Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy and child’s cognitive performance at 6–8 years of age in rural Burkina Faso: an observational study

View article
PeerJ

Introduction

The World Health Organization (WHO) recently stated that harmful consumption of alcohol is among the top five risk factors for disease, disability and death throughout the world. It is a causal factor in several diseases and injury conditions, and intake is on the increase, especially in low income countries (Rehm et al., 2009; WHO, 2014a).

Children exposed to prenatal alcohol have cognitive, physical and behavioural deficiencies (Popova et al., 2016b). Many studies have shown that regular and heavy consumption of alcohol during pregnancy are associated with neuropsychological and cognitive impairments in memory, executive function, processing speed, visual and spatial abilities, attention, language and academic achievement (Kodituwakku, Kalberg & May, 2001; O’Callaghan et al., 2007; Falgreen Eriksen et al., 2012; Flak et al., 2014). Recent reviews highlighted how prenatal alcohol can be sensitive on spatial abilities, reasoning (Mattson, Crocker & Nguyen, 2011), and memory (Du Plooy et al., 2016).

However, most of the evidence comes from high-income countries (Lewis et al., 2015; Lewis et al., 2016; Fan et al., 2016), and data are scarce in an African context where lack of resources, rural areas and home brewing alcohol consumption are common (Martinez et al., 2011). Burkina Faso is a country in Africa where the use of alcohol is increasing among women; it has among the highest national proportion of women consuming alcohol in the continent, 30% (Martinez et al., 2011). In 2016, a systematic review highlighted that the predicted prevalence of any amount of alcohol consumption during pregnancy among the general population in Burkina Faso was 11.3% (Popova et al., 2016a). According to the WHO, the level of total alcohol consumption was 6.8 litres of pure alcohol per capita for adults above 15 years of age from 2008 to 2010 (WHO, 2014b). The home brewed alcohol represented 84% of the type of alcohol consumed, followed by beer (10%), spirit (3%) and wine (3%) (WHO, 2014b).

Given the known harm from prenatal alcohol consumption and the evolving evidence of increasing drinking patterns among women in Africa, there is a need to explore alcohol consumption among pregnant women and its effect on the neuro-cognitive outcomes in their offspring in a context where lack of resources, rural areas and home brewing alcohol consumption are common. We aimed to study the association between maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy and the offspring’s cognitive performance using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd edition (KABC-II) and the Children’s Category Test Level 1 (CCT-1) in rural Burkina Faso.

Subjects and Methods

Study area, setting, study design and participants

Burkina Faso is a low income country located in the middle of West Africa; the population resides mainly in rural areas (70.1% in 2015), and the population aged 0–14 years was 46.3% in 2013 (INSD, 2016; UN Statistics, 2016). The literacy rate is very low and the mean years of education attained in women and girls was less than 3 years in 2013 (Patton et al., 2016). The official language in Burkina Faso is French. However, the country has more than 60 different ethnic groups. Several local languages are spoken in the study area Gouin, Karaboro, Dioula, Senoufo, Turka, Moore, and Fulfulde (Hama Diallo et al., 2012; Rossier et al., 2013; Ethnologue, 2016), which is a challenge when performing cognitive testing.

In 2006, a community-based cluster-randomised trial of children was conducted, the PROMISE Exclusive Breastfeeding (EBF) study. One of the sites was in rural Burkina Faso (Diallo et al., 2010; Diallo et al., 2011; Tylleskär et al., 2011; Hama Diallo et al., 2012). The sampling has been described (Diallo et al., 2010; Tylleskär et al., 2011). From 2013 to 2015, a cross-sectional follow-up study was conducted through the PROMISE Saving Brains study to assess the neuro-cognitive performance of the children aged 6–8 years old. We sought to re-enrol all children from the initial PROMISE EBF trial who were found to be alive and still residing in the study area.

