Skip to main content
Erschienen in: neuropsychiatrie 1/2018

Open Access 11.01.2018 | original article

Maternal bonding behavior, adult intimate relationship, and quality of life

verfasst von: Michaela Schmoeger, Ph.D., Matthias Deckert, Petra Wagner, Ulrike Sirsch, Ulrike Willinger

Erschienen in: neuropsychiatrie | Ausgabe 1/2018

Summary

Continuity and discontinuity in the development of social relationships have been investigated by reviewing the course of social bonds and by analyzing the effects of a sound intimate relationship in adulthood in conjunction with recalled maternal bonding on the quality of life among students.
A questionnaire-based study of 207 students was conducted. Perceptions of maternal bonding were designated as being representative of one of the two contrasting bonding types “optimal maternal bonding” and “affectionless maternal control” assessed by the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) and combined with perceptions of a sound intimate adult relationship measured by the Family Assessment Measure III Dyadic Relationships Scale (FAM-III-D). Quality of life and general health data were determined by using the World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL-BREF) instrument.
Students who reported “optimal maternal bonding” had intimate relationships in adulthood that were of significantly higher quality than those who recalled “affectionless maternal control”. Students who recalled “optimal maternal bonding” and described their intimate relationship as sound showed significantly higher scores in all domains of quality of life and indicated having better general health than those who reported “affectionless maternal control” and a sound intimate relationship.
A sound intimate relationship in adulthood does not appear to compensate the impact of a recalled maternal bonding behavior in terms of affectionless control, on quality of life. Furthermore, results seem to support the hypothesis of continuity of the development of social relationships among psychologically well individuals based on the association between maternal bonding and later intimate relationships.

Introduction

Bonding represents the emotional attachment between the primary caregiver and the child, appears shortly after birth, and implies a special and focused relationship towards the caregiver’s own offspring [13]. Bonding is a biological and adaptive process that warrants the satisfaction of the child’s needs for nurture and protection by its caregivers, primarily by the mother. The sensitivity of the mother in reading the signals made by the child, and an appropriate and quick response to these signals helps the child to develop a sense of trust in the world and in later social relationships. Individual differences in the security of child and parent relationships are thought to be important due to their implications for later development. Different patterns of attachment reflect the child’s different expectations or internal working models of the social world [13].
According to Bowlby [13], the development of social relationships is continuous. Early socialization experiences strongly influence any later capacity to form affectionate bonds. These experiences are gradually incorporated into the individual’s psychological organization and significantly influence the course of other relationships [4]. Childhood experiences are the principal determinants of abnormalities in adult relationships and can consequently lead to increased psychopathology [5]. Hazan and Shaver [6] suggested romantic love as an attachment process. Adults with insecure intimate relationships presented more negative expectations and beliefs about love, histories of shorter intimate relationships and less favorable childhood experiences with their parents than adults with secure intimate relationships. In subsequent research, disadvantages of insecure compared to secure attached individuals with respect to several aspects of adult intimate relationships have been reported (e. g. [79]).
In contrast, Belsky and Nezworski [10] argue that later social relationships may not be influenced continuously but may change in adulthood. The association between early bonding and attachment experiences and later social relationships may be described, according to Main Kaplan, and Cassidy [11], as being discontinuous. They reported that parents, who recalled their childhood attachments as insecure work cognitively through negative issues, become sufficiently secure in their own parenting skills and form secure attachments with their children. According to Parker et al. [5] any impairment in relationships between parents and children, except perhaps gross parental deprivation, seem to be subject to modification by a range of experiences, in particular subsequent interpersonal relationships.
Gittleman, Klein, Smider, and Essex [12] suggested that both continuity and discontinuity principles can be applied to understand links between early experiences with parents, adult relationships and adult mental health. They investigated associations of parental bonding behavior, adult attachment and adult mental health by considering independent, mediating, or moderating effects. They found patterns of moderating effects of adult relationship styles on the association between parental bonding behavior and adult mental health.
In the present study two research questions regarding the continuity versus discontinuity hypotheses of the development of social relationships were considered. First, the course of social relationships was investigated by testing the differences between two contrasting types of recalled maternal bonding behavior, namely “optimal maternal bonding” and “affectionless maternal control” [13], with respect to the soundness of adult intimate relationship. Second, a possible mediating effect of a healthy intimate relationship regarding the association between maternal bonding behavior and quality of life in adulthood was investigated. The two types of recalled maternal bonding behavior, “optimal maternal bonding” and “affectionless maternal control”, were combined with a sound intimate relationship in adulthood. If an intimate relationship buffers the effect of a negative maternal bonding type on quality of life, no differences between the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound intimate relationship” and “affectionless maternal control/sound intimate relationship” with respect to quality of life should occur.

