Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Trials 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Study protocol

Mechanisms and modulators of cognitive training gain transfer in cognitively healthy aging: study protocol of the AgeGain study

verfasst von: Dominik Wolf, Oliver Tüscher, Stefan Teipel, Andreas Mierau, Heiko Strüder, Alexander Drzezga, Bernhard Baier, Harald Binder, Andreas Fellgiebel, the German AgeGain study group

Erschienen in: Trials | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Cognitively healthy older people can increase their performance in cognitive tasks through training. However, training effects are mostly limited to the trained task; thus, training effects only poorly transfer to untrained tasks or other contexts, which contributes to reduced adaptation abilities in aging. Stabilizing transfer capabilities in aging would increase the chance of persistent high performance in activities of daily living including longer independency, and prolonged active participation in social life. The trial AgeGain aims at elaborating the physiological brain mechanisms of transfer in aging and supposed major modulators of transfer capability, especially physical activity, cerebral vascular lesions, and amyloid burden.

Methods

This 4-year interventional, multicenter, phase 2a cognitive and physical training study will enroll 237 cognitively healthy older subjects in four recruiting centers. The primary endpoint of this trial is the prediction of transfer of cognitive training gains. Secondary endpoints are the structural connectivity of the corpus callosum, Default Mode Network activity, brain-derived neurotrophic factors, motor fitness, and maximal oxygen uptake.

Discussion

Cognitive transfer allows making use of cognitive training gains in everyday life. Thus, maintenance of transfer capability with aging increases the chance of persistent self-guidance and prolonged active participation in social life, which may support a good quality of life. The AgeGain study aims at identifying older people who will most benefit from cognitive training. It will increase the understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of transfer in aging and will help in determining the impact of physical activity and sport as well as pathologic factors (such as cerebrovascular disease and amyloid load) on transfer capability.

Trial registration

German Clinical Trials Register (DRKS), ID: DRKS00013077. Registered on 19 November 2017.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13063-018-2688-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
ACT
Combined aerobic and coordination training
AE
Adverse event
APOE
Apolipoprotein E
BDNF
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor
CETP
Cholesteryl ester transfer protein
DTI
Diffusion-tensor imaging
eCRF
Electronic Case Report Form
FA
Fractional anisotropy
fMRI
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
GPAQ
Global Physical Activity Questionnaire
HAROLD
Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults
HOA
Healthy older adult
MRI
Magnetic resonance imaging
NP
Neuropsychological examination
NT
Non-transfer
PET
Positron-emission tomography
SNP
Single nucleotide polymorphism
ST
Stable tansfer
VO2max
Maximum oxygen uptake
VO2peak
Peak oxygen uptake

Background

Scientific background

Cognitive health has been consistently quoted as significant for life quality by older people and is regarded as an important contributor to late-life functioning [1, 2]. Patterns of cognitive change show great variation in healthy aging [3]. Cognitive training might contribute to enhance or preserve cognitive skills in cognitively healthy older adults (HOA). While improvements in cognition through training have been reported frequently in HOA, the capability to transfer cognitive training gains decreases with age [48]. Ideally, cognitive training would not only improve the function of trained cognitive tasks but also of untrained tasks of the same or a different cognitive domain (i.e., transfer of training gains). There is little evidence for successful transfer of cognitive training effects in HOA [6, 9]. Moreover, although transfer has been the subject of research for many years [10] the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the complex capability of transfer are still unknown. We recently demonstrated successful transfer of logical reasoning training gains to fluid intelligence immediately after a 4-week training in 71% of the HOA (N = 29 out of 41), but this effect persisted only in a subgroup of 22% over a 3-month follow-up period (N = 9 out of 41) [11].
When we assessed structural underpinnings of successful transfer in this previous study [11], we focused on corpus callosum integrity as a structural prerequisite for successful bihemispheric cooperation, since cross-hemisphere processing results in better performance than within-hemisphere processing in complex cognitive tasks [12]. Since the callosal structural integrity is related to age (see Fig. 1a), and age-related disruption of corpus callosum microstructure in HOA impacts the efficiency of bihemispheric processing [13], we expected that associations between age and transfer might be moderated by the structural integrity of the corpus callosum. While short-term transfer was not related to structural integrity, stable transfer (ST) was predicted by the structural integrity/ connectivity of the corpus callosum [11]. Based on this preliminary evidence [11] we hypothesize that bihemispheric processing supported by intact corpus callosum integrity is a precondition of successful transfer in HOA (see Fig. 1a and b). Beside age, several other factors are likely to modulate corpus callosum integrity and other preconditions of successful bihemispheric processing and thereby facilitate or impair the transfer capability in HOA, such as physical activity or physical training, cerebral vascular or cerebral amyloid pathology, genetic factors of risk or resilience, and general intelligence [14, 15].

Objectives

Primary objective

Four types of transfer training interventions have been described: strategy-based (e.g., logical reasoning), multimodal (complex, often social or lifestyle interventions), cardiovascular (i.e., physical training), and specific cognitive process-targeted training interventions (e.g., working memory) [16]. Transfer effects differ between transfer types but share a common feature: transfer effects decline with age [16]. To date, little is known about the neural mechanisms of transfer [16]. Specifically, the mechanisms of either maintenance or age-related decrease of transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA are largely unknown. Increased bihemispheric cooperation can reliably be seen with increasing processing demands in young adults and, as we showed, already on lower levels of cognitive demand in HOA [17]. This compensatory increase of bihemispheric processing in HOA is known as the concept of Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults (HAROLD) [12]. This phenomenon could be the functional link between the observed association of transfer capabilities and structural integrity of the corpus callosum. Beyond pure compensation, HAROLD may serve as a mechanism enabling the structurally and functionally altered aged brain to use alternative neural circuitry and thereby to re-establish more efficient lateralized processing during learning [16]. Hence, increased bihemispheric cooperation/HAROLD, based on the structural integrity of the corpus callosum, may indeed be a functional mechanism mediating transfer capabilities in HOA.
Main research goal of the AgeGain study is to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms of transfer of cognitive training gains in detail by elaborating brain network capabilities representing transfer using a multimodal-neuroimaging approach in HOA. Specifically, the following primary hypothesis will be tested: Structural integrity of the corpus callosum (high / low) and bihemispheric cooperation (reduced / increased) will predict successful transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA. The association of corpus callosum integrity with transfer capability will be determined by diffusion-tensor imaging (DTI) measures of structural integrity / connectivity, mainly fractional anisotropy (FA) of the corpus callosum, see Fig. 2a. The association of bihemispheric processing and transfer capability will be determined using task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (see Fig. 2b).

