Lacey et al., 2007; Alloway et al., 2014). Although Pf sends some projections to motor cortex, the density of these cortical terminals is significantly lower than in the striatum (Galvan and Smith, 2011).

By contrast, the caudal half of POm projects to the caudal part of DLS, but these connections represent collaterals of the main axon, which innervates sensorimotor cortex (Deschênes et al., 1995; Ohno et al., 2012). Furthermore, whereas Pf projects sparsely to sensorimotor cortex, the projections from caudal POm terminate densely in SI, SII and to a lesser extent in MI (Lu and Lin, 1993; Wimmer et al., 2010; Viaene et al., 2011; Ohno et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2012). In fact, the thalamocortical projections from POm terminate most densely in layer Va (Wimmer et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2012), which contains most of the corticostriatal neurons that project to DLS (Reiner et al., 2003). Significantly, the apical dendrites of layer V corticostriatal neurons receive very few synaptic contacts from the ventroposterior nuclei (i.e., VPM and VPL), even though they extend into layer IV, which is densely innervated by these thalamic nuclei (Hersch and White, 1982). Hence, when compared to the classic lemniscal circuit for transmitting somesthetic information to SI cortex, differences in the density and cellular distribution of synaptic contacts suggest that POm is probably more efficacious in activating corticostriatal neurons than the thalamocortical projections from VPM or VPL.

As shown in Figure 3, the striatal territories innervated by Pf and POm are significantly different. Although Pf is considerably smaller than POm, it provides extensive innervation throughout the medial and lateral parts of the dorsal striatum. By comparison, thalamostriatal projections from caudal POm terminate in DLS, especially its caudal half, but do not innervate any other striatal region (Ohno et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014). Consistent with these different projection patterns, the axonal arbors of Pf branch much more extensively than those originating from POm (Deschênes et al., 1995; Lacey et al., 2007).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Patterns of striatal innervation revealed by placing an anterograde tracer in Pf (top) or POm (bottom). (A) Photomicrographs of adjacent sections depict cytoarchitecture and biotinylated dextran amine (BDA) deposits in Pf or POm of two different rats. (B) Rostrocaudal series showing spatial distribution of BDA-labeled axonal varicosities in the striatum. Each series based on superimposing reconstructions from three rats that received similar BDA injections in Pf or POm. Numbers indicate distance (mm) from bregma. Scale bars: 500 microns in (A). Abbreviations: fr, fasciculus retroflexus; GP, globus pallidus; VPM, ventroposteromedial nucleus. Adapted from Alloway et al. (2014).

Although individual neurons in Pf contribute more axonal terminals to the striatum, POm is much larger than Pf and contains many more neurons that project to the DLS. In fact, as illustrated by Figure 4, retrograde tracer injections in DLS revealed that POm contains the largest number of thalamic neurons that project to DLS (Smith et al., 2012). These numerical differences, along with the fact that POm receives direct trigeminal inputs and is strongly connected with corticostriatal neurons in SI, suggest that POm is likely to be an important source of the somesthetic information transmitted to DLS.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Proportion of labeled neurons in different brain regions after injecting a retrograde tracer into the right DLS of seven rats. More than 70% of the projections originate from the two cortical hemispheres, nearly 20% from the basolateral amygdala (BLA), and 10% from the thalamus. Among thalamic neurons that project to DLS, 40% reside in the medial part of the posterior nucleus (POm). Adapted from Smith et al. (2012).

Synaptic Connectivity of Pf and POm Neurons

Thalamostriatal projections from Pf and other intralaminar nuclei form axo-dendritic synapses on both MSNs and on large cholinergic interneurons, which display high rates of spontaneous activity and are often called tonically-active neurons (Dube et al., 1988; Meredith and Wouterlood, 1990; Lapper and Bolam, 1992; Raju et al., 2006; Lacey et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2014). Innervation of cholinergic interneurons by CM/Pf is noteworthy because electrical stimulation of CM/Pf evokes a burst of activity in these cells that is followed by a sequential cascade of striatal events that differentially alter the influence of corticostriatal projections on the MSNs associated with the direct or indirect pathways (Ding et al., 2010; Thorn and Graybiel, 2010).

No study has characterized the synaptic contacts of the thalamostriatal projections from POm. Presumably, if POm projections have the same synaptic pattern reported for other non-CM/Pf nuclei (Dube et al., 1988; Raju et al., 2006; Lacey et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2014), axonal projections from POm should contact the spines of MSN dendrites. If true, this would be significant because corticostriatal terminals also form synaptic contacts on the dendritic spines of MSNs (Dube et al., 1988; Wright et al., 1999; Raju et al., 2008). Given that individual neurons in POm send collateral projections to both DLS and corticostriatal neurons in layer Va (Deschênes et al., 1996; Smith et al., 2012), this putative synaptic configuration should increase the likelihood that the dendritic spines of DLS neurons receive near-coincident excitatory inputs, first from POm and then from the corticostriatal neurons activated by POm.

Striatal Responses to Sensory Stimulation

The first experiments that recorded sensory responses in the striatum were done on cats in the 1960s (Albe-Fessard et al., 1960; Sedgwick and Williams, 1967). Since then, numerous studies have shown that striatal neurons in primates, cats and rodents can respond to visual, auditory, or somesthetic stimulation (Schneider and Lidsky, 1981; Crutcher and DeLong, 1984; DeLong et al., 1984; Alexander and DeLong, 1985; Lidsky et al., 1985; Strecker et al., 1985; Hikosaka et al., 1989; Carelli and West, 1991; Bordi and LeDoux, 1992; Bordi et al., 1993; Carelli et al., 1997; West, 1998; Cromwell et al., 2007; Schulz et al., 2009; Mowery et al., 2011; Pidoux et al., 2011; Syed et al., 2011; Hawking and Gerdjikov, 2013; Znamenskiy and Zador, 2013; Sippy et al., 2015; Xiong et al., 2015). In fact, depending on their location, striatal neurons may integrate inputs from two or more sensory modalities (Krauthamer, 1979; Chudler et al., 1995; Nagy et al., 2006; Reig and Silberberg, 2014).

The striatum can be activated by different sensory modalities, but the latencies of the responses vary considerably depending on the modality of the stimulus. Somesthetic-induced responses in the striatum have latencies of 5–10 ms in rats (Mowery et al., 2011; Reig and Silberberg, 2014), and 10–20 ms in cats and primates (Schneider and Lidsky, 1981; DeLong et al., 1984). Striatal responses to auditory stimuli also have short latencies of 10–20 ms (Bordi and LeDoux, 1992; Bordi et al., 1993; Cromwell et al., 2007). By contrast, striatal responses to visual stimuli have much longer latencies, ranging from 100 ms to 150 ms (Schulz et al., 2009; Reig and Silberberg, 2014).

These latency differences suggest that auditory and somesthetic information are transmitted to the striatum across relatively short neuronal routes, whereas visual information must reach the striatum by longer multi-synaptic circuits. In fact, just as somesthetic information can be transmitted directly to DLS by thalamostriatal projections from caudal POm (Ohno et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014), some reports indicate that auditory information can be transmitted directly from the medial geniculate nucleus to the ventral DLS (Ryugo and Killackey, 1974; LeDoux et al., 1983, 1986; Lin et al., 1984).

Although some striatal neurons respond to auditory or visual stimulation, those responding to somesthetic stimulation are much greater in number and occupy more striatal territory (Krauthamer, 1979; Lidsky et al., 1985; Reig and Silberberg, 2014). Furthermore, consistent with the role of somesthesis in behavioral habits such as grooming and whisking, the DLS in rats is concentrated with neurons that respond to both passive and active movements of the limbs and whiskers (Carelli and West, 1991; Brown, 1992; Mowery et al., 2011; Pidoux et al., 2011; Hawking and Gerdjikov, 2013; Reig and Silberberg, 2014).

Neural Basis of Stimulus-Induced Responses in DLS

Neurons in rodent DLS respond to sensory stimulation, but the neural basis for these responses has not been resolved. Most projections to DLS originate from sensorimotor cortex (Brown et al., 1998; Alloway et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2012), and this fact is partly responsible for the widespread view that sensory-evoked responses in DLS are due entirely to corticostriatal inputs. This view, however, is not supported by experiments in which extracellular discharges were recorded simultaneously in DLS and SI of lightly-anesthetized rats (Mowery et al., 2011).