Outcome measures

The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd edition (KABC™-II) is an individually administered cognitive test with verbal and nonverbal components which has been used across diverse cultural contexts (Boivin et al., 1996; Ochieng, 2003; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004; Malda et al., 2010). In Africa, it has been used to study cognitive development and nutrition in Ethiopia (Bogale et al., 2013), Democratic Republic of Congo (Boivin et al., 2013; Bumoko et al., 2015) and South Africa (Taljaard et al., 2013; Rochat et al., 2016), among HIV infected children in Uganda (Boivin et al., 2010; Ruel et al., 2012; Brahmbhatt et al., 2017), and cerebral malaria in Senegal (Boivin, 2002), and Uganda (Bangirana et al., 2009). KABC-II has different sub-tests and is used in children aged 3–18 years. The sub-tests (Appendix A) used in our study were:

  • Atlantis: a measure of memory

  • Conceptual Thinking: a measure of visual and spatial abilities

  • Face recognition: a measure of visual and spatial abilities

  • Story Completion: a measure of pattern recognition and reasoning

  • Number Recall: a measure of memory

  • Rover: a measure of spatial scanning and reasoning

  • Triangle: a measure of spatial abilities and visualization

  • Block Counting: a measure of reasoning

  • Word Order: a measure of memory

  • Pattern reasoning: a measure of reasoning and visualization (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004; Bangirana et al., 2009).

The Children’s Category Test Level 1 (CCT-1) is a widely used non-verbal test developed to evaluate problem solving in children; it is fast and easy to administer (Boll, 1993; Hundal & Morris, 2011; Goudis, 2014). It was used to examine the effect of different exposures including health conditions like traumatic injuries (Moore, Donders & Thompson, 2004; Donders & Nesbit-Greene, 2004; Horneman & Emanuelson, 2009), brain dysfunction (Allen, Knatz & Mayfield, 2006; Bello, Allen & Mayfield, 2008), diseases (Rosenberg et al., 2010), marihuana and cocaine (Fried, Watkinson & Gray, 2005; Ga et al., 2015), disabilities (Hinton et al., 2004), chemical products (Debes et al., 2006; Wright et al., 2006; Jurewicz, Polańska & Hanke, 2013), and alcohol (Mattson et al., 1998). CCT-1 is an individually administered standardized test for children from 5 to 8 years to test their ability to solve problems on the basis of corrective feedback. It is presented in booklet form and consists of five subtests. At the end of the test, the total number of errors is counted. Children with more errors are the one who performed worst (Boll, 1993; Moore, Donders & Thompson, 2004; Allen, Knatz & Mayfield, 2006).

The KABC-II and the CCT-1 were administered by a team of four trained psychologists who spoke the local languages. The children were randomly assigned to the psychologists for assessment. The assessors administered individually the KABC-II and the CCT-1 during a one-to-one session. The instructions of the measures were translated in the main local language (Dioula) commonly spoken in the study area. Independent back translations were done.

Exposure measure

Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy was the main exposure for this analysis. Information about maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy was collected in a household interview with the caretaker prior to the neuro-cognitive assessment. Data collectors approached each child’s household to administer a questionnaire to the child’s caregiver during a one-to-one interview. Mothers were the primary respondents. A yes/no question of any alcohol consumption during pregnancy was asked. Of all the 554 caretakers, 518 were able to provide information on this question and 36 (6.5%) were not.

Covariates

In the interview, questions were asked about background characteristics and socio-economic status that may influence the child’s performance. These include the child’s age, child’s schooling, father’s employment, father’s education (dichotomized to educated = at least one year in school, or not educated), mother’s age, mothers’ employment, mother’s education, current maternal alcohol status (a yes/no question of any current alcohol consumption), mother’s depression status using the Hopkins symptom checklist (Sirpal et al., 2016) (dichotomized to depression = at least one symptom in the checklist, no depression = no symptom in any of the checklist), mother’s chewing tobacco status (a yes/no question of current tobacco chewing), and presence of latrine in the compound (a yes/no question). Questions regarding past hospitalizations since birth of the child were asked and anthropometric data (height, age) were measured according to standard procedures (CDC, 2007) by a paediatrician at the study site. Stunting was defined as below-2 standard deviations of height-for-age.