Secondary objectives

There is evidence that physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness are associated with reduced brain tissue loss and reduced risk for cognitive impairment in aging humans [18, 19]. Moreover, aerobic exercise training increased gray and white matter volume in the prefrontal cortex [20] of older adults and led to significant improvements of executive function [21, 22]. Further, hippocampal and medial temporal lobe volumes were larger in older adults with greater cardiorespiratory fitness [19, 23], and larger hippocampal volumes mediated improvements in spatial memory [19]. Aerobic exercise training increases cerebral blood volume [24], perfusion [25] and the adult hippocampal volume. It has been suggested that the aforementioned structural changes are mediated by neurotrophins, in particular the brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [26]. We [2730] and others [31, 32] demonstrated that exercise induces significant increases in levels of BDNF and other growth factors, and higher serum BDNF after aerobic training was associated with larger anterior hippocampal volume [33]. Using DTI, more recent studies measured brain structural integrity as a function of cardiorespiratory fitness and aerobic exercise training in HOA. Fractional anisotropy of the corpus callosum was reported to be positively correlated to cardiorespiratory fitness in HOA [34]. Thus, cardiorespiratory fitness might modulate age-related alterations of corpus callosum integrity. In addition, greater aerobic fitness derived from a walking program was associated with increases in white matter integrity of the frontal and temporal lobes, and improvement of memory performance [14]. Consistently, using resting state fMRI analyses, it has been shown that cardiorespiratory fitness is positively associated with functional connectivity in the Default Mode Network, and that this in part mediates improved performance on tasks requiring set-shifting, task switching, and spatial working memory [35]. Furthermore, 1 year of regular walking increased functional connectivity between parts of the frontal, posterior, and temporal cortices within the Default Mode Network and the Frontal Executive Network [35]. Of note, there is evidence that the beneficial effects of physical activity on cognitive performance is not limited to cardiorespiratory fitness but is also present after resistance [3638] and coordination exercise training [39, 40]. Moreover, different types of physical training have been shown to affect different neurocognitive networks [40, 41]. We hypothesize that coordination exercise training positively modulates cognitive transfer abilities in HOA based on the following rationale: coordination training aims at improving the efficiency of complex body movements including eye-hand coordination, bimanual coordination, leg-arm coordination, and reactions to moving objects [40]. Such complex movements require bihemispheric interactions via the corpus callosum [42], and both, size and structural integrity of the corpus callosum typically change after sensorimotor skill acquisition [43]. These interactions are probably not limited to pure motor processes but rather include selective aspects of cognition necessary for adaptive motor behavior such as attention, inhibitory and facilitatory processes, working memory, and error-related processing. In line with this, it has been demonstrated that rather anterior corpus callosum projections to the prefrontal cortex than middle corpus callosum projections to the primary motor cortex predict bimanual motor learning [44].
In sum, aerobic exercise – and coordination exercise training are associated with positive structural and functional brain changes as well as improvements of cognitive performance in HOA. Moreover, cerebral regions targeted by these interventions are primarily involved in age-related cognitive decline including the corpus callosum, the hippocampus, frontal and temporal cortices, and the Default Mode Network. As a secondary outcome, we will determine if physical training, comprising aerobic and coordination components, modulates transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA. Moreover, we will determine the impact of baseline physical activity on transfer in HOA.
Subcortical cerebrovascular lesions reflected in white matter hyperintensities and cortical β-amyloid aggregation are highly prevalent and strongly age associated with central nevous system (CNS) pathologies [45]. Both findings are suspected to have a deleterious effect on neuronal function. White matter hyperintensities have been found to affect structural connectivity in the brain on the subcortical level [46]. Amyloid pathology, on the other hand, which is a hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease [47], has been discussed to affect regional synaptic function on the cortical level [48]. Although both findings have been associated with manifest cognitive decline or dementia, they are known to be present in a significant proportion of the cognitively healthy elderly, potentially leading to subclinical deficits [49, 50]. White matter hyperintensities frequently observed in the prefrontal cortex in clinically HOA have been linked to subclinical disturbances of executive function, whereas cortical amyloid deposition has been associated with a minor decline in memory function in this population [51]. Transfer capability represents a multidimensional cognitive function which requires executive and memory skills and presumably depends on highly efficient neuronal pathways including bihemispherical communication. Thus, the two mentioned pathologies are supposed to have a cumulative negative effect on transfer performance, even ahead of other cognitive symptoms. Consistently, as a further secondary outcome, the study aims at investigating white matter hyperintensities and cerebral amyloid burden as potential modulators of transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA.
We and others found that Default Mode Network activity is related to cognitive function and cognitive reserve in HOA [52, 53]. Default Mode Network activation during cognitive tasks was accompanied by less default suppression with greater task demand and less connectivity among Default Mode Network regions as well as increased frontal activation [54]. Thus, since cognitive reserve capacity as referring to Default Mode Network might also predict transfer cabilities, resting-state fMRI connectivity measurements will be also conducted [55]. Moreover, based on the comprehensive data survey the study aims at modeling and analyzing multimodal, high-dimensional datasets with respect to transfer prediction and to build a robust individual index of transfer likelihood.
All secondary outcomes are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1
Overview of secondary outcomes
Secondary objectives
To evaluate the predictive value of a preceding aerobic and coordination training for transfer of cognitive training gains in healthy older adults (HOA)
To evaluate the predictive value of baseline physical activity on transfer in HOA. Baseline physical activity will be measured by a 1-week actigraphy and the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (see “Measures” section below).
To evaluate the predictive value of brain vascular lesion (as determined by T2-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), cortical amyloid burden (as determined by positron-emission tomography), higher default mode network activity (as determined by resting state functional MRI (fMRI) for transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA
To model and analyze multimodal, high-dimensional datasets with respect to transfer prediction and to build a robust individual index of transfer likelihood

Methods/design

Study type

This is a longitudinal, interventional, parallel-group, multicenter, multimodal-imaging trial.

Study design

In this 4-year parallel-group, multicenter, multimodal-imaging study, cognitively healthy elderly subjects will be enrolled by three recruiting centers in Germany: Mainz (University Medical Center Mainz - Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy), Rostock (University Medical Center Rostock, Clinic of Psychosomatic and Psychotherapeutic Medicine and German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE)), and Cologne (German Sport University Cologne and University Hospital Cologne – Department of Nuclear Medicine). The experimental protocol can be divided into five phases (see Table 2). In phase 1, inclusion and exclusion criteria will be assessed (screening). In phase 2 (pre-cognitive-training phase), participants will undergo baseline neuropsychological and physical activity assessment, MRI, positron-emission tomography (PET), and genetics. Thereafter, in phase 3 (cognitive-training phase), a cognitive training will be applied over a period of 4 weeks (with three 60-min-long training sessions per week). Directly following, in phase 4 (post-cognitive-training phase), participants will repeat the neuropsychological assessment to determine immediate transfer effects. Finally, in phase 5 (follow-up, after 3 month), a final neuropsychological assessment will be applied to determine ST effects. The study protocol follows the SPIRIT (Standard Protocol Items: Recommendations for Interventional Trials) recommendations (see Additional file 1). 
Table 2
Trial schedule University Medical Center Mainz and University Rostock
https://static-content.springer.com/image/art%3A10.1186%2Fs13063-018-2688-2/MediaObjects/13063_2018_2688_Tab2_HTML.png
aSchedule for each subject
bThe Florbetaben-PET can be postponed to the post-cognitive-training phase or the follow-up phase
Abbreviations: MRI  magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI functional magnetic resonance imaging, PET positron-emission tomography, ApoE4  apolipoprotein E4, CETP Cholesteryl Ester Transfer Protein, SNP  single nucleotide polymorphism, BDNF brain-derived neurotrophic factor
To investigate the impact of a physical training (combined aerobic and coordination training, ACT) on transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA (secondary outcome), subjects recruited at the center in Cologne will undergo an extended experimental protocol with two additional phases prior to the pre-cognitive-training phase (pre-ACT-training phase, ACT-training phase). The primary protocol (as shown in Table 3) will be complemented by a 20-week physical training period and an evaluation of the physical fitness, as well as an additional MRI scan evaluation (see Table 3).
Table 3
Trial schedule German Sport University Cologne/University Hospital Cologne
https://static-content.springer.com/image/art%3A10.1186%2Fs13063-018-2688-2/MediaObjects/13063_2018_2688_Tab3_HTML.png
aSchedule for each subject
bThe Florbetaben-PET can be postponed to the post-cognitive-training phase or the follow-up phase
Abbreviations: MRI  magnetic resonance imaging, PET positron-emission tomography, ApoE4 apolipoprotein E4; CETP  Cholesteryl Ester Transfer Protein, SNP single nucleotide polymorphism, BDNF  brain-derived neurotrophic factor