Comparison of DLS and SI Response Properties

As shown by Figure 5, when multiple whiskers are mechanically deflected in tandem, regular-spiking neurons in SI barrel cortex habituate quickly to repetitive whisker stimulation, but MSNs in DLS adapt very slowly (Mowery et al., 2011). Furthermore, whereas DLS neurons display high-fidelity “doublet” discharges in response to rapid back-and-forth whisker deflections, neurons in SI respond only to the initial phase of the same back-and-forth whisker motion.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Neuronal responses recorded simultaneously in primary somatosensory cortex (SI; top) and DLS (bottom) during computer-controlled whisker deflections administered at 2, 5 and 8-Hz. Nearly two dozen whiskers were deflected in tandem in which each deflection consisted of back-and-forth movements in a 50-ms period. Photomicrographs illustrate the recording sites (asterisks) in SI and DLS. Peristimulus time histograms show that SI neurons adapt rapidly to repetitive whisker movements whereas DLS neurons adapt slowly. First deflection in each epoch is classified as 1-Hz because it is separated from previous deflections by at least 1 s. Adapted from Mowery et al. (2011).

Rapid adaptation is a classic feature of regular-spiking neurons in rodent SI cortex (Simons and Carvell, 1989; Ahissar et al., 2001; Khatri et al., 2004; Melzer et al., 2006; Chakrabarti and Alloway, 2009). This phenomenon is related to the fact that subthreshold responses in pyramidal neurons are characterized by EPSP-IPSP sequences in which an initial excitation is followed by prolonged inhibition (Innocenti and Manzoni, 1972; Creutzfeldt et al., 1974; Carvell and Simons, 1988; Moore and Nelson, 1998). Long-lasting cortical inhibition, which is mediated by local feed-forward and recurrent inhibitory circuits (Alloway et al., 1989; Swadlow, 2003), reduces the responsiveness of regular-spiking neurons when sequential sensory inputs are separated by short time intervals (Laskin and Spencer, 1979). Furthermore, corticostriatal neurons in layer V have intrinsic biophysical properties that cause them to adapt much more rapidly than other cortical neurons (Hattox and Nelson, 2007). These findings raise serious doubts about the possibility that rapidly-adapting corticostriatal neurons are capable of driving the slowly-adapting responses that have been observed in DLS (Mowery et al., 2011).

Comparisons of stimulus-induced response latencies in SI and DLS add further support to the view that SI cortex is not the only source of sensory inputs to DLS. As shown by cumulative latency distributions in Figure 6, DLS and SI neurons respond at the same time during low frequency whisker stimulation (1 or 2 Hz), but the SI neurons discharged after DLS neurons when the whiskers were deflected at higher frequencies (5 or 8 Hz). The DLS latency distributions were nearly identical at each tested frequency, but the distributions for SI were characterized by a shift towards longer latencies as stimulus frequency increased. This finding is consistent with the fact that prolonged cortical inhibition evoked by one stimulus will interfere with responses to subsequent stimuli that occur within intervals of 200 ms or less.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Cumulative distributions of response latencies of regular spiking and medium spiny neurons (MSNs) recorded, respectively, in SI and DLS during whisker deflections at different frequencies. At 8-Hz, nonresponsive neurons in SI are depicted by NR. Adapted from Mowery et al. (2011).

Some reports, however, have argued that stimulus-induced responses in DLS are determined by corticostriatal inputs. This view is based on studies in which whisker-evoked responses in DLS and SI cortex were recorded simultaneously in deeply-anesthetized animals (Pidoux et al., 2011; Hawking and Gerdjikov, 2013; Reig and Silberberg, 2014, 2016). In one study, for example, whole cell subthreshold recordings in DLS were conducted simultaneously with extracellular recordings in SI cortex during whisker stimulation of deeply anesthetized mice (Reig and Silberberg, 2014). In response to whisker movements evoked by air puffs, SI neurons discharged action potentials on a regular basis, but neuronal discharges were infrequently observed in DLS. The relative lack of discharges in DLS is not surprising because deep anesthesia suppresses the production of stimulus-evoked discharges in the striatum (West, 1998; Pidoux et al., 2011; Hawking and Gerdjikov, 2013). Consequently, subthreshold EPSPs were recorded to detect any stimulus-induced sensory input to DLS. Using this approach, Reig and Silberberg observed subthreshold responses in DLS immediately after the whisker-evoked neuronal discharges in SI. Based on this finding, they concluded that “responses in striatal neurons under our experimental conditions are generated primarily by cortical inputs without engaging a thalamostriatal shortcut (Mowery et al., 2011)”.

While this conclusion is valid for subthreshold responses recorded during deep anesthesia, several studies indicate that spontaneous and stimulus-induced activity in POm and DLS are suppressed by anesthesia or other manipulations that alter behavioral state (Carelli et al., 1997; West, 1998; Trageser et al., 2006; Masri et al., 2008). Stimulus-induced responses in POm are suppressed by GABAergic projections from ventral ZI (ZIv; Trageser and Keller, 2004; Lavallée et al., 2005), and this is significant because GABAergic transmission in the thalamus is enhanced by anesthesia (Detsch et al., 2002; Franks, 2008; Ying et al., 2009; Joksovic and Todorovic, 2010). These and other findings have prompted the view that POm transmits sensory information only in the awake state (Trageser et al., 2006; Urbain and Deschênes, 2007). However, if rats are maintained in a lightly-anesthetized state, as indicated by electrocorticographic (ECoG) activity ranging from 4 Hz to 6 Hz (Friedberg et al., 1999), we have consistently found that deflections of multiple whiskers will evoke neuronal discharges in both POm and DLS (Mowery et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012).

As indicated by Figure 7, neurons in POm and DLS respond to whisker deflections in the awake or lightly-anesthetized state, but become unresponsive during deep anesthesia. By contrast, SI neurons respond robustly to peripheral stimulation in both the awake and the deeply-anesthetized state, which is consistent with cortical mapping studies performed during deep anesthesia (Chapin and Lin, 1984). Although time-locked neuronal discharges and subthreshold responses can be detected in SI and DLS, respectively, during deep anesthesia (Pidoux et al., 2011; Reig and Silberberg, 2014), the loss of stimulus-induced DLS discharges during deep anesthesia is correlated with a similar loss of responsiveness in POm.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Stimulus-induced responses in POm, DLS and SI cortex as a function of behavioral state. In the lightly-anesthetized state, when electrocorticographic (ECoG) activity is 4–6 Hz, neurons in POm, DLS and SI display strong phasic responses to back-and-forth whisker deflections. When ECoG activity is dominated by frequencies below 1 Hz, however, neurons in POm and DLS are unresponsive, but neurons in SI cortex still display rapidly-adapting responses. Responses in the idealized peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) are based on studies that examined the effects of anesthesia and other behavioral states on somesthetic responses in POm (Trageser et al., 2006; Masri et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2015), DLS (West, 1998; Mowery et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014) and SI cortex (Chapin and Lin, 1984; Mowery et al., 2011).

Comparison of Stimulus-Induced Responses in POm and DLS

Simultaneous extracellular recordings of neuronal discharges in POm and DLS of lightly-anesthetized rats have revealed many similarities in the stimulus-induced responses in these two regions. In contrast to the rapid habituation observed in SI, neurons in POm and DLS display slowly-adapting responses during repetitive whisker stimulation (Mowery et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014). In fact, rapid back-and-forth whisker movements evoke “doublet” responses in both POm and DLS, but not in SI cortex. Furthermore, when POm and DLS neurons are recorded simultaneously during whisker stimulation, POm discharges consistently precede the DLS discharges by 1–2 ms regardless of stimulus frequency (Smith et al., 2012).

Several reports indicate that POm and DLS neurons have relatively complex response properties when compared to neurons in VPM or SI cortex. Neurons in VPM and SI barrel cortex are easily activated by deflections of a single whisker (Brumberg et al., 1996), but whisker-sensitive neurons in POm have larger receptive fields and are less responsive unless multiple whiskers are stimulated simultaneously (Chiaia et al., 1991b; Diamond et al., 1992; Sosnik et al., 2001). Similarly, DLS neurons may occasionally discharge in response to the motion of a single whisker, but most whisker-sensitive neurons in DLS require stimulation of multiple whiskers (Carelli and West, 1991; Mowery et al., 2011). Even in the awake state, neuronal discharges are not consistently evoked in DLS when only a single whisker is deflected (Sippy et al., 2015). When these facts are considered together, the relatively crude somatotopic organization and the prevalence of neurons with large receptive fields emphasizes the importance of global, spatially-distributed sensory stimulation for activating both POm and DLS.

Functions of the Thalamostriatal Projections from POm and Pf

Neurons in both Pf and POm respond to sensory inputs, but the stimulus-induced responses in these nuclei are significantly different. The CM/Pf complex receives multimodal sensory inputs from the intermediate and deep layers of the superior colliculus (Yamasaki et al., 1986; Nothias et al., 1988; Grunwerg and Krauthamer, 1992; Krout et al., 2001; Coizet et al., 2007; Schulz et al., 2009; Alloway et al., 2014), which process visual, auditory and somatosensory inputs (May, 2006). Consistent with this, recordings in both primates and rats have shown that neurons in the CM/Pf complex display multisensory responses, but these responses habituate very quickly during repeated stimulus presentations (Chiaia et al., 1991b; Grunwerg and Krauthamer, 1992; Matsumoto et al., 2001; Minamimoto and Kimura, 2002).