Before the starting of data collection, field-testing and piloting of all the instruments was conducted to calibrate and standardize the assessment of cognitive measures and the data collection. For instance, the stadiometer for height was calibrated according to the instruction of manual, and the psychologists underwent field training and refresher training to standardize the way to administer the KABC-II and CCT-1 on local children prior to the study participants.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted in several stages:

  1. To examine within population variance of the sub-tests, the distribution of scores (mean, standard deviation, median, minimum and maximum) were used. Box-and-whisker plots per exposed and unexposed groups were used to illustrate the children’s performances on different sub-tests of KABC-II and CCT-1. Extreme scores were winsorized to discount the influence of outliers by replacing their values with the nearest scores within this range.

  2. To examine the reliability of items of the sub-tests, split-half reliability coefficients were calculated for KABC-II (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004; Malda et al., 2010) and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was calculated for CCT-1 (Boll, 1993; Moore, Donders & Thompson, 2004; Allen, Knatz & Mayfield, 2006). The level of significance of the reliability coefficient was ≥0.7.

  3. To examine the association between maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy and cognitive performance of KABC-II and CCT-1, effect size differences using the Cohen’s d (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012; Cumming, 2014), and linear regression analysis were conducted. No validated norms of the KABC-II and the CCT-1 were available in Burkina Faso at the time of the study; we then used the raw scores instead of the scaled scores. However, all scores were standardized (Z) and analysis were conducted on both unstandardized and standardized scores. All the coefficients were adjusted for potential confounders including child’s age, sex, schooling, stunting, father’s employment, father’s education (Martinez et al., 2011; Falgreen Eriksen et al., 2012; Flak et al., 2014; Kesmodel et al., 2015) and the promotion of exclusive breastfeeding (‘intervention arm’ of the initial trial). A bivariate analysis between each covariate and the outcome was conducted (Table A1). STATA 13 was used to perform the analysis.

Ethical considerations

Written informed consent was obtained from all care-takers in the study and oral assent was obtained from the children. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Centre MURAZ number 008-2013/CE-CM.

Study profile of children who completed the KABC-II and having information on maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy at the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.

Figure 1: Study profile of children who completed the KABC-II and having information on maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy at the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.

Results

Study population

Of the initial 794 enrolled children in the PROMISE EBF trial in Burkina Faso site, 561 were alive, traced and re-consented for the follow-up study, 554 children completed the KABC-II and the CCT-1, and 518 children had information on their maternal alcohol consumption status (Fig. 1).

Of these, 51.7% (268/518) were boys, and 49.4% (256/518) were at school. The mean (± standard deviation, SD) age at assessment was 7.2 (±0.4 years), the median (interquartile range, IQR) was 7.2 (6.9–7.4) years and the range was 6.3–8 years. Of the mothers, 18.5% (96/518) reported to have consumed alcohol during the pregnancy and none of them had been more than 1 year in school. The mean (±SD) age of the mothers at assessment was 33.4 (±6.3 years). Of the fathers, 30.6% (156/510) had attended at least 1 year in school and 12.9% (67/518) had an employment. Three quarters of the compounds reported having a pit latrine 73.4%, (380/518) (Table 1).