Population

Sample size

It is planned to include 237 cognitively healthy elderly subjects (aged 60 years and older) in total distributed over the three trial sites, satisfying the statistical power requirements for different research questions, as described below. Paticipants will be recruited by local newspaper announcements and flyers.
For the investigation of the primary objective, a sample size of 160 subjects has been determined based on a power calculation (see “Statistics”). Assuming a dropout rate of about 15%, the recruitment of subjects will be distributed as follows: Mainz: N = 60, Rostock: N = 60, German Sport University Cologne/University Hospital Cologne: N = 72.
For the investigation of the predictive value of a preceding ACT for the transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA (secondary objective), all 72 subjects recruited at the German Sport University Cologne/University Hospital Cologne (who are also part of the investigation of the primary outcome) will undergo an extended experimental protocol, including ACT followed by the cognitive training. The sample size of 72 subjects has been determined based on a power calculation (see “Statistics”). Since, so far, no evidence has been published showing that physical activity may have an impact on transfer via mechanisms other than brain structure and function, it is assumed that the preceding physical training in subjects recruited in Cologne will not confound the investigation of the primary objective.
To minimize/avoid bias, the following control subjects will be included: 15 HOA will be enrolled in Cologne undergoing the standard experimental protocol without ACT to assure that the net effect of the cognitive training does not differ from the effects at the other trial sites. To control performance alterations in the neuropsychological examinations for retest effects, an additional sample of 30 HOA, generated in Mainz (N = 10), Rostock (N = 10), and German Sport University Cologne/University Hospital Cologne (N = 10), will undergo the repeated neuropsychological examinations without cognitive training.
Within each trial site, participants will be distributed randomly to experimental or control groups, stratified centrally by the trial coordinator at the trial site University Medical Center Mainz. The study code is centrally managed by this trial site. Participants will be enrolled by their respective trial site.

Inclusion criteria (for all trial sites)

The following inclusion criteria will be applied:
  • Age ≥ 60 years
  • Ability of subject to understand character and individual consequences of clinical trial
  • Signed and dated informed consent must be available before start of any procedures
  • Sufficient mobility and motivation to be able to participate in the examinations

Exclusion criteria (for all trial sites)

  • Incapability of giving consent
  • Current (or history of) psychiatric illness
  • Current (or history of) neurological or cerebrovascular illness, brain lesions
  • Current (or history of) cardiovascular disease (i.e., myocardial infarct, peripheral arterial disease)
  • Secondary disorders restricting individuals’ physical capacity (i.e., chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, rheumatism, osteoarthritis, bone fractures)
  • Current (or history of) cognitive illness
  • Diabetes types 1 and 2
  • Intake of medications that may influence cognitive performance
  • Insufficient German language skills
  • Participation in other clinical trials during the present clinical trial or within the last month
  • MRI contraindication (pacemaker, metal implants, tattoos, permanent-make-up, chochlear implant, medication pump, acupuncture needles)

Measures

Assessment of inclusion/exclusion criteria

  • Telephone screening
    Demographics, psychiatric screening (diagnostic expert system for psychiatric disorders – Stamm Screening Interview [56], International Diagnostic Checklists for ICD − 10 and DSM-IV [57]
  • Study information and informed consent
    General study information and informed consent, MRI information and informed consent, PET information and informed consent, genetics information and informed consent

Neuropsychological examination at baseline (NP I)

  • Intelligence
    Wortschatztest [58], Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligence Test for adults-revised – subtests: picture completion, similarities, block design, arithmetic [59]
  • Memory
    Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest [60], Wechsler-Memory Scale-revised – subtests: digit span, block span (forward and backward, respectively) [61], Cogpack – subtest: remembering [62]
  • Executive function
    Leistungsprüfsystem (comparable to the Raven Matrices): subtest 4 [63], Tower of London [64], Trail-Making Test B [65], Cogpack – subtest: reasoning [62].
  • Stimulus interference
    Computerized version of the Stroop Test
  • Information processing speed
    Trail-Making Test A [65], Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligence Test for adults-revised: digit-symbol substitution test [59]
  • Visual construction
    Ray-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test – copy [66].
  • Attention
    Test battery for attention performance – subtests: alertness, divided attention

Neuropsychological examination immediately after cognitive training (NP II)

In the second neuropsychological examination (NP II, post-training phase), the same test battery as in the baseline examination (NPI) will be applied. However, the tests measuring intelligence will only be applied once (at NP I). If available, we will apply parallel test versions to avoid retest effects.

Neuropsychological examination after 3 months (NP III)

In the third neuropsychological examination (NP III, follow-up), the same test battery as in NP II will be applied. If available, we will apply parallel test versions to avoid retest effects.

Neuropsychological training

  • Executive functions, memory, information processing speed
    Cogpack: Cogpack is a computerized cognitive training and testing program. The subtests comparisons, searching, logic, anagrams, complete a logical block, and remembering will be applied
  • Alertness
    Attention capacities will be trained using the test battery for attention performance, which permits us to assess/train a variety of attentional aspects. The subtests alertness and divided attention will be trained.
  • Working memory
    The computerized training software TATOOL [67] will be used to apply a working memory training designed in accordance to Batian et al. [68]

Physical activity examination

  • Actigraphy
    Objective physical activity, sleep/wake and energy expenditure measurement solution. The portable wristband (GeneActive, Kimbolton, UK) uses a three-axis accelerometer, a heat flux sensor, a galvanic skin-response sensor, a skin-temperature sensor, and a near-body ambient temperature sensor to capture data for 1 week
  • Global Physical Activity Questionaire
    The Global Physical Activity Questionaire covers several components of physical activity typical of an elderly population. The score takes into account self-reported occupational, household and leisure activities items over a 1-week period (World Health Organization (WHO), http://​www.​who.​int/​chp/​steps/​GPAQ/​en/​)

Fitness examinations

  • Cardiovascular fitness
    Cardiovascular fitness will be assessed using a modified, graded, exercise-testing protocol in the field. Endurance capacity will be estimated on the basis of walking/running speed, the corresponding concentration of blood lactate and the perceived exertion of each participant.
    Additionally, the maximum oxygen uptake (VO2max) and the peak oxygen uptake (VO2peak) will be estimated using the 6-minute Walking Test.
  • Motor fitness
    Gross motor fitness will be assessed using a test battery comprising the following basic motor skills:
    • Static and dynamic balance
    • Short Physical Performance Battery
    • Feet tapping
    • Hand tapping
    • Kasten-Bumerang Test
    • Movement-Coordination Test
    • Agility Test
  • Fine motor fitness will be assessed using the following subtests of the Vienna Test System:
    • Motor performance series
    • Sensomotor coodination
    • Spatial orientation
    • Reaction time
    • Response inhibition
    • Time/movement anticipation