By contrast, POm receives very few inputs from the superior colliculus (Roger and Cadusseau, 1984), and the relative lack of auditory and visual inputs has prompted the view that POm is mainly concerned with processing somesthetic information (Diamond, 1995). Furthermore, POm neurons adapt very slowly to repetitive whisker stimulation (Sosnik et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014), and simultaneous recordings in Pf and POm indicate that response latencies are significantly shorter in POm than in Pf (Alloway et al., 2014). These facts are consistent with reports showing that POm receives somesthetic inputs directly from the trigeminal nuclei (Chiaia et al., 1991a,b; Veinante et al., 2000; Sosnik et al., 2001; Masri et al., 2008), whereas Pf depends on sensory information that is transmitted across longer multisynaptic routes that depend on inputs from the superior colliculus (see Figure 2).

Collicular Control of Pf and POm

The superior colliculus is known for having extensive connections with brainstem circuits that control eye movements and related aspects of visual orientation so that attention can be re-directed to unexpected, highly salient stimuli (Sparks, 1986; May, 2006). Consistent with these functions, the thalamostriatal projections from POm and Pf appear to be part of a feed-forward network that regulates communication between the superior colliculus and the basal ganglia.

Tracing studies indicate that superior colliculus projects to Pf and other intralaminar thalamic nuclei (Yamasaki et al., 1986; Yamasaki and Krauthamer, 1990; Krout et al., 2001; Alloway et al., 2014). In addition, the superior colliculus sends dense projections to ZIv, which is located immediately dorsal to the STN (Roger and Cadusseau, 1985; Mitrofanis, 2005; Alloway et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2015; Kita et al., 2016). The ZIv is notable because it is concentrated with GABAergic neurons that project to POm (Barthó et al., 2002; Watson et al., 2015), and the inhibitory influence of ZIv on POm was demonstrated by showing that ZI lesions enhance somesthetic responsiveness in POm (Trageser and Keller, 2004; Lavallée et al., 2005).

According to the circuit connections in Figure 2, activation of the superior colliculus should exert opposing influences on Pf and POm, both of which project to the striatum. As indicated by Figure 8, prior work has shown that Pf and other thalamic intralaminar neurons are activated by the superior colliculus (Grunwerg and Krauthamer, 1992). Consistent with the intervening inhibitory projections from ZIv to POm, Figure 8 also demonstrates that the superior colliculus can inhibit spontaneous activity in caudal POm (Watson et al., 2015). Furthermore, in contrast to the relatively brief activation of Pf that is typically evoked by collicular stimulation, neuronal activity in POm is inhibited by the superior colliculus for a prolonged period.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Electrical stimulation of the superior colliculus exerts opposing influences on Pf and POm. (A) Excitatory responses recorded in the Pf and centrolateral nuclei following electrical stimulation (arrows) of the superior colliculus. (B) Histology shows location of bipolar stimulating electrode in superior colliculus. Data reprinted with permission (Grunwerg and Krauthamer, 1992). Scale Bars: 10 ms (A), 1 mm (B). (C) PSTH shows inhibition of POm activity following electrical stimulation (arrow) of superior colliculus. Waveform scales: 1 ms, 100 μV. PSTH: 50 trials, 5-ms binwidths. (D,E) Photomicrographs depicting recording and stimulating sites (arrowheads) in POm and superior colliculus. Adapted from Watson et al. (2015).

Opposing Behavioral Functions of Pf and POm Striatal Projections

The differential influences exerted by the superior colliculus on Pf and POm suggest that the thalamostriatal projections from these nuclei have behavioral functions that are incompatible with each other. Although the precise nature of these behavioral functions remain unclear, the available data suggest that the CM/Pf complex has a role in facilitating corticostriatal activation of the indirect pathway (Ding et al., 2010), whereas the POm nucleus is involved in facilitating the functions of the direct pathway.

Thalamostriatal projections from the CM/Pf complex innervate all of the dorsal striatum and appear to be involved in gating the influence of corticostriatal projections throughout this region. In addition to contacting MSNs, these projections provide strong input to cholinergic interneurons (Meredith and Wouterlood, 1990; Lapper and Bolam, 1992; for review, see Smith et al., 2014). In brain slices, electrical stimulation of the CM/Pf complex evokes a burst of activity among cholinergic interneurons that is followed by a noticeable pause (Ding et al., 2010; Schulz and Reynolds, 2013; Doig et al., 2014). This burst-pause sequence among cholinergic interneurons is coupled to an immediate decrease in activity among all MSNs, which is followed by a selective increase in the sensitivity of the indirect pathway to corticostriatal inputs (Ding et al., 2010). By contrast, inhibition of the cholinergic interneurons reduces catalepsy and other motor deficits in a mouse model of PD (Maurice et al., 2015). Furthermore, these behavioral effects are associated with prolonged inhibition of the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNR), which suggests increased output of the direct pathway.

Converging evidence indicates that the indirect pathway has a suppressive effect on behavior, whereas the direct pathway facilitates motor output (Albin et al., 1989; DeLong, 1990; Kravitz et al., 2010; Sippy et al., 2015). In view of data showing that the CM/Pf complex is activated by unexpected stimuli that cause a shift in attention (Matsumoto et al., 2001; Minamimoto and Kimura, 2002), this thalamic region appears to be involved in suppressing an on-going behavior so that a more adaptive behavior can be selected (Ding et al., 2010; Thorn and Graybiel, 2010).

In contrast to the CM/Pf complex, the POm nucleus innervates the DLS but no other part of the striatum (Smith et al., 2012; Alloway et al., 2014). Given the importance of DLS for expressing habitual behaviors in a familiar context (Cromwell and Berridge, 1996; Aldridge and Berridge, 1998; Yin et al., 2004; Yin and Knowlton, 2006; Yin, 2010), POm projections to DLS appear to be vital for transmitting somesthetic information that is critical for executing specific sequences of well-learned behaviors. To the extent that CM/Pf activation initiates the circuit mechanisms that pause an ongoing behavior, any role played by POm in facilitating a DLS-mediated sensorimotor habit is likely to be incompatible with the functional role of the CM/Pf complex. Therefore, consistent with the physiological evidence, collicular-induced activation of Pf should be accompanied by inhibition of POm to ensure that an ongoing habitual behavior is suppressed when an unexpected stimulus requires selection of another behavior.

Unresolved Issues Concerning the Functional Role of POm

The available evidence supports the hypothesis that POm transmits sensory-related information to DLS to facilitate the expression of behavioral habits, but several critical issues remain unresolved. No studies have characterized POm neuronal activity during the performance of a behavioral task, nor has there been any attempt to characterize whether POm responses are altered as a behavior is transformed into a habit. Related to this functional role of POm, no study has determined whether POm impairment blocks the acquisition or expression of a well-learned behavioral habit.

A related issue concerns how thalamostriatal inputs from POm are coordinated with respect to neuronal responses in DLS, and to what extent POm cooperates with sensorimotor cortex in activating DLS, either in response to sensory stimulation or with respect to the acquisition of a behavioral habit. Addressing this latter issue is difficult, however, and would require chronic, simultaneous recordings from all three brain regions during prolonged behavioral training.

Another issue concerns the specific mechanisms that determine whether POm is activated by sensory inputs or is inhibited by stimulus-induced activation of the superior colliculus. Both POm and the superior colliculus receive somesthetic information directly from the trigeminal nuclei (see Figure 2), and these circuits could either activate POm or activate the collicular-incertal pathway cause inhibition of POm. The nature of the somesthetic inputs and the circuit mechanisms that determine whether POm is activated or inhibited represent an important problem for understanding the behavioral functions of POm and its thalamostriatal projections. Several findings indicate that POm is inhibited by ZIv (Trageser and Keller, 2004; Lavallée et al., 2005; Watson et al., 2015), and while some have suggested that MI projections to ZI may release POm from incertal inhibition during a behavioral activity (Urbain and Deschênes, 2007), evidence for this circuit mechanism has not been obtained.