Table 1:
Description of the children who completed the KABC-II and CCT-1 from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.
Total N = 518N (% ) Maternal alcohol N = 96(18.5)N (% ) No maternal alcohol N = 422(81.5)N (%) p-value
Child age Mean ± SD (in years) 7.2 ± 0.4 7.2 ± 0.3 7.2 ± 0.4 0.38
Mothers age Mean ± SD (in years) 33.4 ± 6.3 34.4 ± 6.6 33.2 ± 6.2 0.17
Sex 0.7
Girls 250 (48.6) 45 (46.9) 205 (48.6)
Boys 268 (51.4) 51 (53.1) 217 (51.4)
Child in school 0.7
Yes 256 (49.4) 46 (47.9) 210 (49.8)
No 262 (50.6) 50 (52.1) 212 (50.2)
Stunting (<-2 SD in height-for-age) 0.8
No 426 (84.2) 79 (85.0) 347 (84.0)
Yes 80 (15.8) 14 (15.0) 66 (16.0)
Child has been hospitalized 0.6
No 391 (77.9) 71 (76.3) 320 (78.2)
Yes 111 (22.1) 22 (23.7) 89 (21.8)
Father employed 0.1
Yes 67 (12.9) 8 (8.3) 59 (14.0)
No 451 (87.1) 88 (91.7) 363 (86.0)
Father educated 0.8
Yes 156 (30.6) 28 (29.8) 128 (30.8)
No 354 (69.4) 66 (70.2) 288 (69.2)
Mother employed 0.1
Yes 26 (5.0) 2 (2.1) 24 (5.7)
No 492 (95.0) 94 (97.9) 398 (94.3)
Mother’s current alcohol consumption 0.0001
No 89 (17.2) 25 (26.3) 19 (4.5)
Yes 428 (82.8) 70 (73.7) 403 (95.5)
Mothers depression status 0.2
No 267 (51.5) 55 (57.29) 212 (50.2)
Yes 251 (48.9) 41 (42.71) 210 (49.8)
Mothers chewing tobacco 0.0001
No 495 (95.6) 85 (88.5) 410 (97.2)
Yes 23 (4.4) 11 (11.5) 12 (2.8)
Latrine in compound 0.003
Yes 380 (73.4) 59 (61.5) 321 (76.1)
No 138 (26.6) 37 (38.5) 101 (23.9)
PROMISE EBF intervention 0.07
Control arm 274 (52.9) 43 (44.8) 231 (54.7)
Intervention arm 244 (47.1) 53 (55.2) 191 (45.3)
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3507/table-1

Notes:

SD, Standard deviation

Box-and-whisker plots with median, interquartile range (box), minimum and maximum (whiskers) of child’s performance at KABC-II subtests and CCT-1 test by maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.

Figure 2: Box-and-whisker plots with median, interquartile range (box), minimum and maximum (whiskers) of child’s performance at KABC-II subtests and CCT-1 test by maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.

(A) Atlantis raw score; (B) Conceptual thinking raw score; (C) Face recognition raw score; (D) Story completion raw score; (E) Number recall raw score; (F) Rover raw score; (G) Triangle raw score; (H) Block counting raw score; (I) Word order raw score; (J) Pattern reasoning raw score; (K) CCT-1 total errors.
Table 2:
Tests description and internal consistency of 518 children who completed the KABC-II and CCT-1 from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.
Tests Mean ± SD Median (IQR) Min score Max score Reliability coefficient
Atlantis 43.5 ± 19.4 43 (28–59) 12 90 0.96
Conceptual Thinking 5.1 ± 3.4 4 (2–8) 0 16 0.80
Face recognition 5.0 ± 3.0 4 (3–8) 0 13 0.74
Story completion 3.1 ± 1.3 3 (2–4) 0 6 0.44
Number recall 5.9 ± 1.8 6 (5–7) 2 9 0.76
Rover 6.0 ± 1.9 6 (5–7) 2 10 0.45
Triangle 6.7 ± 2.8 6 (5–8) 1 14 0.78
Block counting 4.1 ± 3.6 3 (1–7) 0 15 0.73
Word order 10.4 ± 1.8 10 (9–12) 5 16 0.64
Pattern reasoning 1.5 ± 1.0 1 (1–2) 0 3 0.56
CCT-1 errors 35.6 ± 7.2 35 (31–40) 17 55 0.82
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3507/table-2

Notes:

SD

Standard deviation

IQR

Inter Quartile Range

On the KABC-II, sufficient variability (mean ± SD) of the raw scores was found for all the sub-tests except ‘Pattern Reasoning’ (Fig. 2 and Table 2). No child scored 0 in ‘Atlantis’, ‘Number recall’, ‘Rover’, ‘Triangle’ and ‘Word order’ (Fig. 2 and Table 2). The Split-half reliability coefficient was acceptable (>0.70) for all the sub-tests except ‘Story completion’, ‘Rover’, ‘Word order’ and ‘Pattern Reasoning’ (Table 2).