Imaging data acquisition

  • Structural MRI
    DTI, T1-weighted structural MRI, FLAIR, high-resolution T2-weighted structural MRI
  • Functional MRI
    Resting-state MRI, task-related fMRI. For task-related fMRI, participants will perform three runs of the Hybrid Response Inhibition task [69]. Using identical visual stimulus material the Hybrid Response Inhibition task assesses three subcomponents of response inhibition: response interference, action withholding, and action cancelation (Fig. 3). Stimuli will be presented in the center of the screen. Participants will be asked to perform a button press according to the pointing direction of an arrow and to refrain from a button press whenever the ellipse surrounding the arrows turns blue (nogo−/stop trials). Event-related fMRI data acquisition will be performed using standard echo planar imaging sequences with whole-brain coverage and isotropic voxels. SPM 12 (http://​www.​fil.​ion.​ucl.​ac.​uk/​spm) will be used to conduct all image preprocessing and statistical analyses. In addition to standard general linear model analyses, a BOLD lateralization index [70] will be computed for each subcomponent separately. The lateralization index is hypothesized to mediate transfer capabilities in older adults and to be associated with the structural integrity of the corpus callosum.
  • PET
    The amyloid tracer [18F]Florbetaben will be applied via a venous cannula. Subjects will be instructed to void their bladder to allow rapid excretion of unbound radioactivity. Subjects will be placed on the scanner approximately 70 min after injection. At 80 min p.i. (post injection) two low-dose computer tomography scans will be acquired for position and attenuation correction. At 90 mins p.i., the PET acquisition will be initiated. PET data will be acquired for 20 min. After the scan the subject will be asked about their well-being and instructed to void their bladder to accelerate excretion of radioactivity. Subjects will be instructed to minimize contact with small children and pregnant women for 12 h after tracer injection. The entire examination, including preparation and scanning procedures, can be finished within 3–3.5 h.

Genetics

Two samples of 5 ml ethylenediamine tetraacetic acid blood will be collected from participants at the pre-training phase and at the post-training phase in Mainz and Rostock and at the pre-ACT-training phase, post-ACT-training phase, and post-cognitive-training phase in Cologne. Blood samples will be kept on ice for a maximum of 15 min and subsequently stored at − 80 °C in the recruiting centers. Frozen blood samples will be shipped on dry ice to the Laboratory of Molecular Genetics, Institute of Human Genetics, University Medical Center Mainz. There, deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) extraction from one of the frozen blood samples taken at the pre-training phase will be performed on a chemagic Magnetic Separation Module I using the chemagic DNA Blood Kit special for the automated isolation of DNA from 5 ml whole blood followed by quantification of DNA on a NanoDrop 2000UV-Vis spectrophotometer. Genotyping for the single nucleotide polymorphisms rs7412 and rs429358 in the apolipoprotein E gene, rs5882 and rs1800775 in the Cholesteryl Ester Transfer Protein gene, rs6265 in the BDNF gene, rs2765 in the Neurokinin.3 Receptor gene, rs6439886 in the Calsyntenin 2 gene, and rs17070145 in the Kidney and Brain Expressed Protein gene will be carried out from extracted genomic DNA samples using polymerase chain reaction followed by pyrosequencing on a Pyromark Q96 ID Instrument using PyroMark Gold Q96 SQA Reagents and the Pyromark Q96 ID software. The second blood sample taken at the pre-training phase and both blood samples taken at the post-training phase will be stored at − 80 °C for prospective analyses of training-induced epigenetic and transcriptome changes. Blood samples will be stored in secured rooms at the Institute of Human Genetics of the University Medical Center Mainz for a maximum of 60 years.

Statistics

Sample size

Primary outcome: the analyses will focus on training gain transfer (defined as a binary criterion for each subject) and simultaneously incorporate measures of corpus callosum integrity and bihemispheric processing. The sample size calculation was performed based on main effects analyses where the effects of both measures will be considered as main effects. Assuming a transfer rate of cognitive training gains of about 15% for participants with low corpus callosum integrity, and a transfer odds ratio of 3 for participants with high corpus callosum integrity, a sample size of 160 provides a power of 0.8 at a significance level of 0.05 for a Wald Test in a logistic regression model. Assuming a dropout rate of about 15%, a total of 192 participants need to be included.
Secondary outcome: for evaluating physical training as a potential modulator of cognitive training gain transfer, ACT-induced fitness changes will be analyzed with respect to changes of corpus callosum integrity, as well as with respect to effects on transfer. The former analysis corresponds to considering the correlation between fitness changes and integrity changes. For the latter, an interaction between corpus callosum integrity and fitness changes with respect to transfer will be considered in a logistic regression model. Such an interaction can be expressed as a correlation between fitness changes and the degree of transfer in a group with low/high corpus callosum integrity. Based on the results by Voss et al. [14], we assume a correlation of about 0.43 between both measures. At a significance level of 0.05, 30 individuals per (equally sized) integrity group are needed to detect such a correlation with a power of 0.8. As a result, ACT needs to be performed for 60 individuals in total. Assuming a dropout rate of about 15%, 72 subjects at the German Sport University Cologne will undergo the extended experimental protocol.

Definition and analysis of primary endpoint

The primary endpoint is defined as the dichotomous outcome training gain transfer (Yes/No). Successful transfer will be defined as: (1) Performance improvement in a logical reasoning training task from the second to the last training session, (2) performance improvement in an untrained fluid intelligence transfer task (Leistungsprüfsystem, subtest 4) from the pre-cognitive-training phase to the post-cognitive-training phase, and (3) maintenance of the performance in the transfer task from post-cognitive-training phase to follow-up. Transfer task performance alterations have to be numerically greater than the mere retest effect of an untrained control group.
The primary endpoint will be analyzed using a logistic regression with measures of corpus callosum integrity and bihemispheric processing as independent variables. As a primary analysis, the effects of both measures will be considered as main effects. In subsequent analyses, we will take into account that bihemispheric processing may not be necessary for all participants, whereby strong training performance might specifically indicate such participants. This will be incorporated into the analyses by interaction terms and by allowing for non-linear effects. The models will be adjusted for age. In the primary analysis subjects with missing values will be excluded. Sensitivity analysis will be performed to evaluate the impact of the missing values.
The primary population is the intention-to-treat population. As a sensitivity analysis, an as-treated analysis will be performed. To investigate the center effects, center number will be additionally incorporated into the models.

Analysis of secondary endpoints

For evaluating physical training as potential modulator of cognitive training gain transfer, correlations between fitness changes and integrity changes of the corpus callosum will be applied. Moreover, an interaction between corpus callosum integrity and fitness changes with respect to transfer will be considered in a logistic regression model. Such an interaction can be expressed as a correlation between fitness changes and the degree of transfer in a group with low/high corpus callosum integrity.
To evaluate the predictive value of brain vascular lesion, cortical amyloid load, higher Default Mode Network activity, and genetics for transfer of cognitive training gains in HOA, a logistic regression model with transfer as the dependent variable is used. All effects of the potential predictors are included as main effects. Additionally, to adjust for ACT, ACT and all interactions with ACT are included in the model as independent covariates.
To determine the impact of ACT on the structural integrity of the corpus callosum, Default Mode Network activity, BDNF, motor fitness, and VO2peak/VO2max, logistic regression models for these variables are fitted. ACT will be included as an independent variable. Additionally, the model will be adjusted for all other variables and the interactions with ACT. For example, the model for structural integrity of the corpus callosum will include ACT, Default Mode Network activity, BDNF, motor fitness, VO2peak/VO2max and all interactions with ACT as independent covariates.
Similar models will be fitted to examine the association between baseline physical activity and the structural integrity of the corpus callosum, Default Mode Network activity, and BDNF. Here, baseline physical activity and all possible interactions with baseline physical activity will be additionally included as independent covariates.
To build a robust individual index of transfer likelihood based on all potential predictors and interactions, a multimodal, high-dimensional dataset needs to be analyzed. For this purpose a regularized logistic regression for transfer will be performed including all potential predictors and relevant interactions. Specifically, to automatically select relevant covariates, a componentwise likelihood boosting approach will be used. To better detect potentially complex patterns in the multimodal data that are linked to transfer, a deep learning approach will be employed; specifically, deep Boltzmann machines [71]. This specific approach will potentially allow for the detection of non-linear relations and interactions while still providing type 1 error control, and results in an integrated statistical model for transfer prediction.