Clinical Significance of Thalamostriatal Projections from POm

The ZI is located directly above the STN, which is a major target of DBS used to treat PD. Given that STN and ZI are adjacent to each other (see Figure 9), it is not surprising that electrode contacts intended for STN have often led to inadvertent stimulation of ZI. Many clinicians report that stimulation of caudal ZI (ZIc) reduces PD symptoms (as measured by scores on the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scales), lowers the L-dopa dosage needed to treat PD, and is effective in treating essential tremor (Plaha et al., 2006, 2008; Sun et al., 2008; Blomstedt et al., 2011, 2012; Fytagoridis et al., 2014; Lukins et al., 2014). Furthermore, the optimal DBS target for treating PD is at the interface of the ZIc and STN (Caire et al., 2013; Garcia-Garcia et al., 2016), presumably because both regions are co-stimulated at that location. Although the mechanisms of DBS are controversial, some data indicate that high-frequency stimulation disrupts the timing of aberrant oscillatory signals in STN and other sites (Vitek, 2008; Ponce and Lozano, 2010; Agnesi et al., 2013; Miocinovic et al., 2013).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Deep brain stimulation (DBS) in the STN can alter ZI activity. (A,B) Coronal sections from human and rat brains illustrate the close proximity of STN and ZI in myelin and Nissl-processed sections, respectively. (C) Schematic of a coronal section through the human brain shows the diencephalon with respect to the basal ganglia. Inset shows the region depicted in panel (D). (D) Schematic showing how multiple electrode contacts combined with current spread may cause stimulation of both STN and ZI in both the primate and rodent brain. Green lines, inhibitory connections; red lines, excitatory connections. Abbreviations: GPe, globus pallidus external; GPi, globus pallidus internal; POm, posteromedial nucleus; Pulv, Pulvinar; Put, Putamen; STN, subthalamic nucleus; Thal, thalamus; ZI, zona incerta.

The circuit connections of ZI are phylogenetically conserved in mammalian brain. In both rodents and primates, superior colliculus innervates Pf and ZIv, and the latter structure is concentrated with GABAergic neurons (Mitrofanis, 2005; Watson et al., 2014). While ZIc has been implicated with the beneficial effects of DBS, all sectors of ZI are interconnected (Power and Mitrofanis, 1999), and it is likely that ZIc stimulation would also influence GABAergic neurons located more rostrally in ZI. If DBS of human ZIc alters the activity of the GABAergic projections to the human homolog of POm, then an increase in the activity of its thalamostriatal projections could facilitate the striatal mechanisms that enable execution of normal behavioral movements.

Although evidence is limited, data from several sources suggest that the anterior pulvinar nucleus represents the primate homolog of rodent POm (Butler and Hodos, 2005). The primate pulvinar adjoins the dorsomedial side of the ventrobasal complex, just as POm does in rodents (Jones, 1985; Pons and Kaas, 1985). Both the pulvinar and POm have corticothalamic and corticocortical connections that signify higher-order thalamic nuclei (Feig and Harting, 1998; Killackey and Sherman, 2003; Alloway et al., 2008), and the divergent collateral projections from rodent POm to DLS and SI cortex resemble the collateral projections from the pulvinar to putamen and SI cortex in primates and other species in related phylogenetic lineages (Lin et al., 1984; Jones, 1985; Pons and Kaas, 1985; Smith and Parent, 1986; Day-Brown et al., 2010). The primate pulvinar remains poorly understood, however, and investigations of the collicular-incertal-POm-DLS circuit in the rat, and eventually in primates, could represent useful models for determining why ZI stimulation is beneficial for treating PD and essential tremor. Hopefully, elucidation of this mechanism would reveal new therapeutic approaches in the treatment of PD and related neurological diseases.

Author Contributions

KDA wrote the draft of the manuscript, constructed some figures and reviewed the literature described in this article, and approves this submission and takes responsibility for all information contained in this article. JBS made significant revisions to multiple drafts of this review article, constructed some figures and reviewed the literature cited in this article, and approves this submission, and takes responsibility for all information contained in this article. TMM made significant revisions to multiple drafts of this article, and contributed to the cited literature, and approves this submission, and takes responsibility for all information contained in this article. GDRW made significant revisions to multiple drafts of this article, and constructed some of the figures, and approves this submission, and takes responsibility for all information contained in this article.

Funding

Funding was provided by the Grace Woodward Foundation and the Huck Institutes of Pennsylvania University awarded to KDA.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling Editor declared a shared affiliation, though no other collaboration, with one of the authors with one of the authors GDRW and states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr. Henry Yin for his comments on a previous draft of the manuscript.

References

Abramson, B. P., and Chalupa, L. M. (1988). Multiple pathways from the superior colliculus to the extrageniculate visual thalamus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 271, 397–418. doi: 10.1002/cne.902710308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 34, 77–98. doi: 10.1080/14640748208400878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, C. D., and Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 33, 109–121. doi: 10.1080/14640748108400816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnesi, F., Johnson, M. D., and Vitek, J. L. (2013). Deep brain stimulation: how does it work? Handb. Clin. Neurol. 116, 39–54. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53497-2.00004-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahissar, E., Sosnik, R., Bagdasarian, K., and Haidarliu, S. (2001). Temporal frequency of whisker movements. II. Laminar organization of cortical representations. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 354–367.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Albe-Fessard, D., Rocha-Miranda, C., and Oswaldo-Cruz, E. (1960). Activity in the caudate nucleus of the cat in response to various types of afferent stimulation II. Microphysiology study. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. 12, 649–661. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(60)90108-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albin, R. L., Young, A. B., and Penney, J. B. (1989). The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 12, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldridge, J. W., and Berridge, K. C. (1998). Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence. J. Neurosci. 18, 2777–2787.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alexander, G. E., and DeLong, M. R. (1985). Microstimulation of the primate neostriatum. II. Somatotopic organization of striatal microexcitable zones and their relation to neuronal response properties. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 1417–1430.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Crist, J., Mutic, J. J., and Roy, S. A. (1999). Corticostriatal projections from rat barrel cortex have an anisotropic organization that correlates with vibrissal whisking movements. J. Neurosci. 19, 10908–10922.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Hoffer, Z. S., and Hoover, J. E. (2003). Quantitative comparisons of the topographic organization in the ventrobasal complex and posterior nucleus of the rodent thalamus. Brain Res. 968, 54–68. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)04265-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Alloway, K. D., Lou, L., Nwabueze-Ogbo, F., and Chakrabarti, S. (2006). Topography of cortical projections to the dorsolateral striatum in rats: multiple overlapping sensorimotor pathways. J. Comp. Neurol. 499, 33–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.21039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Mutic, J. J., Hoffer, Z. S., and Hoover, J. E. (2000). Overlapping corticostriatal projections from the rodent vibrissal representations in primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 426, 451–467. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001009)426:1<51::aid-cne4>3.0.co;2-n

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Mutic, J. J., and Hoover, J. E. (1998). Divergent corticostriatal projections from a single cortical column in the somatosensory cortex of rats. Brain Res. 785, 341–346. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01363-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Olson, M. L., and Smith, J. B. (2008). Contralateral corticothalamic projections from MI whisker cortex: potential route for modulating hemispheric interactions. J. Comp. Neurol. 510, 100–116. doi: 10.1002/cne.21782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Rosenthal, P., and Burton, H. (1989). Quantitative measurements of receptive field changes during antagonism of GABAergic transmission in primary somatosensory cortex of cats. Exp. Brain Res. 78, 514–532. doi: 10.1007/bf00230239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Smith, J. B., Beauchemin, K. J., and Olson, M. L. (2009). Bilateral projections from rat MI whisker cortex to the neostriatum, thalamus, and claustrum: forebrain circuits for modulating whisking behavior. J. Comp. Neurol. 515, 548–564. doi: 10.1002/cne.22073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, K. D., Smith, J. B., and Watson, G. D. R. (2014). Thalamostriatal projections from the medial posterior and parafascicular nuclei have distinct topographic and physiologic properties. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 36–50. doi: 10.1152/jn.00399.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arango, V., and Scalia, F. (1978). Differential projections of the nuclei of the pretectal complex of the rat. Anat. Rec. 190, 327–328.