Maternal alcohol consumption and cognitive performance

Children whose mothers reported alcohol consumption during pregnancy performed significantly poorly for memory and spatial abilities tests from small effect size difference for ‘Atlantis’ (0.27) and ‘Triangle’ (0.29) to moderate effect size difference for ‘Number recall’ (0.72) compared to children whose mothers did not consume alcohol during pregnancy; the exposed children scored significantly higher errors with a small effect size (0.37) at problem solving (CCT-1) test compared to unexposed children (Table 3).

At unstandardized and standardized multivariable analysis, children whose mothers reported alcohol consumption during pregnancy performed significantly poorer for memory-‘Atlantis’ (p = 0.03) and ‘Number recall’ (p = 0.0001), and spatial ability tests-‘Triangle’ (p = 0.03); they scored significantly higher errors at problem solving CCT-1 test (p = 0.002); all the results were adjusted for age, sex, schooling, stunting, father’s education, mother’s employment and the promotion of exclusive breastfeeding (Table 4). No statistical association was found for visual abilities-‘Conceptual Thinking’, ‘Face recognition’, ‘Story completion’, and reasoning tests-‘Rover’, ‘Block counting’, and ‘Pattern Reasoning’ (Table 4).

Table 3:
Effect size and bivariate analysis between maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy, KABC-II and CCT-1 performance of children from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.
Effect size Bivariate analysis p-value
Cohen’s d Crude coefficient 95% CI
Atlantis (memory)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.27a −5.45 −9.74 to −1.14 0.01
Conceptual Thinking (visual abilities)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.02 −0.06 −0.82–0.69 0.86
Face recognition (visual abilities)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.10 −0.28 −0.94–0.39 0.41
Story completion (reasoning)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.05 −0.07 −0.3–0.2 0.62
Number recall (memory)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.72b −1.21 −1.59 to −0.84 <0.0001
Rover (reasoning)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.11 −0.2 −0.6–0.2 0.29
Triangle (spatial abilities)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.29a −0.80 −1.42 to −0.18 0.01
Block counting (reasoning)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.19 −0.71 −1.51–0.09 0.08
Word order (memory)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.26 −0.5 −0.8 to −0.06 0.02
Pattern reasoning (reasoning)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.09 −0.09 −0.3–0.1 0.42
CCT-1 errors (problem solving)
No alcohol Reference
Alcohol 0.37a 2.7 1.1–4.3 0.001
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3507/table-3

Notes:

Small effect size from 0.2 to 0.49.
Moderate effect size from 0.5 to 0.79.
Table 4:
Multivariable analysis between maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy, KABC-II and CCT-1 performance of children from the PROMISE Saving Brains study in rural Burkina Faso.
Unstandardized Standardized
coefficienta (95% CI) coefficienta (95% CI) p-value
Atlantis (memory)
No alcohol
Alcohol −4.4 (−8.6 to −0.3) −0.2 (−0.4 to −0.01) 0.03
Conceptual Thinking (visual abilities)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.03 (−0.8–0.7) −0.007 (−0.2–0.2) 0.9
Face recognition (visual abilities)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.1 (−0.8–0.5) −0.04 (−0.3–0.2) 0.7
Story completion (reasoning)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.01 (−0.3–0.2) −0.01 (−0.2–0.2) 0.9
Number recall (memory)
No alcohol
Alcohol −1.1 (−1.5 to −0.7) −0.6 (−0.8 to −0.4) 0.0001
Rover (reasoning)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.2 (−0.6–0.2) −0.1 (−0.3–0.1) 0.3
Triangle (spatial abilities)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.6 (−1.2 to −0.03) −0.2 (−0.4 to −0.01) 0.03
Block counting (reasoning)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.6 (−1.4 to −0.2) −0.2 (−0.4 to −0.06) 0.1
Word order (memory)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.3 (−0.7–0.04) −0.2 (−0.4–0.03) 0.08
Pattern Reasoning (reasoning)
No alcohol
Alcohol −0.1 (−0.3–0.1) −0.1 (−0.3–0.1) 0.3
CCT-1 errors (problem solving)
No alcohol
Alcohol 2.6 (0.9–4.2) 0.4 (0.1–0.6) 0.002
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3507/table-4