Data management

Responsibilities

The data management in each trial center during the trial will be conducted by one study member of the respective trial site (specified before the start of data collection). The trial coordinator is authorized to contact trial centers to monitor the data management.

Data collection

An electronic Case Report Form (eCRF) will be provided for each subject. All trial data will be documented in the subject’s source data and in the eCRF. The principal investigators of the trial sites or designated representatives are responsible for ensuring that all sections of the eCRFs of their trial sites are correctly completed and that entries can be verified against source data. All changes in an eCRF entry will be tracked. The investigator, or a designated representative, should complete the eCRF pages as soon as possible after the information is collected. Any outstanding entries must be completed immediately after the final examination. An explanation should be given for any missing data.

Data handling

After completion of data entry all data will be collected at the University Medical Center Mainz. The access for data entry will be blocked and checks for plausibility, consistency, and completeness of the data will be performed. Based on these checks, queries will be produced. Any missing data or inconsistencies will be reported back to the respective site and clarified by the responsible investigator. If no further corrections are to be made in the database it will be declared closed and used for statistical analysis. The data checks will be done by the trial coordinator or a designated representative at the University Medical Center Mainz.

Assessment of safety

Assessment of adverse events (AEs) by investigator

Subjects must be carefully monitored for AEs by the investigator. The intensity of the AEs and the causal relation to trial medication and/or procedures are to be assessed.
The intensity of an AE will be assessed by the investigator as follows:
  • Mild: temporary event which is tolerated well by the subject and does not interfere with normal daily activities
  • Moderate: event which results in discomfort for the subject and impairs their normal activity
  • Severe: event which results in substantial impairment of normal activities of the subject
The assessment of the relationship of an AE to trial procedures is a clinical decision based on all available information at the time of the completion of the eCRF. The investigator will evaluate the causal relationship of each adverse event with the trial procedures according to modified criteria of the WHO 1991.

Documentation of AEs and follow-up

All AEs (whether serious (SAE) or non-serious) reported by the subject or detected by the investigator will be documented on the “Adverse Event Page” of the eCRF. If an AE is serious, the investigator must complete, in addition to the “Adverse Event Page”, a “Serious Adverse Event Form” at the time the SAE is detected. SAEs are required to be reported by the investigator to the sponsor immediately (i.e., no more than 24 h after learning of the event).
All subjects who experience AEs, whether considered associated with the use of the investigational products or not, must be monitored to determine the outcome. The clinical course of the AE will be followed up according to accepted standards of medical practice, even after the end of the period of observation, until a satisfactory explanation is found or the investigator considers it medically justifiable to terminate follow-up, but no longer than 90 days after the end of the trial.

Good Clinical Practice

The procedures set out in this trial protocol, pertaining to the conduct, evaluation, and documentation of this trial, are designed to ensure that all persons involved in the trial abide by the quality standards of Good Clinical Practice and the ethical principles described in the Declaration of Helsinki. The trial will be carried out in accordance with all applicable local legal and regulatory requirements.

Patient information and informed consent

Before being enrolled into the clinical trial, the subject must consent to participate after being fully informed about the nature, scope, and possible consequences of the clinical trial. The documents must be in a language understandable to the subject and must specify who informed the subject. A copy of the signed informed consent document must be given to the subject. The investigator will retain the original signed consent document. The investigator will not undertake any measures specifically required only for the clinical trial until valid consent has been obtained. After reading the informed consent document, the subject must give consent in writing. The subject’s consent must be confirmed by the personally dated signature of the subject and by the personally dated signature of the person conducting the informed consent discussions.

Confidentiality

The name of the subject and other confidential information are subject to medical professional secrecy and the regulations of the German Data Protection Act (Bundesdatenschutzgesetz). The name of the subjects and other confidential information will not be supplied to the sponsor. During the clinical trial, subjects will be identified solely by means of an individual identification code (e.g., subject number, randomization number). Trial findings stored on a computer will be stored in accordance with local data protection law and will be handled in strictest confidence. For protection of these data, organizational procedures are implemented to prevent distribution of data to unauthorized persons. The appropriate regulations of data legislation will be fulfilled in their entirety. The investigator will maintain a personal subject identification list (subject numbers with the corresponding subject names) to enable records to be identified.

Approval of trial protocol

The study protocol was approved by the local Ethics Committees of all three trial sites (Mainz: Ethics Commission of the Landesärztekammer Rheinland-Pfalz; Cologne: Ethics Commission of Cologne University’s Faculty of Medicine; Rostock: Ethics Commission of the Rostock University’s Faculty of Medicine). The reference number of the ethics approval of the main trial site Mainz is 837.385.15 (10153). The study was registered at the German Clinical Trials Resgister (ID: DRKS00013077).

Publication policy

Any publication of the results, either in part or in total (articles in journals or newspapers, oral presentation, etc.) by the investigators, their representatives, or by the sponsor, will require the approval of all principal investigators of the trial (Prof. Dr. Andreas Fellgiebel, Prof. Dr. Oliver Tüscher, Dr. Andreas Mierau, Prof. Dr. Alexander Drzezga, Prof. Dr. Stefan Teipel, Prof. Dr. Harald Binder). It is planned to publish the results of the trial as original articles in appropriate journals as well as to present the results at congresses.

Discussion

Knowledge on the mechanisms and possibly modifiable modulators of transfer of cognitive training gains is urgently needed to design future intervention programs for the promotion of cognitive health including lifelong learning in aging. Today, neuroimaging techniques allow a reliable and valid investigation of neuronal mechanisms of transfer based on structural and functional brain network surrogates directly in humans. For this important purpose, the AgeGain Consortium combines all relevant multidisciplinary expertise, including cognitive training and transfer assessments, conduction of multimodal neuroimaging, physical training, and the assessment of subclinical brain pathology that occurs frequently and is known to significantly contribute to incident decline of cognitive function in HOA. As shown by recent publications [11, 17, 55, 72, 73], the individual research groups assembled in AgeGain are already focusing on these research questions and the first successful European collaborations have been established. The formation of the consortium will enable the development of a strong partnership of basic clinical research in HOA.
The AgeGain study should provide important information for the determination of transfer likelihood in older people, and thus for the identification of HOA, who will most benefit from cognitive training. Findings of this trial should contribute to a better understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of transfer in aging and will help determining the impact of physical activity and sport as well as of pathological factors (such as cerebrovascular disease and amyloid load) on transfer capability. Specifically, the trial will contribute to an increased understanding of the association of corpus callosum integrity as a structural measure and bihemispheric cooperation as a functional measure (as well as their interaction) with transfer capabilities in older adults. Beyond that, the trial should add important information about the importance of HAROLD for learning in old age. The study results should have a strong impact on future clinical, basic, and healthcare research evaluating lifestyle and training strategies to maintain successful and lifelong learning. A limitation of the study is that it is restricted to the investigation of neurobiological mechanisms of transfer on a macrostructural level. Thus, it will not provide information about microstructural neurobiological mechanisms of transfer.