Methods

Subjects

A sample of 207 students enrolled at the University of Vienna was recruited. The sample consisted of 171 (83%) female and 36 (17%) male students (CHI2 = 88.043; df = 1; p ≤ 0.0001). The students had been attending the university for a mean period of six terms (SD = 4.0); the mean age of the students was 24.64 years (SD = 6.27). 169 (82%) of the students were studying psychology, 38 (18%) were studying other disciplines. Being in a current intimate relationship was an inclusion criterion for participants. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to the study.

Measures

Information about self-rated maternal bonding behavior during the first 16 years was assessed with the German version [14] of the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) [13], which contains two scales. Care was evaluated by 12 items on a continuum, ranging from empathy, closeness, emotional warmth, and affection to neglect, indifference, and emotional coldness. Control was measured by 13 items, ranging from overprotection, intrusion, excessive contact, control, infantilization, and prevention of independent behavior to autonomy and allowance of independence. Both the validity and the reliability of the PBI have been shown to be acceptable [13]. The scales of the PBI have been shown to be stable over time [15] and independent of current mood state [16] and the recipient’s gender [13].
Information about the quality of the intimate relationship was measured by the two “affective” [17] dimensions “involvement” and “affective expression” by means of the German version [17] of the Family Assessment Measure III Dyadic Relationships Scale (FAM-III-D) [18]. “Involvement” represents the quantity and quality of interest the partners have for each other. Emotional needs should be complementary in order to guarantee a feeling of security, a sense of togetherness and mutual esteem for appreciation of one another, a higher level of self-esteem, and independence. “Affective expression” reflects a dialectic relation between the security of the partnership and the autonomy of the individuals.
Information about quality of life and general health was gathered with the German version [19] of the World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL-BREF) instrument [20], which is composed of 26 items assigned to four domains. The physical domain represents constraints on quality of life by pain and discomfort, sleep and rest, energy and fatigue, mobility, activities of daily living, dependence on medicinal substances and medical aids, and work capacity. The psychological domain covers questions regarding positive feelings, thinking, learning, memory and concentration, self-esteem, body image and appearance, negative feelings, and spirituality/religion/personal beliefs. The domain social relationships reflects personal relationships, social support, and sexual activity. The domain environment is concerned with freedom, physical safety and security, home environment, financial resources, health and social care considering accessibility and quality, opportunities for acquiring new information and skills, participation in and opportunities for recreation/leisure activity, physical environment (pollution/noise/traffic/climate), and transport.

Statistical analyses

Two groups of students were formed with respect to the two maternal bonding types and the quality of adult intimate relationship: one group with a recalled “optimal maternal bonding type” (high level of care and low level of control), combined with a healthy intimate relationship in adulthood, and the other with a recalled “affectionless maternal control type” (low level of care and high level of control), also combined with a sound adult intimate relationship. High levels regarding care and control refer to values above the median, low levels to values below the median. A sound adult intimate relationship is represented by values below the median on the two dimensions “involvement” and “affective expression”. Due to the reciprocal coding of the FAM-III-D, lower scores on these two dimensions indicate a sound intimate relationship.
Group differences between females and males within the student sample were tested for significance by chi-square tests. Group differences between the two groups of students were analyzed for significance by t‑tests. The cut-off level for statistical significance was set at p < 0.05, 2‑tailed. Alpha adjustment for multiple comparisons was applied using Holm’s Sequential Bonferroni Procedure [21]. All statistical analyses were performed by IBM SPSS Statistics 24.

Results

Within the sample two maternal bonding types were distributed in the following way: 74 students (37%) recalled high levels of maternal care and low levels of maternal control, conceptualized as the “optimal bonding type” and 75 (37%) recalled low levels of maternal care and high levels of maternal control, conceptualized as “affectionless control”. The other two maternal types, not used for further analyses in this study, namely “affectionate constraint” and “absent of weak bonding” were equally shared by the remaining 56 (26%) students. (For more detail see [22]). According to the central limit theorem, normal distribution can be assumed for group comparisons [23]. A post hoc power analysis (N1 = 74, N2 = 75, d = 0.05) showed a power of 86% for the final sample size of 149 [24].
There were no significant differences between the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound intimate relationship” (sound “involvement” and sound “affective expression”, respectively) and “maternal affectionless control/sound intimate relationship” (sound “involvement” and sound “affective expression”, respectively) with respect to gender (CHI2 = 1.09; df = 1; p 0.297, CHI2 = 2.46; df = 1; p = 0.117, respectively). Consequently, the two groups were combined in the statistical analyses.
With respect to the first research question, significant differences were found between the two bonding types “optimal maternal bonding” and “affectionless control” regarding the two adult intimate relationship variables “involvement” (t = 2.7; df = 134; p = 0.007) and “affective expression” (t = 3.3; df = 112.7; p = 0.001). Students who reported an “optimal maternal bonding” showed a higher quality of “involvement” and “affective expression” in adulthood. Means and standard deviations are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Means and standard deviations among the two groups “optimal maternal bonding” and “affectionless maternal control” with respect to “involvement” and “affective expression”
 
Group 1
Optimal maternal bonding
Group 2
Affectionless maternal control
Involvementa
2.0 (±1.8)
2.9 (±2.0)
Affective expressiona
1.1 (±1.4)
2.2 (±2.4)
aLower scores indicate higher quality on both dimensions
As to the second research question, significant differences were found between the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound adult intimate relationship” and “affectionless maternal control/sound adult intimate relationship” with respect to all four domains as well as the “general health” section of the WHOQOL. Students who recalled an “optimal maternal bonding” and described their adult intimate relationship as healthy (sound “involvement” and sound “affective expression”, respectively) showed significantly higher scores in “general health” (t = 4.0; df = 49.3; p ≤ 0.0001, t = 3.7; df = 52.1; p = 0.001, respectively), in the “physical domain” (t = 2.5; df = 49.4; p = 0.016, t = 2.2; df = 76; p = 0.03, respectively), in the “psychological domain” (t = 3.7; df = 50.5; p ≤ 0.0001, t = 3.1; df = 50.2; p = 0.004, respectively), in the domain “social relationships” (t = 2.5; df = 79; p = 0.015, t = 3.5; df = 76; p = 0.001, respectively), and in the domain “environment” (t = 3.0; df = 78; p = 0.004, t = 2.9; df = 75; p = 0.006, respectively) in comparison to students who recalled “maternal affectionless control” and had a healthy intimate relationship. Results are significant at the p < 0.05 level after Holm-Bonferroni correction [21]. Means and standard deviations are shown in Tables 2 and 3.
Table 2
Means and standard deviations among the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound adult intimate relationship: intimate” and “affectionless maternal control/sound adult intimate relationship: intimate” with respect to quality of life
 