Notes:

Adjusted for age, sex, schooling, stunting, father’s education, mother’s employment, and EBF (N = 498).

Discussion

In the present study, we observed an association between maternal alcohol consumption in pregnancy and poorer cognitive performance for memory (‘Atlantis’ and ‘Number recall’), and spatial ability (‘Triangle’) tests as measured by the KABC-II and for problem solving as measured by CCT-1 among children aged 6 to 8 years in rural Burkina Faso. No statistical association was found for visual abilities (‘Conceptual Thinking’, ‘Face recognition’) and reasoning (‘Story completion’, ‘Rover’, ‘Block counting’, ‘Pattern Reasoning’).

Our study was conducted in an African rural context where home brewing is common and most commonly done by women. (Martinez et al., 2011; WHO, 2014b; Popova et al., 2016a). Its cost is low compared to commercially-made alcoholic beverages in many parts of Africa (Mccall, 1996; Willis, 2002) and quantifying its amount is challenging because home brews are often consumed in containers of various sizes (Hahn et al., 2012; Thakarar et al., 2016).

In its first application in rural Burkina Faso, we found variation in performances in the KABC-II and CCT-1. Children were positively engaged in carrying out the tests. Two things might explain the association between maternal alcohol consumption in pregnancy and poorer cognitive performance for ‘Atlantis’, ‘Number recall’, ‘Triangle’ and CCT-1. The first is the heavy home brewing consumption of alcohol during pregnancy. Numerous biological mechanisms have been suggested as contributing to alcohol-induced foetal damage, particularly deficits in brain function (Goodlett & Horn, 2001; Kim et al., 2016). The second is the good level of reliability for ‘Atlantis’, ‘Number recall’, ‘Triangle’ and ‘Block counting’ in accordance with the reliabilities reported in the KABC-II manual (Kaufman et al., 2005). Malda found similar results in India (Malda et al., 2010). These findings compare well some studies. In a recent systematic reviews of the literature which includes 33 relevant studies using cognitive test scores, children prenatally exposed to alcohol performed worse on problem solving, visual-spatial ability and specific domains of memory such as immediate or delayed recall memory compared to children who were prenatally unexposed (Du Plooy et al., 2016). Another review highlighted that heavy prenatal alcohol exposure had adverse effect on spatial abilities (Mattson, Crocker & Nguyen, 2011).