Trial status

The study is currently in the recruitment phase. Recruitment began in July 2016.
Protocol version: 1.3 (06–21-2016).

Acknowledgements

AgeGain study group: University Medical Center Mainz: Andreas Fellgiebel, Oliver Tüscher, Bernhard Baier, Dominik Wolf, Bianca Kollmann, Florian Fischer, Alexandra Sebastian; German Sport University Cologne: Heiko Strüder, Andreas Mierau, Kristel Knaepen, David Riedel; University Clinic Cologne: Alexander Drzezga; University Medical Center Rostock: Stefan Teipel, Katharina Brüggen, Judith Henf, Esther Lau; University of Freiburg: Harald Binder.
The study was granted ethical approval by the local Ethics Commitees of all trial sites (Mainz: Ethics Commission of the Landesärztekammer Rheinland-Pfalz; Cologne: Ethics Commission of (1) Cologne University’s Faculty of Medicine and (2) German Sport University; Rostock: Ethics Commission of the Rostock University’s Faculty of Medicine). Reference number of the ethics approval of the main trial site Mainz is 837.385.15 (10153). Subjects gave written consent to participate in the trial.

Funding

The AgeGain study is funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) (grant number: 01GQ1425A). The funding body has no influence on the trial whatsoever.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Depp CA, Jeste DV. Definitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:6–20.CrossRefPubMed Depp CA, Jeste DV. Definitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:6–20.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Reichstadt J, Depp CA, Palinkas LA, Jeste DV. Building blocks of successful aging: a focus group study of older adults’ perceived contributors to successful aging. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15:194–201.CrossRefPubMed Reichstadt J, Depp CA, Palinkas LA, Jeste DV. Building blocks of successful aging: a focus group study of older adults’ perceived contributors to successful aging. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15:194–201.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bach J, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:179.CrossRefPubMed Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bach J, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:179.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Smith DM, Tennstedt SL. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271–81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Smith DM, Tennstedt SL. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271–81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science. 2008;320:1510–2.CrossRefPubMed Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science. 2008;320:1510–2.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Dahlin E, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Neely AS. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol Aging. 2008;23:720.CrossRefPubMed Dahlin E, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Neely AS. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol Aging. 2008;23:720.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Meyers RS, Roenker DR, Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology. 2002;48:329–340. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Meyers RS, Roenker DR, Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology. 2002;48:329–340.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Derwinger A, Neely AS, Persson M, Hill RD, Bäckman L. Remembering numbers in old age: mnemonic training versus self-generated strategy training. Aging Neuropsychol Cognit. 2003;10:202–14.CrossRef Derwinger A, Neely AS, Persson M, Hill RD, Bäckman L. Remembering numbers in old age: mnemonic training versus self-generated strategy training. Aging Neuropsychol Cognit. 2003;10:202–14.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmiedek F, Lövdén M, Lindenberger U. Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: findings from the COGITO study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:27. Schmiedek F, Lövdén M, Lindenberger U. Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: findings from the COGITO study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:27.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Blume BD, Ford JK, Baldwin TT, Huang JL. Transfer of training: a meta-analytic review. J Manag. 2010;36:1065–105. Blume BD, Ford JK, Baldwin TT, Huang JL. Transfer of training: a meta-analytic review. J Manag. 2010;36:1065–105.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolf D, Fischer FU, Fesenbeckh J, Yakushev I, Lelieveld IM, Scheurich A, Schermuly I, Zschutschke L, Fellgiebel A. Structural integrity of the corpus callosum predicts long-term transfer of fluid intelligence-related training gains in normal aging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:309–18.CrossRefPubMed Wolf D, Fischer FU, Fesenbeckh J, Yakushev I, Lelieveld IM, Scheurich A, Schermuly I, Zschutschke L, Fellgiebel A. Structural integrity of the corpus callosum predicts long-term transfer of fluid intelligence-related training gains in normal aging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:309–18.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Daselaar SM, Cabeza R. Age-related changes in hemispheric organization. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park DC, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 325–53. Daselaar SM, Cabeza R. Age-related changes in hemispheric organization. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park DC, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 325–53.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulte T, Sullivan EV, Müller-Oehring E, Adalsteinsson E, Pfefferbaum A. Corpus callosal microstructural integrity influences interhemispheric processing: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1384–92.CrossRefPubMed Schulte T, Sullivan EV, Müller-Oehring E, Adalsteinsson E, Pfefferbaum A. Corpus callosal microstructural integrity influences interhemispheric processing: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1384–92.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, Szabo AN, Mailey EL, Wojcicki TR, White SM. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2972–85.CrossRefPubMed Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, Szabo AN, Mailey EL, Wojcicki TR, White SM. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2972–85.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan L, Walther K, Bendlin BB, Lue L-F, Walker DG, Glisky EL. Age-related differences in white matter integrity and cognitive function are related to APOE status. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1565–77.CrossRefPubMed Ryan L, Walther K, Bendlin BB, Lue L-F, Walker DG, Glisky EL. Age-related differences in white matter integrity and cognitive function are related to APOE status. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1565–77.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Lustig C, Shah P, Seidler R, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:504–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lustig C, Shah P, Seidler R, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:504–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Sebastian A, Baldermann C, Feige B, Katzev M, Scheller E, Hellwig B, Lieb K, Weiller C, Tüscher O, Klöppel S. Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:2183–93.CrossRefPubMed Sebastian A, Baldermann C, Feige B, Katzev M, Scheller E, Hellwig B, Lieb K, Weiller C, Tüscher O, Klöppel S. Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:2183–93.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:125–30.CrossRefPubMed Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:125–30.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson K, Raji C, Lopez O, Becker J, Rosano C, Newman A, Gach H, Thompson P, Ho A, Kuller L. Physical activity predicts gray matter volume in late adulthood The Cardiovascular Health Study. Neurology. 2010;75:1415–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erickson K, Raji C, Lopez O, Becker J, Rosano C, Newman A, Gach H, Thompson P, Ho A, Kuller L. Physical activity predicts gray matter volume in late adulthood The Cardiovascular Health Study. Neurology. 2010;75:1415–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, Elavsky S, Marquez DX, Hu L, Kramer AF. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2006;61:1166–70.CrossRef Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, Elavsky S, Marquez DX, Hu L, Kramer AF. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2006;61:1166–70.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Elavsky S. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:3316–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Elavsky S. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:3316–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosano C, Venkatraman VK, Guralnik J, Newman AB, Glynn NW, Launer L, Taylor CA, Williamson J, Studenski S, Pahor M. Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2010;65:glq038. Rosano C, Venkatraman VK, Guralnik J, Newman AB, Glynn NW, Launer L, Taylor CA, Williamson J, Studenski S, Pahor M. Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2010;65:glq038.