Balleine, B. W., Liljeholm, M., and Ostlund, S. B. (2009). The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 199, 43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, T. D., Kubota, Y., Hu, D., Jin, D. Z., and Graybiel, A. M. (2005). Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature 437, 1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barthó, P., Freund, T. F., and Acsády, L. (2002). Selective GABAergic innervation of thalamic nuclei from zona incerta. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 999–1014. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02157.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, R. W., and Kleinfeld, D. (2003). Rhythmic whisking by rat: retraction as well as protraction of the vibrissae is under active muscular control. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 104–117. doi: 10.1152/jn.00600.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Whishaw, I. Q. (1992). Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: control of serial order in a grooming sequence. Exp. Brain Res. 90, 275–290. doi: 10.1007/bf00227239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomstedt, P., Fytagoridis, A., Åström, M., Linder, J., Forsgren, L., and Hariz, M. I. (2012). Unilateral caudal zona incerta deep brain stimulation for Parkinsonian tremor. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 18, 1062–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomstedt, P., Sandvik, U., Linder, J., Fredricks, A., Forsgren, L., and Hariz, M. I. (2011). Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus versus the zona incerta in the treatment of essential tremor. Acta Neurochir. 153, 2329–2335. doi: 10.1007/s00701-011-1157-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordi, F., and LeDoux, J. (1992). Sensory tuning beyond the sensory system: an initial analysis of auditory response properties of neurons in the lateral amygdaloid nucleus and overlying areas of the striatum. J. Neurosci. 12, 2493–2503.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bordi, F., LeDoux, J., Clugnet, M. C., and Pavlides, C. (1993). Single-unit activity in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala and overlying areas of the striatum in freely behaving rats: rates, discharge patterns, and responses to acoustic stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 757–769. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.5.757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. L. (1992). Somatotopic organization in rat striatum: evidence for a combinational map. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 89, 7403–7407. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.16.7403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. L., Smith, D. M., and Goldbloom, L. M. (1998). Organizing principles of cortical integration in the rat neostriatum: corticostriate map of the body surface is an ordered lattice of curved laminae and radial points. J. Comp. Neurol. 392, 468–488. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980323)392:4<468::aid-cne5>3.0.co;2-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brumberg, J. C., Pinto, D. J., and Simons, D. J. (1996). Spatial gradients and inhibitory summation in the rat whisker barrel system. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 130–140.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Butler, A. B., and Hodos, W. (2005). Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation, 2nd Edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Caire, F., Ranoux, D., Guehl, D., Burbaud, P., and Cuny, E. (2013). A systematic review of studies on anatomical position of electrode contacts used for chronic subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurochir. 155, 1647–1654. doi: 10.1007/s00701-013-1782-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carelli, R. M., and West, M. O. (1991). Representation of the body by single neurons in the dorsolateral striatum of the awake, unrestrained rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 309, 231–249. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carelli, R. M., Wolske, M., and West, M. O. (1997). Loss of lever press-related firing of rat striatal forelimb neurons after repeated sessions in a lever pressing task. J. Neurosci. 17, 1804–1814.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carvell, G. E., and Simons, D. J. (1988). Membrane potential changes in rat SmI cortical neurons evoked by controlled stimulation of mystacial vibrissae. Brain Res. 448, 186–191. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91118-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chakrabarti, S., and Alloway, K. D. (2009). Differential response patterns in the SI barrel and septal compartments during mechanical whisker stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 1632–1646. doi: 10.1152/jn.91120.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandler, H. C., King, V., Corwin, J. V., and Reep, R. L. (1992). Thalamocortical connections of rat posterior parietal cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 143, 237–242. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90273-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapin, J. K., and Lin, C. S. (1984). Mapping the body representation in the SI cortex of anesthetized and awake rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 229, 199–213. doi: 10.1002/cne.902290206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiaia, N. L., Rhoades, R. W., Bennett-Clarke, C. A., Fish, S. E., and Killackey, H. P. (1991a). Thalamic processing of vibrissal information in the rat: I. Afferent input to the medial ventral posterior and posterior nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 314, 201–216. doi: 10.1002/cne.903140202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiaia, N. L., Rhoades, R. W., Fish, S. E., and Killackey, H. P. (1991b). Thalamic processing of vibrissal information in the rat: II. Morphological and functional properties of medial ventral posterior nucleus and posterior nucleus neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 314, 217–236. doi: 10.1002/cne.903140203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chudler, E. H., Sugiyama, K., and Dong, W. K. (1995). Multisensory convergence and integration in the neostriatum and globus pallidus of the rat. Brain Res. 674, 33–45. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)01427-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coizet, V., Overton, P. G., and Redgrave, P. (2007). Collateralization of the tectonigral projection with other major output pathways of superior colliculus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 500, 1034–1049. doi: 10.1002/cne.21202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creutzfeldt, O. D., Kuhnnt, U., and Benevento, L. A. (1974). An intracellular analysis of visual cortical neurons to moving stimuli: responses in a co-operative neuronal network. Exp. Brain Res. 21, 251–274. doi: 10.1007/bf00235746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cromwell, H. C., and Berridge, K. C. (1996). Implementation of action sequences by a neostriatal site: a lesion mapping study of grooming syntax. J. Neurosci. 16, 3444–3458.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cromwell, H. C., Klein, A., and Mears, R. P. (2007). Single unit and population rersponses during inhibitory gating of striatal activity in freely moving rats. Neuroscience 146, 69–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crutcher, M. D., and DeLong, M. R. (1984). Single cell studies of the primate putamen. I. Functional organization. Exp. Brain Res. 53, 233–243. doi: 10.1007/bf00238153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day-Brown, J. D., Wei, H., Chomsung, R. D., Petry, H. M., and Bickford, M. E. (2010). Pulvinar projections to the striatum and amygdala. Front. Neuroanat. 4:143. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2010.00143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLong, M. R. (1990). Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci. 13, 281–285. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90110-v

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLong, M. R., Georgopoulos, A. P., Crutcher, M. D., Mitchell, S. J., Richardson, R. T., and Alexander, G. E. (1984). Functional organization of the basal ganglia: contributions of single-cell recording studies. Ciba Found. Symp. 107, 64–82. doi: 10.1002/9780470720882.ch5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deschênes, M., Bourassa, J., Doan, V. D., and Parent, A. (1996). A single cell study of the axonal projections arising from the posterior intralaminar thalamic nuclei in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 329–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01217.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deschênes, M., Bourassa, J., and Parent, A. (1995). Two different types of thalamic fibers innervate the rat neostriatum. Brain Res. 701, 288–292. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01124-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Detsch, O., Kochs, E., Siemers, M., Bromm, B., and Vahle-Hinz, C. (2002). Differential effects of isoflurane on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to thalamic neurons in vivo. Br. J. Anaesth. 89, 294–300. doi: 10.1093/bja/aef170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devan, B. D., Hong, N. S., and McDonald, R. J. (2011). Parallel associative processing in the dorsal striatum: segregation of stimulus-response and cognitive control subregions. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 95–120. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devan, B. D., McDonald, R. J., and White, N. M. (1999). Effects of medial and caudate-putamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze: relation to thigmotaxis. Behav. Brain Res. 100, 5–14. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00107-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devan, B. D., and White, N. M. (1999). Parallel information processing in the dorsal striatum: relation to hippocampal function. J. Neurosci. 19, 2789–2798.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Diamond, M. E. (1995). “Somatosensory thalamus of the rat,” in Cerebral Cortex, The Barrel Cortex of Rodentsv, Vol 11, eds E. G. Jones and I. T. Diamond (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 189–219.