In the present study, we found no statistical association between maternal alcohol consumption in pregnancy and poorer cognitive performance for ‘Conceptual Thinking’, ‘Face recognition’, ‘Story completion’, ‘Rover’, ‘Block counting’ and ‘Pattern Reasoning’. Diverse explanations are possible as to why the children were not responsive to these tests. The most plausible is that the amount of maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy was not enough to be associated with visual abilities and reasoning tests. In our study, the level of alcohol was unknown and might have been very low to detect significance association. These results are similar to other studies which found no difference between low to moderate alcohol consumption during pregnancy and neuro-cognitive outcomes among children (O’Callaghan et al., 2007; Alati et al., 2008; Kelly et al., 2012; Falgreen Eriksen et al., 2012; Kesmodel et al., 2012). Also, the reliability coefficient was low for ‘Story completion’, ‘Rover’ and ‘Pattern Reasoning’ and we found cultural unfamiliarity of the items for ‘Conceptual Thinking’ and ‘Face Recognition’. For example, ‘Face Recognition’ uses mainly photographs of faces from white people to which most children in rural Burkina Faso have not been exposed. In a study in rural Kenya using KABC-I, ‘Face Recognition’ has been adapted by substituted the photographs with those of persons from their region to increase the validity and the reliability of the measures (Holding et al., 2004). Given the fact that our study was implemented in similar context, such adaptations may have contributed to increase the responsiveness of children in our context. The reason of the low internal for ‘Story completion’, ‘Rover’ and ‘Pattern Reasoning’ might be explained by the weak understanding of the items; these tests measure reasoning and the understanding of the items might have been complex for the children due to the cultural context. The potential effect of cultural inappropriateness decreasing the performance has been described in multiple studies (Greenfield, 1997; Malda & Van der Vijver, 2008). While cognitive constructs appear to be universal (Koziol et al., 2014), the cultural context influences the engagement of the test taker in the testing process, and thus, potentially the reliability and validity of tests (Malda & Van der Vijver, 2008). Adaptations of the tests may therefore be needed to ensure the responsiveness of a test to group differences (Holding et al., 2004; Alcock et al., 2008). Thus, these sub-tests may differentiate children in our context after adaptations.

Our study has several strengths. Firstly, the risk of selection bias is small; the participants were part of a community-based cluster-randomised trial of children (Diallo et al., 2010; Diallo et al., 2011; Tylleskär et al., 2011; Hama Diallo et al., 2012). In addition, only two participants declined to be tested in the study. Secondly, the assessment was based on a standardized measure of cognition for children which has been widely used in a number of countries, also in Africa (Boivin, 2002; Bangirana et al., 2009; Boivin et al., 2010; Ruel et al., 2012; Bogale et al., 2013; Taljaard et al., 2013; Bumoko et al., 2015; Rochat et al., 2016; Brahmbhatt et al., 2017; Ajayi et al., 2017). In addition, the assessments were performed by trained psychologists who were blinded to the main exposure (maternal alcohol consumption). Thirdly, adjustment for the potential confounders was done in the analysis.

However, the study also has some limitations. The assessment of alcohol consumption was self-reported based on a dichotomous response without further probing. Therefore, we have no information about the volume, frequency and concentration of alcohol consumed. Misclassification of maternal alcohol consumption, in particular under-reporting and recall bias due to the recall time and social desirability cannot be excluded. However, the relatively high self-reported frequency of prenatal alcohol consumption might indicate that the population is naïve to health system information on the harmful effects on alcohol consumption in pregnancy and provide answers with limited social desirability. Given the relatively high frequency of drinking one could also assume that only ‘visible’ drinking of a certain ‘magnitude’ is reported and ‘sips’; ‘low alcohol beverages’ and ‘ritual drinking’ is not counted as drinking. Another limitation is the lack of overall reliability and validity of the measures which were used for the first time in the country and were not normed in the settings.

We consider this paper to be important as it demonstrates an association between maternal alcohol consumption and the poor cognitive performance among children in Burkina Faso. The study highlights the need to raise awareness of the risks of maternal alcohol consumption on the offspring’s cognitive performance. Healthcare professionals may have an important role in advising the public on its potential consequences. Prevention initiatives need to be designed and advice on abstaining from drinking during pregnancy needs to be provided. Strategies of monitoring alcohol intake on women and children may be considered during antenatal and postnatal visits. The cognitive outcome measures needs to be validated in the local context and culturally adapted.

Conclusions

Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy is associated with poorer cognitive performance for memory, spatial ability, and problem solving tests in the offspring in rural Burkina Faso. Futures studies needs to assess in more detail the maternal alcohol consumption patterns in Burkina Faso and possible preventive strategies.

Supplemental Information

13 Citations   Views   Downloads