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus. 2009;19:1030–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus. 2009;19:1030–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Pereira AC, Huddleston DE, Brickman AM, Sosunov AA, Hen R, McKhann GM, Sloan R, Gage FH, Brown TR, Small SA. An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104:5638–43.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pereira AC, Huddleston DE, Brickman AM, Sosunov AA, Hen R, McKhann GM, Sloan R, Gage FH, Brown TR, Small SA. An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104:5638–43.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Burdette JH, Laurienti PJ, Espeland MA, Morgan A, Telesford Q, Vechlekar CD, Hayasaka S, Jennings JM, Katula JA, Kraft RA. Using network science to evaluate exercise-associated brain changes in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:23. Burdette JH, Laurienti PJ, Espeland MA, Morgan A, Telesford Q, Vechlekar CD, Hayasaka S, Jennings JM, Katula JA, Kraft RA. Using network science to evaluate exercise-associated brain changes in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:23.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:295–301.CrossRefPubMed Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:295–301.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Rojas Vega S, Hollmann W, Strüder H. Influences of exercise and training on the circulating concentration of prolactin in humans. J Neuroendocrinol. 2012;24:395–402.CrossRefPubMed Rojas Vega S, Hollmann W, Strüder H. Influences of exercise and training on the circulating concentration of prolactin in humans. J Neuroendocrinol. 2012;24:395–402.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Vega SR, Abel T, Lindschulten R, Hollmann W, Bloch W, Strüder H. Impact of exercise on neuroplasticity-related proteins in spinal cord injured humans. Neuroscience. 2008;153:1064–70.CrossRef Vega SR, Abel T, Lindschulten R, Hollmann W, Bloch W, Strüder H. Impact of exercise on neuroplasticity-related proteins in spinal cord injured humans. Neuroscience. 2008;153:1064–70.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Vega SR, Kleinert J, Sulprizio M, Hollmann W, Bloch W, Strüder HK. Responses of serum neurotrophic factors to exercise in pregnant and postpartum women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:220–7.CrossRefPubMed Vega SR, Kleinert J, Sulprizio M, Hollmann W, Bloch W, Strüder HK. Responses of serum neurotrophic factors to exercise in pregnant and postpartum women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:220–7.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Vega SR, Strüder HK, Wahrmann BV, Schmidt A, Bloch W, Hollmann W. Acute BDNF and cortisol response to low intensity exercise and following ramp incremental exercise to exhaustion in humans. Brain Res. 2006;1121:59–65.CrossRef Vega SR, Strüder HK, Wahrmann BV, Schmidt A, Bloch W, Hollmann W. Acute BDNF and cortisol response to low intensity exercise and following ramp incremental exercise to exhaustion in humans. Brain Res. 2006;1121:59–65.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Rasmussen P, Brassard P, Adser H, Pedersen MV, Leick L, Hart E, Secher NH, Pedersen BK, Pilegaard H. Evidence for a release of brain-derived neurotrophic factor from the brain during exercise. Exp Physiol. 2009;94:1062–9.CrossRefPubMed Rasmussen P, Brassard P, Adser H, Pedersen MV, Leick L, Hart E, Secher NH, Pedersen BK, Pilegaard H. Evidence for a release of brain-derived neurotrophic factor from the brain during exercise. Exp Physiol. 2009;94:1062–9.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Zoladz J, Pilc A, Majerczak J, Grandys M, Zapart-Bukowska J, Duda K. Endurance training increases plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor concentration in young healthy men. J Physiol Pharmacol. 2008;59:119–32.PubMed Zoladz J, Pilc A, Majerczak J, Grandys M, Zapart-Bukowska J, Duda K. Endurance training increases plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor concentration in young healthy men. J Physiol Pharmacol. 2008;59:119–32.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011:201015950. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011:201015950.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson NF, Kim C, Clasey JL, Bailey A, Gold BT. Cardiorespiratory fitness is positively correlated with cerebral white matter integrity in healthy seniors. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1514–23.CrossRefPubMed Johnson NF, Kim C, Clasey JL, Bailey A, Gold BT. Cardiorespiratory fitness is positively correlated with cerebral white matter integrity in healthy seniors. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1514–23.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Voss MW, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Malkowski E, Alves H, Kim JS, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR. Functional connectivity: a source of variance in the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognition? Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1394–406.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Voss MW, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Malkowski E, Alves H, Kim JS, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR. Functional connectivity: a source of variance in the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognition? Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1394–406.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170:170–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170:170–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Cassilhas RC, Viana VA, Grassmann V, Santos RT, Santos RF, Tufik S, Mello MT. The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1401.CrossRefPubMed Cassilhas RC, Viana VA, Grassmann V, Santos RT, Santos RF, Tufik S, Mello MT. The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1401.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:1690–8.CrossRefPubMed Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:1690–8.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Hötting K, Reich B, Holzschneider K, Kauschke K, Schmidt T, Reer R, Braumann K-M, Röder B. Differential cognitive effects of cycling versus stretching/coordination training in middle-aged adults. Health Psychol. 2012;31:145.CrossRefPubMed Hötting K, Reich B, Holzschneider K, Kauschke K, Schmidt T, Reer R, Braumann K-M, Röder B. Differential cognitive effects of cycling versus stretching/coordination training in middle-aged adults. Health Psychol. 2012;31:145.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Voelcker-Rehage C, Godde B, Staudinger UM. Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Voelcker-Rehage C, Godde B, Staudinger UM. Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Voelcker-Rehage C, Niemann C. Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:2268–95.CrossRefPubMed Voelcker-Rehage C, Niemann C. Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:2268–95.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Serrien DJ, Ivry RB, Swinnen SP. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:160–6.CrossRefPubMed Serrien DJ, Ivry RB, Swinnen SP. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:160–6.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Takeuchi N, Izumi S-I. Maladaptive plasticity for motor recovery after stroke: mechanisms and approaches. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:1–9. Takeuchi N, Izumi S-I. Maladaptive plasticity for motor recovery after stroke: mechanisms and approaches. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:1–9.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Sisti HM, Geurts M, Gooijers J, Heitger MH, Caeyenberghs K, Beets IA, Serbruyns L, Leemans A, Swinnen SP. Microstructural organization of corpus callosum projections to prefrontal cortex predicts bimanual motor learning. Learn Mem. 2012;19:351–7.CrossRefPubMed Sisti HM, Geurts M, Gooijers J, Heitger MH, Caeyenberghs K, Beets IA, Serbruyns L, Leemans A, Swinnen SP. Microstructural organization of corpus callosum projections to prefrontal cortex predicts bimanual motor learning. Learn Mem. 2012;19:351–7.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Jack CR, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Therneau TM, Knopman DS, Lowe V, Vemuri P, Mielke MM, Roberts RO, Machulda MM. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence of cerebral β-amyloidosis, tauopathy, and neurodegeneration in cognitively unimpaired individuals aged 50–95 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:435–44.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Jack CR, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Therneau TM, Knopman DS, Lowe V, Vemuri P, Mielke MM, Roberts RO, Machulda MM. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence of cerebral β-amyloidosis, tauopathy, and neurodegeneration in cognitively unimpaired individuals aged 50–95 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:435–44.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Bennett IJ, Madden DJ. Disconnected aging: cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience. 2014;276:187–205.CrossRefPubMed Bennett IJ, Madden DJ. Disconnected aging: cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience. 2014;276:187–205.CrossRefPubMed