Google Scholar

Diamond, M. E., Armstrong-James, M., and Ebner, F. F. (1992). Somatic sensory responses in the rostral sector of the posterior group (POm) and in the ventral posterior medial nucleus (VPM) of the rat thalamus. J. Comp. Neurol. 318, 462–476. doi: 10.1002/cne.903190108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioral autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 308, 67–78. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A., Nicholas, D. J., and Adams, C. D. (1983). The effect of the instrumental training contingency on susceptibility to reinforcer devaluation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 35, 35–51. doi: 10.1080/14640748308400912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, J. B., Guzman, J. N., Peterson, J. D., Goldberg, J. A., and Surmeier, D. J. (2010). Thalamic gating of corticostriatal signaling by cholinergic interneurons. Neuron 67, 294–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doig, N. M., Magill, P. J., Apicella, P., Bolam, J. P., and Sharott, A. (2014). Cortical and thalamic excitation mediate the multi-phasic responses of striatal cholinergic interneurons to motivationally salient stimuli. J. Neurosci. 34, 3101–3117. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4627-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donahue, C. H., and Kreitzer, A. C. (2015). A direct path to action initiation. Neuron 88, 240–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnelly, J. F., Thompson, S. M., and Robertson, R. T. (1983). Organization of projections from the superior colliculus to the thalamic lateral posterior nucleus in the rat. Brain Res. 288, 315–319. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90109-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dube, L., Smith, A. D., and Bolam, J. P. (1988). Identification of synaptic terminals of thalamic or cortical origin in contact with distinct medium-size spiny neurons in the rat neostriatum. J. Comp. Neurol. 267, 455–471. doi: 10.1002/cne.902670402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabri, M., and Burton, H. (1991). Topography of connections between primary somatosensory cortex and posterior complex in rat: a multiple fluorescent tracer study. Brain Res. 538, 351–357. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90455-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feig, S., and Harting, J. K. (1998). Corticocortical communication via the thalamus: ultrastructural studies of corticothalamic projections from area 17 to the lateral posterior nucleus of the cat and inferior pulvinar nucleus of the owl monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 395, 281–295. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980808)395:3<281::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W., and Graybiel, A. M. (1991). Corticostriatal transformations in the primate somatosensory system. Projections from physiologically-mapped body-part representations. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 1249–1263.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W., and Graybiel, A. M. (1993). Two input systems for body representations in the primate striatal matrix: experimental evidence in the squirrel monkey. J. Neurosci. 13, 1120–1137.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W., and Graybiel, A. M. (1994). Input-output organization of the sensorimotor striatum in the squirrel monkey. J. Neurosci. 14, 599–610.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Flaherty, A. W., and Graybiel, A. M. (1995). Motor and somatosensory corticostriatal projection magnifications in the squirrel monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 2638–2648.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Franks, N. P. (2008). General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 370–386. doi: 10.1038/nrn2372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedberg, M. H., Lee, S. M., and Ebner, F. F. (1999). Modulation of receptive field properties of thalamic somatosensory neurons by depth of anesthesia. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2243–2252.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fytagoridis, A., Sandvik, U., Åström, M., Bergenheim, T., and Blomstedt, P. (2014). Long term follow-up of deep brain stimulation of caudal zona incerta for essential tremor. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 83, 258–262. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-300765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galvan, A., and Smith, Y. (2011). The primate thalamostriatal systems: anatomical organization, functional roles and possible involvement in Parkinson’s disease. Basal Ganglia 1, 179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.baga.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, P., Bermejo, R., and Zeigler, H. P. (2001). Whisker deafferentation and rodent whisking patterns: behavioral evidence for a central pattern generator. J. Neurosci. 21, 5374–5380.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garcia-Garcia, D., Guridi, J., Toledo, J. B., Alegre, M., Obeso, J. A., and Rodríguez, M. C. (2016). Stimulation sites in the subthalamic nucleus and clinical improvement in Parkinson’s disease: a new approach for active contact localization. J. Neurosurg. 125, 1068–1079. doi: 10.3171/2015.9.jns15868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graybiel, A. M. (1998). The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70, 119–136. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graybiel, A. M. (2008). Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 359–387. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groenewegen, H. J., and Berendse, H. W. (1994). The specificity of the ‘nonspecific’ midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Trends Neurosci. 17, 52–57. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90074-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groh, A., Bokor, H., Mease, R. A., Plattner, V. M., Hangya, B., Stroh, A., et al. (2013). Convergence of cortical and sensory driver inputs on single thalamocortical cells. Cereb. Cortex 24, 3167–3179. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grunwerg, B. S., and Krauthamer, G. M. (1992). Sensory responses of intralaminar thalamic neurons activated by the superior colliculus. Exp. Brain Res. 88, 541–550. doi: 10.1007/bf00228183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hattox, A. M., and Nelson, S. B. (2007). Layer V neurons in mouse cortex projecting to different targets have distinct physiological properties. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 3330–3340. doi: 10.1152/jn.00397.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawking, T. G., and Gerdjikov, T. V. (2013). Populations of medium spiny neurons encode vibrotactile frequency in rats: modulation by slow wave oscillations. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 315–320. doi: 10.1152/jn.00489.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazlett, J. C., Dutta, C. R., and Fox, C. A. (1976). The neurons in the centromedian-parafascicular complex of the monkey (Macaca mulatta): a Golgi study. J. Comp. Neurol. 168, 41–73. doi: 10.1002/cne.901680104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, L. F., Redgrave, P., and Obeso, J. A. (2015). Habitual behavior and dopamine cell vulnerability in Parkinson disease. Front. Neuroanat. 9:99. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2015.00099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hersch, S. M., and White, E. L. (1982). A quantitative study of the thalamocortical and other synapses in layer IV of pyramidal cells projecting from mouse SmI cortex to the caudate-putamen nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 211, 217–225. doi: 10.1002/cne.902110302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hikosaka, O., Sakamoto, M., and Usui, S. (1989). Functional properties of monkey caudate neurons. II. Visual and auditory responses. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 799–813.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hintiryan, H., Foster, N. N., Bowman, I., Bay, M., Song, M. Y., Gou, L., et al. (2016). The mouse cortico-striatal projectome. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1100–1114. doi: 10.1038/nn.4332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffer, Z. S., and Alloway, K. D. (2001). Organization of corticostriatal projections from the vibrissal representations in the primary motor and somatosensory cortical areas of rodents. J. Comp. Neurol. 439, 87–103. doi: 10.1002/cne.1337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover, J. E., Hoffer, Z. S., and Alloway, K. D. (2003). Projections from primary somatosensory cortex to the neostriatum: the role of somatotopic continuity in corticostriatal convergence. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 1576–1687. doi: 10.1152/jn.01009.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innocenti, G. M., and Manzoni, T. (1972). Response patterns of somatosensory cortical neurones to peripheral stimuli: an intracellular study. Arch. Ital. Biol. 110, 322–347.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

James, W. (1887). The laws of habit. Pop. Sci. Mon. 30, 431–451.

Jin, X., and Costa, R. M. (2010). Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature 466, 457–462. doi: 10.1038/nature09263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, X., and Costa, R. M. (2015). Shaping action sequences in basal ganglia circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurbiol. 33, 188–196. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, X., Tecuapetla, F., and Costa, R. M. (2014). Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 423–430. doi: 10.1038/nn.3632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jog, M. S., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C. I., Hillegaart, V., and Graybiel, A. M. (1999). Building neural representations of habits. Science 286, 1745–1749. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joksovic, P. M., and Todorovic, S. M. (2010). Isoflurane modulates neuronal excitability of the nucleus reticularis thalami in vitro. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1199, 36–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05172.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. G. (1985). The Thalamus. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Kamishina, H., Yurcisin, G. H., Corwin, J. V., and Reep, R. L. (2008). Striatal projections from the rat lateral posterior thalamic nucleus. Brain Res. 1204, 24–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, R., Markman, T., Poddar, R., Ko, R., Fantana, A. L., Dhawale, A. K., et al. (2015). Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron 86, 800–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khatri, V., Hartings, J. A., and Simons, D. J. (2004). Adaptation in thalamic barreloid and cortical barrel neurons to periodic whisker deflections varying in frequency and velocity. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 3244–3254. doi: 10.1152/jn.00257.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killackey, H. P., and Sherman, S. M. (2003). Corticothalamic projections from the rat primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 23, 7381–7384.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kincaid, A. E., Zheng, T., and Wilson, C. J. (1998). Connectivity and convergence of single corticostriatal axons. J. Neurosci. 18, 4722–4731.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kita, T., Shigematsu, N., and Kita, H. (2016). Intralaminar and tectal projections to the subthalamus in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 44, 2899–2908. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krauthamer, G. M. (1979). “Sensory functions of the neostriatum,” in The Neostriatum, ed. I. Divac (Oxford: Pergamon Press), 263–289.

Google Scholar

Kravitz, A. V., Freeze, B. S., Parker, P. R., Kay, K., Thwin, M. T., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2010). Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature 466, 622–626. doi: 10.1038/nature09159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Krout, K. E., Loewy, A. D., Westby, G. W. M., and Redgrave, P. (2001). Superior colliculus projections to midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 431, 198–216. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20010305)431:2<198::aid-cne1065>3.0.co;2-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacey, C. J., Bolam, J. P., and McGill, P. J. (2007). Novel and distinct operational principles of intralaminar thalamic neurons and their striatal projections. J. Neurosci. 27, 4374–4384. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5519-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Allan, D. M., Bennett-Clarke, C. A., Howell, D. L., and Rhoades, R. W. (1997). Projection status of calbindin- and parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the superficial layers of the rat’s superior colliculus. Vis. Neurosci. 14, 277–286. doi: 10.1017/s095252380001141x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Bennett-Clarke, C. A., Allan, D. M., and Mooney, R. D. (1993). Immunochemical heterogeneity in the tecto-LP pathway of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 333, 210–222. doi: 10.1002/cne.903330207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapper, S. R., and Bolam, J. P. (1992). Input from the frontal cortex and the parafascicular nucleus to cholinergic interneurons in the dorsal striatum of the rat. Neuroscience 51, 533–545. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90293-b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laskin, S. E., and Spencer, W. A. (1979). Cutaneous masking. II. Geometry of excitatory and inhibitory receptive fields of single units in somatosensory cortex of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 42, 1061–1082.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lavallée, P., Urbain, N., Dufresne, C., Bokor, H., Acsády, L., and Deschênes, M. (2005). Feedforward inhibitory control of sensory information in higher-order thalamic nuclei. J. Neurosci. 25, 7489–7498. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2301-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E., Sakaguchi, A., Iwata, J., and Reis, D. J. (1986). Interruption of projections from the medial geniculate body to an archi-neostriatal field disrupts the classical conditioning of emotional responses to acoustic stimuli. Neuroscience 17, 615–627. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(86)90034-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E., Sakaguchi, A., and Reis, D. J. (1983). Subcortical efferent projections of the medial geniculate nucleus mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli. J. Neurosci. 4, 683–698.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lidsky, T. I., Manetto, C., and Schneider, J. S. (1985). A consideration of sensory factors involved in motor functions of the basal ganglia. Brain Res. Rev. 9, 133–146. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(85)90010-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C.-S., May, P. J., and Hall, W. C. (1984). Nonintralaminar thalamostriatal projections in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) and tree shrew (Tupaia glis). J. Comp. Neurol. 230, 33–46. doi: 10.1002/cne.902300104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, S. M., and Lin, R. C. (1993). Thalamic afferents of the rat barrel cortex: a light and electron-microscopic study using Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin as an anterograde tracer. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 10, 1–16. doi: 10.3109/08990229309028819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukins, T. R., Tisch, S., and Jonker, B. (2014). The latest evidence on target selection in deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. J. Clin. Neurosci. 21, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2013.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masri, R., Bezdudnaya, T., Tragesaer, J. C., and Keller, A. (2008). Encoding of stimulus frequency and sensor motion in the posterior medial thalamic nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 681–689. doi: 10.1152/jn.01322.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, N., Minamimoto, T., Graybiel, A. M., and Kimura, M. (2001). Neurons in the thalamic CM-Pf complex supply striatal neurons with information about behaviorally significant sensory events. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 960–976.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maurice, N., Liberge, M., Jaouen, F., Ztaou, S., Hanini, M., Camon, J., et al. (2015). Striatal cholinergic interneurons control motor behavior and basal ganglia function in experimental Parkinsonism. Cell Rep. 13, 657–666. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. J. (2006). The mammalian superior colliculus: laminar structure and connections. Prog. Brain Res. 151, 321–378. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(05)51011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGeorge, A. J., and Faull, R. L. (1989). The organization of the projection from the cerebral cortex to the striatum in the rat. Neuroscience 29, 503–537. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90128-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHaffie, J. G., Stanford, T. R., Stein, B. E., Coizet, V., and Redgrave, P. (2005). Subcortical loops through the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 28, 401–407. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melzer, P., Sachdev, R. N. S., Jenkinson, N., and Ebner, F. F. (2006). Stimulus frequency processing in awake rat barrel cortex. J. Neurosci. 26, 12198–12205. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2620-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, G. E., and Wouterlood, F. G. (1990). Hippocampal and midline thalamic fibers and terminals in relation to the choline acetyltransferase-immunoreactive neurons in nucleus accumbens of the rat: a light and electron microscopic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 296, 204–221. doi: 10.1002/cne.902960203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minamimoto, T., and Kimura, M. (2002). Participation of the thalamic CM-Pf complex in attentional orienting. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 3090–3101.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miocinovic, S., Somayajula, S., Chitnis, S., and Vitek, J. L. (2013). History, applications, and mechanisms of deep brain stimulation. JAMA Neurol. 70, 163–171. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamaneurol.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchinson, B., Marin, C. J., Grant, R. A., and Prescott, T. J. (2007). Feedback control in active sensing: rat exploratory whisking is modulated by environmental contact. Proc. Biol. Sci. 274, 1035–1041. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitrofanis, J. (2005). Some certainty for the “zone of uncertainty”? Exploring the function of the zona incerta. Neuroscience 130, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, C. L., and Nelson, S. B. (1998). Spatio-temporal subthreshold receptive fields in the vibrissa representation of rat primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2882–2892.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mowery, T. M., Harrold, J. B., and Alloway, K. D. (2011). Repeated whisker stimulation evokes invariant neuronal responses in the dorsolateral striatum of anesthetized rats: a potential correlate of sensorimotor habits. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2225–2238. doi: 10.1152/jn.01018.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, A., Eördegh, G., Paróczy, Z., Márkus, Z., and Benedel, G. (2006). Multisensory integration in the basal ganglia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 917–924. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04942.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nothias, F., Peschanski, M., and Besson, J. M. (1988). Somatotopic reciprocal connections between the somatosensory cortex and the thalamic Po nucleus in the rat. Brain Res. 447, 169–174. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90980-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohno, S., Kuramoto, E., Furuta, T., Hioka, H., Tanaka, Y. R., Fujiyama, F., et al. (2012). A morphological analysis of thalamocortical axon fibers of rat posterior thalamic nuclei: a single neuron tracing study with viral vectors. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2840–2857. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Packard, M. G., and McGaugh, J. L. (1992). Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systems. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 439–446. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.3.439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pidoux, M., Mahon, S., Deniau, J. M., and Charpier, S. (2011). Integration and propagation of somatosensory responses in the corticostriatal pathway: an intracellular study in vivo. J. Physiol. 589, 263–281. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.199646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaha, P., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Patel, N. K., and Gill, S. S. (2006). Stimulation of the caudal zona incerta is superior to stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in improving contralateral parkinsonism. Brain 129, 1732–1747. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaha, P., Khan, S., and Gill, S. S. (2008). Bilateral stimulation of the caudal zona incerta nucleus for tremor control. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 504–513. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.112334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponce, F. A., and Lozano, A. M. (2010). Deep brain stimulation: state of the art and novel stimulation targets. Prog. Brain Res. 184, 311–324. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(10)84016-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pons, T. P., and Kaas, J. H. (1985). Connections of area 2 of somatosensory cortex with the anterior pulvinar and subdivisions of the ventroposterior complex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 240, 16–36. doi: 10.1002/cne.902400103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, B. D., and Mitrofanis, J. (1999). Evidence for extensive inter-connections within the zona incerta in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 267, 9–12. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00313-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raju, D. V., Ahern, T. H., Shah, D. J., Wright, T. M., Standaert, D. G., Hall, R. A., et al. (2008). Differential synaptic plasticity of the corticostriatal and thalamostriatal systems in an MPTP-treated monkey model of parkinsonism. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1647–1658. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06136.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raju, D. V., Shah, D. J., Wright, T. M., Hall, R. A., and Smith, Y. (2006). Differential synaptology of vGluT2-containing thalamostriatal afferents between the patch and matrix compartments in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 499, 231–243. doi: 10.1002/cne.21099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramanathan, S., Hanley, J. J., Deniau, J. M., and Bolam, J. P. (2002). Synaptic convergence of motor and somatosensory cortical afferents onto GABAergic interneurons in the rat striatum. J. Neurosci. 22, 8158–8169.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Redgrave, P., Rodriguez, M., Smith, Y., Rodriguez-Oroz, M., Lehericy, S., Bergman, H., et al. (2010). Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 760–772. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reep, R. L., and Corwin, J. V. (1999). Topographic organization of the striatal and thalamic connections of rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Res. 841, 43–52. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01779-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reep, R. L., and Corwin, J. V. (2009). Posterior parietal cortex as part of a neural network for directed attention in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 104–113. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reig, R., and Silberberg, G. (2014). Multisensory integration in the mouse striatum. Neuron 83, 1200–1212. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reig, R., and Silberberg, G. (2016). Distinct corticostriatal and intracortical pathways mediate bilateral sensory responses in the striatum. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4405–4415. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiner, A., Jiao, Y., Del Mar, N., Laverghetta, A. V., and Lei, W. L. (2003). Differential morphology of pyramidal tract-type and intratelencephalically projecting-type corticostriatal neurons and their intrastriatal terminals in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 457, 420–440. doi: 10.1002/cne.10541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, R. T. (1983). Efferents of the pretectal complex: separate populations of neurons project to lateral thalamus and to inferior olive. Brain Res. 258, 91–95. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)91229-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roger, M., and Cadusseau, J. (1984). Afferent connections of the nucleus posterior thalami in the rat, with some evolutionary and functional considerations. J. Hirnforsch. 25, 473–485.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Roger, M., and Cadusseau, J. (1985). Afferents to the zona incerta in the rat: a combined retrograde and anterograde study. J. Comp. Neurol. 241, 480–492. doi: 10.1002/cne.902410407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, M. M., Dahmen, J. C., Muir, D. R., Imhof, F., Martini, F. J., and Hofer, S. B. (2016). Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 299–307. doi: 10.1038/nn.4197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryugo, D. K., and Killackey, H. P. (1974). Differential telencephalic projections of the medial and ventral divisions of the medial geniculate body of the rat. Brain Res. 82, 173–177. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90903-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Sachdev, R. N., Berg, R. W., Champney, G., Kleinfeld, D., and Ebner, F. F. (2003). Unilateral vibrissa contact: changes in amplitude but not timing of rhythmic whisking. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 20, 163–169. doi: 10.1080/08990220311000405208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J. S., and Lidsky, T. I. (1981). Processing of somatosensory information in striatum of behaving cats. J. Neurophysiol. 45, 841–851.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schulz, J. M., Redgrave, P., Mehring, C., Aertsen, A., Clements, K. M., Wickens, J. R., et al. (2009). Short-latency activation of striatal spiny neurons via subcortical visual pathways. J. Neurosci. 29, 6336–6347. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4815-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, J. M., and Reynolds, J. N. (2013). Pause and rebound: sensory control of cholinergic signaling in the striatum. Trends Neurosci. 36, 41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedgwick, E. M., and Williams, T. D. (1967). The response of single units in the caudate nucleus to peripheral stimulation. J. Physiol. 189, 281–298. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seger, C. A., and Spiering, B. J. (2011). A critical review of habit learning and the basal ganglia. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:66. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W., Flajolet, M., Greengard, P., and Surmeier, D. J. (2008). Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science 321, 848–851. doi: 10.1126/science.1160575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (1998). On the actions that one nerve cell can have on another: distinguishing “drivers” from “modulators”. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 7121–7126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., and Carvell, G. E. (1989). Thalamocortical response transformation in the rat vibrissa/barrel system. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 311–330.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sippy, T., Lapray, D., Crochet, S., and Petersen, C. C. H. (2015). Cell-type-specific sensorimotor processing in striatal projection neurons during goal-directed behavior. Neuron 88, 298–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, Y., Galvan, A., Ellender, T. J., Doig, N., Villalba, R. M., Huerta-Ocampo, I., et al. (2014). The thalamostriatal system in normal and diseased states. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:5. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. S., and Graybiel, A. M. (2014). Investigating habits: strategies, technologies and models. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:39. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. B., Mowery, T. M., and Alloway, K. D. (2012). Thalamic POm projections to the dorsolateral striatum of rats: potential pathway for mediating stimulus-response associations for sensorimotor habits. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 160–174. doi: 10.1152/jn.00142.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, Y., and Parent, A. (1986). Differential connections of caudate nucleus and putamen in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri Sciureus). Neuroscience 18, 347–371.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sosnik, R., Haidarliu, S., and Ahissar, E. (2001). Temporal frequency of whisker movement. I. Representations in brain stem and thalamus. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 339–353.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sparks, D. L. (1986). Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. Physiol. Rev. 66, 118–171.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Strecker, R. E., Steinfels, G. F., Abercrombie, E. D., and Jacobs, B. L. (1985). Caudate unit activity in freely moving cats: effects of phasic auditory and visual stimuli. Brain Res. 329, 350–353. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90548-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugita, S., Otani, K., Tokunaga, A., and Terasawa, K. (1983). Laminar origin of the tecto-thalamic projections in the albino rat. Neurosci. Lett. 43, 143–147. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(83)90178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, D. A., Yu, H., Spooner, J., Tatsas, A. D., Davis, T., Abel, T. W., et al. (2008). Postmortem analysis following 71 months of deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus for Parkinson disease. J. Neurosurg. 109, 325–329. doi: 10.3171/JNS/2008/109/8/0325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surmeier, D. J., Ding, J., Day, M., Wang, Z., and Shen, W. (2007). D1 and D2 dopamine-receptor modulation of striatal glutamatergic signaling in striatal medium spiny neurons. Trends Neurosci. 30, 228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swadlow, H. A. (2003). Fast-spike interneurons and feedforward inhibition in awake sensory neocortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 25–32. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.1.25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syed, E. C. J., Sharott, A., Moll, C. K. E., Engel, A. K., and Kraj, A. (2011). Effect of sensory stimulation in rat barrel cortex, dorsolateral striatum and on corticostriatal functional connectivity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 461–470. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07549.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S. M., and Robertson, R. T. (1987). Organization of subcortical pathways for sensory projections to the limbic cortex. II. Afferent projections to the thalamic lateral dorsal nucleus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 265, 189–202. doi: 10.1002/cne.902650204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorn, C. A., and Graybiel, A. M. (2010). Pausing to regroup: thalamic gating of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Neuron 67, 175–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trageser, J. C., Burke, K. A., Masri, R., Li, Y., Sellers, L., and Keller, A. (2006). State-dependent gating of sensory inputs by zona incerta. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 1456–1463. doi: 10.1152/jn.00423.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trageser, J. C., and Keller, A. (2004). Reducing the uncertainty: gating of peripheral inputs by zona incerta. J. Neurosci. 24, 8911–8915. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3218-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urbain, N., and Deschênes, M. (2007). Motor cortex gates vibrissal responses in a thalamocortical projection pathway. Neuron 56, 714–725. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veinante, P., Jacquin, M. F., and Deschênes, M. (2000). Thalamic projections from the whisker-sensitive regions of the spinal trigeminal complex in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 420, 233–243. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000501)420:2<233::aid-cne6>3.0.co;2-t

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viaene, A. N., Petrof, I., and Sherman, S. M. (2011). Properties of the thalamic projection from the posterior medial nucleus to primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in the mouse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 18156–18161. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114828108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitek, J. L. (2008). Deep brain stimulation: how does it work? Cleve. Clin. J. Med. 75, S59–S65. doi: 10.3949/ccjm.75.Suppl_2.S59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, C., Lind, C. R. P., and Thomas, M. G. (2014). The anatomy of the caudal zona incerta in rodents and primates. J. Anat. 224, 95–107. doi: 10.1111/joa.12132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, G. D. R., Smith, J. B., and Alloway, K. D. (2015). The zona incerta regulates communication between the superior colliculus and the posteromedial thalamus: implications for thalamic interactions with the dorsolateral striatum. J. Neurosci. 35, 9463–9476. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1606-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welker, W. I. (1964). Analysis of sniffing in the albino rat. Behaviour 12, 223–244. doi: 10.1163/156853964x00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, M. O. (1998). Anesthetics eliminate somatosensory-evoked discharges of neurons in the somatotopically organized sensorimotor striatum of the rat. J. Neurosci. 18, 9055–9068.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Williams, M. N., Zahm, D. S., and Jacquin, M. F. (1994). Differential foci and synaptic organization of the principal and spinal trigeminal projections to the thalamus in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 6, 429–453. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00286.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, C. J. (1987). Morphology and synaptic connections of crossed corticostriatal neurons in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 263, 567–580. doi: 10.1002/cne.902630408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, C. J. (1995). “The contribution of cortical neurons to the firing pattern of striatal spiny neurons,” in Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia, eds J. C. Houk, J. L. Davis and D. G. Beiser (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 29–50.

Wilson, C. J. (2014). The sensory striatum. Neuron 83, 999–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wimmer, V. C., Bruno, R. M., de Kock, C. P., Kuner, T., and Sakmann, B. (2010). Dimensions of a projection column and architecture of VPM and POm axons in rat vibrissal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2265–2276. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. K., Norrie, L., Ingham, C. A., Hutton, E. A. M., and Arbuthnott, G. W. (1999). Double anterograde tracing of outputs from adjacent “barrel columns” of rat somatosensory cortex. Neostriatal projection patterns and terminal ultrastructure. Neuroscience 88, 119–133. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00186-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. K., Ramanathan, S., and Arbuthnott, G. W. (2001). Identification of the source of the bilateral projection system from cortex to somatosensory neostriatum and an exploration of its physiological actions. Neuroscience 103, 87–96. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00564-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, J. H., Corwin, J. V., and Reep, R. L. (2009). Organization of the corticostriatal projection from rat medial agranular cortex to far dorsolateral striatum. Brain Res. 1280, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, Q., Znamenskiy, P., and Zador, A. M. (2015). Selective corticostriatal plasticity during acquisition of an auditory discrimination task. Nature 521, 348–351. doi: 10.1038/nature14225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, D. S., and Krauthamer, G. M. (1990). Somatosensory neurons projecting from the superior colliculus to the intralaminar thalamus in the rat. Brain Res. 523, 188–194. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91486-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, D. S., Krauthamer, G. M., and Rhoades, R. W. (1986). Superior collicular projection to intralaminar thalamus in rat. Brain Res. 378, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90925-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H. (2010). The sensorimotor striatum is necessary for serial order learning. J. Neurosci. 30, 14719–14723. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3989-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., and Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J., and Balleine, B. W. (2004). Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 181–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., Ostlund, S. B., Knowlton, B. J., and Balleine, B. W. (2005). The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ying, S. W., Werner, D. F., Homanics, G. E., Harrision, N. L., and Goldstein, P. A. (2009). Isoflurane modulates excitability in the mouse striatum via GABA-dependent and GABA-independent mechanisms. Neuropharmacology 56, 438–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Znamenskiy, P., and Zador, A. M. (2013). Corticostriatal neurons in auditory cortex drive decisions during auditory discrimination. Nature 497, 482–485. doi: 10.1038/nature12077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: corticostriatal, intralaminar complex, Parkinson disease, POm nucleus, sensorimotor, superior colliculus, thalamus, zona incerta

Citation: Alloway KD, Smith JB, Mowery TM and Watson GDR (2017) Sensory Processing in the Dorsolateral Striatum: The Contribution of Thalamostriatal Pathways. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 11:53. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00053

Received: 14 June 2017; Accepted: 07 July 2017;
Published: 25 July 2017.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, United States

Reviewed by:

John Mitrofanis, University of Sydney, Australia
Martin Deschenes, Laval University, Canada

Copyright © 2017 Alloway, Smith, Mowery and Watson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kevin D. Alloway, kda1@psu.edu

Download