Group I
Optimal maternal bonding/sound adult intimate relationship: involvement
Group II
Affectionless maternal control/sound adult intimate relationship: involvement
General health
87.2 (±13.3)
70.8 (±21.1)
Physical domain
89.3 (±8.5)
82.6 (±13.5)
Psychological domain
81.6 (±8.5)
71.8 (±13.1)
Domain social relationships
85.9 (±13.0)
77.5 (±17.4)
Domain environment
79.0 (±11.4)
70.2 (±15.0)
Table 3
Means and standard deviations among the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound adult intimate relationship: affective expression” and “affectionless maternal control/sound adult intimate relationship: affective expression” with respect to quality of life
 
Group I
Optimal maternal bonding/sound adult intimate relationship: affective expression
Group II
Affectionless maternal control/sound adult intimate relationship: affective expression
General health
88.3 (±12.6)
74.2 (±19.0)
Physical domain
89.3 (±8.6)
84.3 (±11.3)
Psychological domain
78.9 (±10.5)
70.8 (±14.2)
Domain social relationships
86.2 (±12.7)
75.4 (±17.0)
Domain environment
79.0 (±11.4)
70.2 (±15.0)

Discussion

Students who recalled “optimal maternal bonding”, defined by a high level of maternal care and low level of maternal control [13] indicated having intimate relationships in adulthood that were of significantly higher quality than students who recalled “affectionless maternal control”, defined by a low level of maternal care and high level of maternal control [13]. High levels of maternal empathy, closeness, emotional warmth, and low levels of overprotection, intrusion, excessive contact, control, and infantilization were related to an intimate relationship of high quality, defined by a high level of complementary emotional needs for both partners in order to guarantee a feeling of security, a sense of togetherness and mutual esteem for appreciation of one another, a higher level of self-esteem, independence, a high level of security within the partnership and high autonomy exhibited by both partners.
This result is in accordance with previously reported research findings showing that the perception of poor childhood support was related to lower sociability scores and a recalled lack of satisfactory peer relationships among students [25], that care scales (PBI) were associated with measures of social support made at the same time in a student group and that maternal care was also correlated with social satisfaction made one and a half years later [26], and that low levels of childhood care from both mothers and fathers were related to low levels of perceived social support among medical students [27]. Associations between parent–child bonding and later social bonding were rather found among those individuals who are psychologically well. However, these studies did not specifically investigate intimate adult relationships but rather social relationships in general. Some studies reported a lack of evidence to support the developmental continuity of social relationships (early and later ones) [2831], especially for patients with a psychiatric disorder.
Moreover this result seems to support the hypothesis of continuity of the development of social relationships among psychologically well individuals on the basis of the association between maternal bonding and later intimate relationships. According to Bowlby [13] early bonding and attachment experiences are mentally represented as “internal working models” which influence the course of later social relationships. Children internalize experiences with caretakers in such a way that early attachment relationships come to form a prototype for later relationships. Different patterns of attachment reflect a child’s different expectations or internal working models of the social world. Internal working models include expectations about the caregiver’s availability, the infant’s own worthiness and ability to obtain care, and social relationships in general. Experiences of reliable and responsive care will lead to a model of the caregiver as available, of the self as worthy of care and effective in obtaining it, and of social relationships as pleasurable and rewarding [32].
Security of attachment is thought to be important due to its implications for later development [13]. Securely attached infants seem to be more socially competent, they tend to be more cooperative and obedient, they have better relations with their peers [33, 34] and they are less likely to develop emotional or behavior problems [35]. Children with secure attachment spend less time alone, less time with counselors, and more time with other children than do youngsters who had been attached insecurely as infants and toddler. Furthermore, they spend more time in groups of same-gender peers [36] and show more positive and less negative child–friend interactions [37]. They are less often ridiculed or excluded from group activities in middle childhood [38]. They tend to choose each other as friends, their friendships are likely to be reciprocal rather than one-sided, and a close friendship among secure children does not prevent them from functioning in the larger peer group. They often spend time together in groups, and their play together also includes other children [39]. Parental support, warmth, and authoritativeness are also connected with positive outcomes in adolescents [40].
Studies considering associations between early socialization experiences, quality of intimate relationships and mental health in adulthood concluded that the quality of current attachments might buffer the negative impact of low parental care on mental health [41]. Gittleman et al. [12] reported that adult attachment styles had few mediating effects on the association of parental bonding behavior and adult mental health. Interactions between the fearful style, parental care and control seem to accentuate the effect of high parental care or low parental control on mental health in both men and women. The secure adult relationship style seems to moderate somewhat the negative impact of high parental control in women.
The current study also analyzed the associations between early socialization experiences, defined as the combination of maternal care and control; adult intimate relationship, as reflected by the quantity and quality of interest exhibited by the partners for one another, security within the partnership as well as partner autonomy; and quality of life, whereby adult relationship could possibly moderate the effect of maternal bonding on quality of life. If a healthy adult intimate relationship compensates the effect of a negative maternal bonding type on quality of life, there should not be any differences between the two groups “optimal maternal bonding/sound intimate relationship” and “affectionless maternal control/sound intimate relationship” with respect to quality of life. However, in the present study, a healthy intimate relationship in adulthood did not appear to buffer the impact of recalled maternal bonding behavior characterized by a low level of care and high level of control, which Parker et al. [13] conceptualized as “affectionless control”, on the quality of life. Students who recalled an “optimal maternal bonding” and described their intimate relationship as being sound showed a significantly lower reduction of their quality of life on dimensions of pain and discomfort, sleep and rest, energy and fatigue, and higher values on parameters measuring positive feelings, thinking, learning, memory and concentration, self-esteem, bodily image and appearance, personal relationships, social support, sexual activity, freedom, physical safety and security than students with recalled maternal affectionless control and a healthy intimate relationship.
Gittleman et al. [12] found mediating and moderating effects in their study. The effect of maternal care on depression among women was mediated by the security of their closest relationships. Men and women with secure adult relationship styles showed low levels of distress regardless of levels of parental bonding behavior. Parental control among men or low parental care in combination with a fearful style were related to increased psychological distress. However, they also reported that while the fearful style was associated with higher levels of distress independent of recalled parental control, high levels of control, especially maternal control, were related to increased depression and anxiety among women with secure styles, although these levels never rose to the scores exhibited by those with fearful styles. Gittleman et al. [12] concluded that both continuity and discontinuity principles can be applied to the understanding of associations between early experiences with parents, adult relationships and mental health, and they suggested that it seemed to be more fruitful to consider the circumstances in which each applies rather than considering continuity and discontinuity as opposing hypotheses.
The results of the current study suggest support for the continuity hypothesis and show no buffer effect of a high quality intimate relationship in adulthood on the association of early maternal care and control and quality of life among psychologically well individuals. In short, the special circumstances in the present study were the effects social bonds had on both quality of life, instead of mental health as found in other studies, and on intimate adult relationships among psychological well individuals, instead of social support in general as determined in other studies.
Bowlby [42, p. 9] pointed out: “… I believe there is already sufficient evidence, […], that points to the very substantial influence on personality development and mental health of the way an individual’s parents (or in some cases parent substitutes) treat him or her”. In this context it seems that early childhood experiences of high-quality bonding do not only have an impact on personality development and mental health but also on the basic quality of life.
Nevertheless, some potential limitations of the current study concerning the sample must be taken into consideration. Participants were students, mostly psychology students, with a mean age of approximately 25 years. The selection of included subjects might have hand an influence on the outcome of the study. Psychology students might for example have higher interest in social interactions than the general population or show higher values of selflessness due to their biography. Both factors could have an impact on intimate relationships in adulthood. Furthermore, the age of the included subjects might indirectly influence the results because it can be assumed that many participants may not have experiences with very long-standing sound intimate relationships. The limited duration of sound adult intimate relationships might have influenced the results.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the contribution of Eva Mayr for the input of data.

Compliance with ethical guidelines

Conflict of interest

M. Schmoeger, M. Deckert, P. Wagner, U. Sirsch and U. Willinger declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical standards

All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

neuropsychiatrie

Print-Titel

• Übersichten zu relevanten Themen des Fachs

• Für alle Berufsgruppen, die sich mit Ursachen, Erscheinungsformen
und Behandlungsmöglichkeiten von psychischen Störungen beschäftigen

• Fach -und Fortbildungszeitschrift der ÖGPP und ÖGKJP

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowlby J. Separation. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1971. Bowlby J. Separation. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1971.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowlby J. Separation, anxiety and anger. Attachment and loss, Vol. 2. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1975. Bowlby J. Separation, anxiety and anger. Attachment and loss, Vol. 2. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1975.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowlby J. Loss. Attachment and loss, Vol. 3. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1980. Bowlby J. Loss. Attachment and loss, Vol. 3. Harmondsworth: Penguin; 1980.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52(3):511–24.CrossRefPubMed Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52(3):511–24.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Nezworski T. Clinical implications of attachment. Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 3–17. Belsky J, Nezworski T. Clinical implications of attachment. Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 3–17.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Gittleman MG, Klein MH, Smider NA, Essex MJ. Recollections of parental behaviour, adult attachment and mental health: mediating and moderating effects. Psychol Med. 1998;28(6):1443–55.CrossRefPubMed Gittleman MG, Klein MH, Smider NA, Essex MJ. Recollections of parental behaviour, adult attachment and mental health: mediating and moderating effects. Psychol Med. 1998;28(6):1443–55.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Mackinnon AJ, Henderson AS, Scott R, Duncan-Jones P. The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI): an epidemiological study in a general population sample. Psychol Med. 1989;19(4):1023–34.CrossRefPubMed Mackinnon AJ, Henderson AS, Scott R, Duncan-Jones P. The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI): an epidemiological study in a general population sample. Psychol Med. 1989;19(4):1023–34.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cierpka M, Die Familienbögen FG. Ein Inventar zur Einschätzung von Familienfunktionen (B-Zweierbeziehungsfragebogen). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1994. Cierpka M, Die Familienbögen FG. Ein Inventar zur Einschätzung von Familienfunktionen (B-Zweierbeziehungsfragebogen). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1994.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner HA, Steinhauer PD, Santa-Barbara J. Familiy assessment measure – III manual. Toronto: Multi Health Systems; 1995. Skinner HA, Steinhauer PD, Santa-Barbara J. Familiy assessment measure – III manual. Toronto: Multi Health Systems; 1995.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer MC, Kilian R, Matschinger H. WHOQOL-100 und WHOQOL-BREF. Handbuch fuer die deutschsprachige Version der WHO Instrumente zur Erfassung von Lebensqualitaet. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2000. Angermeyer MC, Kilian R, Matschinger H. WHOQOL-100 und WHOQOL-BREF. Handbuch fuer die deutschsprachige Version der WHO Instrumente zur Erfassung von Lebensqualitaet. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2000.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat WHOQOL Group. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. The WHOQOL Group. Psychol Med. 1998;28(3):551–8.CrossRef WHOQOL Group. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. The WHOQOL Group. Psychol Med. 1998;28(3):551–8.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Abdi H. Holm’s sequential Bonferroni procedure. In: Salkind N, editor. Encyclopedia of research design. Thousand Oaks: SAGE; 2010. pp. 574–8. Abdi H. Holm’s sequential Bonferroni procedure. In: Salkind N, editor. Encyclopedia of research design. Thousand Oaks: SAGE; 2010. pp. 574–8.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmoeger M, Deckert M, Sirsch U, Wagner P, Willinger U. Recalled maternal bonding and quality of life in young adults. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2017. Schmoeger M, Deckert M, Sirsch U, Wagner P, Willinger U. Recalled maternal bonding and quality of life in young adults. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2017.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. London: SAGE; 2009. Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. London: SAGE; 2009.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.CrossRefPubMed Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hojat M, Borenstein BD, Shapurian R. Perception of childhood dissatisfaction with parents and selected personality traits in adulthood. J Gen Psychol. 1990;117(3):241–53.PubMed Hojat M, Borenstein BD, Shapurian R. Perception of childhood dissatisfaction with parents and selected personality traits in adulthood. J Gen Psychol. 1990;117(3):241–53.PubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Hickie I, Parker G, Wilhelm K, Tennant C. Perceived interpersonal risk factors of non-endogenous depression. Psychol Med. 1991;21(2):399–412.CrossRefPubMed Hickie I, Parker G, Wilhelm K, Tennant C. Perceived interpersonal risk factors of non-endogenous depression. Psychol Med. 1991;21(2):399–412.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Truant G, Herscovitch J, Lohrenz J. The relationship of childhood experience to the quality of marriage. Can J Psychiatry. 1987;32(2):87–92.CrossRefPubMed Truant G, Herscovitch J, Lohrenz J. The relationship of childhood experience to the quality of marriage. Can J Psychiatry. 1987;32(2):87–92.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Truant GS. Personality diagnosis and childhood care associated with adult marital quality. Can J Psychiatry. 1994;39(5):269–76.CrossRefPubMed Truant GS. Personality diagnosis and childhood care associated with adult marital quality. Can J Psychiatry. 1994;39(5):269–76.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowlby J. Separation. New York: Basic Books; 1973. Bowlby J. Separation. New York: Basic Books; 1973.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Fagot BI. Attachment, parenting, and peer interactions of toddler children. Dev Psychol. 1997;33(3):489–99.CrossRefPubMed Fagot BI. Attachment, parenting, and peer interactions of toddler children. Dev Psychol. 1997;33(3):489–99.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Kerns KA. Individual differences in friendship quality: links to child-mother attachment. In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, editors. The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge studies in social and emotional development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 137–57. Kerns KA. Individual differences in friendship quality: links to child-mother attachment. In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, editors. The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge studies in social and emotional development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 137–57.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Elicker J, Englund M, Sroufe LA. Predicting peer competence and peer relationships in childhood from early parent-child relationships. Family-peer relationships: modes of linkage. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 77–106. Elicker J, Englund M, Sroufe LA. Predicting peer competence and peer relationships in childhood from early parent-child relationships. Family-peer relationships: modes of linkage. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 77–106.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Grossmann KE, Grossmann K. Attachment quality as an organizer of emotional and behavioral responses in a longitudinal perspective. Attachment across the life cycle. US: Routledge; 1991. pp. 93–114. Grossmann KE, Grossmann K. Attachment quality as an organizer of emotional and behavioral responses in a longitudinal perspective. Attachment across the life cycle. US: Routledge; 1991. pp. 93–114.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Urban poverty and the family context of delinquency: a new look at structure and process in a classic study. Child Dev. 1994;65(2 Spec No):523–40.CrossRefPubMed Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Urban poverty and the family context of delinquency: a new look at structure and process in a classic study. Child Dev. 1994;65(2 Spec No):523–40.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson L, Stevens N. Associations between early experiences with parents and well-being in old age. J Gerontol. 1993;48(3):P109–16.CrossRefPubMed Andersson L, Stevens N. Associations between early experiences with parents and well-being in old age. J Gerontol. 1993;48(3):P109–16.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Maternal bonding behavior, adult intimate relationship, and quality of life
verfasst von
Michaela Schmoeger, Ph.D.
Matthias Deckert
Petra Wagner
Ulrike Sirsch
Ulrike Willinger
Publikationsdatum
11.01.2018
Verlag
Springer Vienna
Erschienen in
neuropsychiatrie / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 0948-6259
Elektronische ISSN: 2194-1327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40211-017-0258-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

neuropsychiatrie 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

bericht aus dem ögpp-vorstand

Bericht aus dem Vorstand

bericht aus dem ögkjp-vorstand

Bericht aus dem Präsidium der ÖGKJP

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.