47.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Ondrejcak T, Klyubin I, Hu N-W, Barry AE, Cullen WK, Rowan MJ. Alzheimer’s disease amyloid β-protein and synaptic function. NeuroMolecular Med. 2010;12:13–26.CrossRefPubMed Ondrejcak T, Klyubin I, Hu N-W, Barry AE, Cullen WK, Rowan MJ. Alzheimer’s disease amyloid β-protein and synaptic function. NeuroMolecular Med. 2010;12:13–26.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Pike KE, Savage G, Villemagne VL, Ng S, Moss SA, Maruff P, Mathis CA, Klunk WE, Masters CL, Rowe CC. β-amyloid imaging and memory in non-demented individuals: evidence for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2007;130:2837–44.CrossRefPubMed Pike KE, Savage G, Villemagne VL, Ng S, Moss SA, Maruff P, Mathis CA, Klunk WE, Masters CL, Rowe CC. β-amyloid imaging and memory in non-demented individuals: evidence for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2007;130:2837–44.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Jack Jr CR, Lowe VJ, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Senjem ML, Knopman DS, Shiung MM, Gunter JL, Boeve BF, Kemp BJ. Serial PIB and MRI in normal, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: implications for sequence of pathological events in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2009;132:1355–65.CrossRef Jack Jr CR, Lowe VJ, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Senjem ML, Knopman DS, Shiung MM, Gunter JL, Boeve BF, Kemp BJ. Serial PIB and MRI in normal, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: implications for sequence of pathological events in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2009;132:1355–65.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Hedden T, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Younger AP, Schultz AP, Becker JA, Buckner RL, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, Rentz DM. Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci. 2012;32:16233–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hedden T, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Younger AP, Schultz AP, Becker JA, Buckner RL, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, Rentz DM. Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci. 2012;32:16233–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, Scheurich A, Feyers D, Collette F, Chételat G. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. NeuroImage. 2012;63:713–22.CrossRefPubMed Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, Scheurich A, Feyers D, Collette F, Chételat G. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. NeuroImage. 2012;63:713–22.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Mevel K, Landeau B, Fouquet M, La Joie R, Villain N, Mézenge F, Perrotin A, Eustache F, Desgranges B, Chételat G. Age effect on the default mode network, inner thoughts, and cognitive abilities. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:1292–301.CrossRefPubMed Mevel K, Landeau B, Fouquet M, La Joie R, Villain N, Mézenge F, Perrotin A, Eustache F, Desgranges B, Chételat G. Age effect on the default mode network, inner thoughts, and cognitive abilities. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:1292–301.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;18:gbq035. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;18:gbq035.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Teipel SJ, Bokde AL, Meindl T, Amaro E, Soldner J, Reiser MF, Herpertz SC, Möller H-J, Hampel H. White matter microstructure underlying default mode network connectivity in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2010;49:2021–32.CrossRefPubMed Teipel SJ, Bokde AL, Meindl T, Amaro E, Soldner J, Reiser MF, Herpertz SC, Möller H-J, Hampel H. White matter microstructure underlying default mode network connectivity in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2010;49:2021–32.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittchen H, Pfister H. DIA-X Interviews (M-CIDI). Frankfurt: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997. Wittchen H, Pfister H. DIA-X Interviews (M-CIDI). Frankfurt: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Hiller W, Zaudig M, Mombour W. Internationale Diagnosen-Checklisten für DSM-IV und ICD-10; IDCL. Hogrefe: Verlag für Psychologie; 1997. Hiller W, Zaudig M, Mombour W. Internationale Diagnosen-Checklisten für DSM-IV und ICD-10; IDCL. Hogrefe: Verlag für Psychologie; 1997.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidt K-H, Metzler P. Wortschatztest. Weinheim: WST. Beltz; 1992. Schmidt K-H, Metzler P. Wortschatztest. Weinheim: WST. Beltz; 1992.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Tewes U. Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene-Revision (HAWIE-R). Bern: Huber; 1991. Tewes U. Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene-Revision (HAWIE-R). Bern: Huber; 1991.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S. VLMT: Verbaler Lern-und Merkfähigkeitstest. Göttingen: Beltz Test; 2001. Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S. VLMT: Verbaler Lern-und Merkfähigkeitstest. Göttingen: Beltz Test; 2001.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1987. Wechsler D. WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1987.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Marker K. Handbuch zum Programmpaket COGPACK, Version 7.6. Ladenburg: Marker Software; 2001. Marker K. Handbuch zum Programmpaket COGPACK, Version 7.6. Ladenburg: Marker Software; 2001.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Horn W. Achievement Measure System (Leistungs-Pruef-System/LPS). Goettingen: Hogrefe; 1962. Horn W. Achievement Measure System (Leistungs-Pruef-System/LPS). Goettingen: Hogrefe; 1962.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1982;298:199–209.CrossRefPubMed Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1982;298:199–209.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Tucson: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992. Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Tucson: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992. Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat von Bastian CC, Locher A, Ruflin M. Tatool: a Java-based open-source programming framework for psychological studies. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45:108–15.CrossRefPubMed von Bastian CC, Locher A, Ruflin M. Tatool: a Java-based open-source programming framework for psychological studies. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45:108–15.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat von Bastian CC, Langer N, Jäncke L, Oberauer K. Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Mem Cogn 2013, 41:611–624. von Bastian CC, Langer N, Jäncke L, Oberauer K. Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Mem Cogn 2013, 41:611–624.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Sebastian A, Pohl M, Klöppel S, Feige B, Lange T, Stahl C, Voss A, Klauer K, Lieb K, Tüscher O. Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition. NeuroImage. 2013;64:601–15.CrossRefPubMed Sebastian A, Pohl M, Klöppel S, Feige B, Lange T, Stahl C, Voss A, Klauer K, Lieb K, Tüscher O. Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition. NeuroImage. 2013;64:601–15.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilke M, Lidzba K. LI-tool: a new toolbox to assess lateralization in functional MR-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;163:128–36.CrossRefPubMed Wilke M, Lidzba K. LI-tool: a new toolbox to assess lateralization in functional MR-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;163:128–36.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Hess M, Lenz S, Blätte TJ, Bullinger L, Binder H. Partitioned learning of deep Boltzmann machines for SNP data. Bioinformatics. 2017;33:3173–80.CrossRefPubMed Hess M, Lenz S, Blätte TJ, Bullinger L, Binder H. Partitioned learning of deep Boltzmann machines for SNP data. Bioinformatics. 2017;33:3173–80.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Drzezga A, Becker JA, Van Dijk KR, Sreenivasan A, Talukdar T, Sullivan C, Schultz AP, Sepulcre J, Putcha D, Greve D. Neuronal dysfunction and disconnection of cortical hubs in non-demented subjects with elevated amyloid burden. Brain. 2011;134:1635–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Drzezga A, Becker JA, Van Dijk KR, Sreenivasan A, Talukdar T, Sullivan C, Schultz AP, Sepulcre J, Putcha D, Greve D. Neuronal dysfunction and disconnection of cortical hubs in non-demented subjects with elevated amyloid burden. Brain. 2011;134:1635–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Boecker H, Hillman CH, Scheef L, Strüder HK. Functional neuroimaging in exercise and sport sciences. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media; 2012. Boecker H, Hillman CH, Scheef L, Strüder HK. Functional neuroimaging in exercise and sport sciences. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media; 2012.
Metadaten
Titel
Mechanisms and modulators of cognitive training gain transfer in cognitively healthy aging: study protocol of the AgeGain study
verfasst von
Dominik Wolf
Oliver Tüscher
Stefan Teipel
Andreas Mierau
Heiko Strüder
Alexander Drzezga
Bernhard Baier
Harald Binder
Andreas Fellgiebel
the German AgeGain study group
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Trials / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1745-6215
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-018-2688-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

Trials 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe