Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research article

Mental health and cerebellar volume during adolescence in very-low-birth-weight infants: a longitudinal study

Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Preterm birth at very low birth weight (VLBW) poses a risk for cerebellar abnormalities and increased psychiatric morbidity compared with reference populations. We aimed to study cerebellar volumes (grey and white matter; GM, WM) and mental health in VLBW individuals and controls at 15 and 19 years of age, as well as changes between the two time points.

Methods

Forty VLBW (≤1500 g) and 56 control adolescents were included in the study at 15 years of age, and 44 VLBW and 60 control adolescents at 19 years of age. We had longitudinal data for 30 VLBW participants and for 37 controls. Clinical diagnoses were assessed following the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children (KSADS). Psychiatric symptoms and function were further investigated with the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA), ADHD Rating Scale-IV and the children’s global assessment scale (CGAS). An automatic segmentation of cerebellar GM and WM volumes was performed in FreeSurfer. The MRI scans were obtained on the same 1.5T scanner at both ages.

Results

The VLBW group had higher rates of psychiatric disorders at both ages. Cerebellar growth trajectories did not differ between VLBW adolescents and controls, regardless of psychiatric status. However, VLBW adolescents who had a psychiatric diagnosis at both ages or developed a psychiatric disorder from 15 to 19 years had maintained smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes than controls and also smaller volumes than VLWB adolescents who were or became healthy in this period. Moreover, there were no differences in cerebellar WM and GM volumes between controls and those VLBW who were healthy or became healthy. In the VLBW group, cerebellar WM and GM volumes correlated positively with psycho-social function at both 15 and 19 years of age, and smaller GM volumes were associated with inattention at 15 years.

Conclusions

Smaller cerebellar volume in adolescents born very preterm and with VLBW may be a biomarker of increased risk of psychiatric problems in young adulthood.

Background

Over the years, very preterm born children (<32 weeks gestation) have better survival rates [1] and improved outcome [2]. It is, however, of concern that an increased risk of psychiatric problems has been reported in preterm-born individuals (<37 weeks gestation), especially anxiety symptoms and disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum traits and disorders (ASD) [35].
The cerebellum is of particular interest in the preterm born due to its extensive development during the third trimester of gestation. Indeed, during this period it surpasses the growth rate of the cerebral hemispheres [6]. For the very preterm born infant, the extensive development of the cerebellum takes place in an extra uterine environment, where respiratory problems, infections and nutritional challenges may influence cerebellar development. Cerebellar injuries (hemorrhage, infarction) and mal/underdevelopment following premature birth occur more frequently than previously thought [7, 8]. It has been proposed that cerebellar involvement may play a central role in cognitive, mental health, and socialization deficits found later in life in this population [9, 10]. In the general population, the cerebellum has been associated with psychiatric problems such us mood disorders, anxiety problems, schizophrenia, ASD and attention problems [11]. The underlying pathophysiology still remains unknown. However, it has been proposed that the cerebellum might serve as modulatory [1214] and timing station [1517] for integrating [18] brain processes, due to its extensive connections with the whole brain [14, 1922]. Projections from the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex constitute the cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) pathway [23, 24], and early disruption of the cerebellar circuitry development has been positively correlated with ASD, attention deficit and emotional problems [25]. Injury to the immature cerebellum could affect neurologic function through mechanisms that interfere with later development of remote regions of the cerebral cortex [26].
Some studies have shown cerebellar abnormalities to be associated with psychiatric symptoms in preterm children. In a retrospective, case–control study, preterm children who had perinatal cerebellar hemorrhage presented a higher prevalence of deficits in cognition, communication, and social and behavioral function at 2–3 years of age than preterm peers without cerebellar pathology [10]. In another MRI study with very preterm participants, total cerebellar volume reduction from adolescence to adulthood was associated with having more psychiatric symptoms [27]. However, these studies used questionnaires to assess mental health problems and did not differentiate between cerebellar gray (GM) and white matter (WM).
Our research group has studied preterm born adolescents with very low birth weight (VLBW ≤ 1500 g) and controls during adolescence. At 15 years of age, the VLBW children had smaller cerebellar WM volumes compared with controls [28], and they had increased rates of psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses assessed with questionnaires and clinical interview [3, 4]. At 19 years of age, the VLBW group still had smaller cerebellar WM volumes than term-born peers [29] and more psychiatric problems [5]. During adolescence they also displayed a trend towards increasing psychiatric morbidity [5]. However, cerebellar growth rate did not differ from controls [29].
Based on these findings, we aimed to study the relationship between cerebellar volumes and psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses at 15 and 19 years of age. Our main hypothesis was that reduced cerebellar volumes would be associated with higher rates of psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses at both 15 and 19 years. Furthermore, we hypothesized that small cerebellar volume was associated with increased risk of developing psychiatric problems during adolescence.
In this article, we report an association between persistent smaller cerebellar volumes and psychiatric symptoms during adolescence in children born preterm and with VLBW.

Method

Participants

We studied, from 15 to 19 years of age, a hospital based cohort of VLBW infants who were admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit at the Trondheim University Hospital (Norway) in 1986–1988 and an age-matched group of controls recruited among term-born children from the same geographical area with birth weight ≥10th percentile for gestational age [28] (Fig. 1). For the present study, 81 VLBW adolescents and 110 controls were invited at the age of 15. Of them, 55 VLBW and 65 control participants underwent MRI examination and psychiatric assessment. At the age of 19, 55 VLBW adolescents and 81 controls were invited. Of them, 50 VLBW and 66 control participants underwent MRI examination and psychiatric assessment. We included subjects who had valid MRI evaluations at least at one of the measuring points. Images of some participants were discarded from the MRI assessment due to dental brace artifacts and poor MRI quality due to movement. In total, 40 VLBW adolescents and 56 controls were included at 15 years, and 44 VLBW adolescents and 60 controls at the age of 19. There were, at both 15 and 19 years of age, a higher number of participants with psychiatric assessment than MRI scans. Thus, some of the participants had longitudinal psychiatric data, even though they did not have longitudinal MRI data. This enabled us to study diagnostic change in those participants with just one MRI scan (See Fig. 1 for details).
There were no significant differences between participants and non-participants with regard to maternal age at time of birth, birth weight, and gestational age.
Further details of the study population and design are given in previous publications [35, 28, 29].
The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics approved the study protocol (project number: 78-00, May 2000 and 4.2005.2605) and the Data Inspectorate assigned the license for keeping a data register with personal information. Written informed consent was obtained from both adolescents and parents at the 15 years assessment, and from the participants at 19 years.

MRI assessment

MRI was performed on the same 1.5 Tesla Siemens Symphony Sonata (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) at St Olav’s University Hospital (Trondheim, Norway) at 15 and 19 years of age with Quantum gradients (30 mT/m) and a quadrature head coil. A structural T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence was acquired with the following specifications: TR = 7.1 ms, TE = 3.45 ms, TI = 1000 ms, flip angle 7o, FOV 256 × 256, slab thickness 170 mm, slice thickness 1.33 mm, acquisition matrix 256 × 192 × 128, reconstructed to 256 × 256 × 128, giving a reconstructed voxel resolution of 1 × 1 × 1.33 mm, and acquisition duration of 8.5 min. Two MPRAGE sequences acquired at each time point (15 and 19 years) were registered to correct for head motion and averaged into a single image. FreeSurfer software package 5.3.0 (http://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu/​) was used for the volumetric segmentation. This is an automated method of labeling human structures to extract GM and WM volumes for each participant’s entire brain [30, 31], and parcellating of the cortex of each participant [32, 33]. All processed images were visually inspected for accuracy of segmentation. Structures with obvious segmentation errors were rejected and no manual editing was performed to avoid introducing bias and increasing variances into the data set of MRI images. All images were processed with the longitudinal stream in FreeSurfer to enable longitudinal analyses [3436] and to account for unbalanced time points [37]. For each participant, mean cerebellar volumes of GM and WM and estimated intracranial volume (eICV) were extracted and used in further analyses. The eICV volume is an indirect measure of the whole volume inside the human cranium except cerebellum, brain stem and ventricles.

Clinical assessment

Parents and children were interviewed separately by senior clinicians at both follow-ups using the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children (KSADS) [38]. Diagnoses were set according to the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) [39] and categorized in three levels according to the KSADS scoring: (I) diagnoses (II) subclinical diagnoses (≥75 % of diagnostic criteria met, but not meeting criteria for full diagnosis), and (III) healthy. In order to study mental health over time, we divided the VLBW adolescents into two groups: persisting/developing diagnosis and healthy/becoming healthy. In the first group, we included those VLBW adolescents who had a psychiatric/subclinical diagnosis at both ages or developed one from 15 to 19 years. In the second group, we included VLBW adolescents who were healthy at both ages or became healthy from 15 to 19 years.
At the interview, general psycho-social functioning was rated according to the children’s global assessment scale (CGAS; scored from 1 to 100) [40]. To further assess psychiatric symptoms, the participants completed the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment (ASEBA); The youth self-report (YSR) at 15 years, and the adult self-report (ASR) at 19 years [41]. This is a screening instrument generating three composite scales: Total problems, internalizing and externalizing scales. ADHD symptoms were measured asking the mother to complete the ADHD Rating Scale-IV (ADHD-RS-IV) Home version for children at the 15-year assessment and the version for young adults at the 19-year assessment [42]. At 19 years, full IQ was obtained by a senior neuropsychologist with Wechsler adult intelligence scale, 3rd edition (WAIS-III) [43].

Statistical analysis

Differences in cerebellar volume between the VLBW group and the control group were analyzed using a general linear model (GLM), adjusting for age, sex and eICV. For the non-normally distributed variables of psychiatric and perinatal data we used the Mann–Whitney U test. Differences in diagnostic levels were analyzed by the unconditional z-pooled test (http://​www4.​stat.​ncsu.​edu/​~boos/​exact/​) [44].
All longitudinal changes in brain volumes were studied by means of mixed model linear regression, adjusting for sex and eICV. Mixed model methods allowed us to perform analyses combining cross-sectional and longitudinal data, accounting for missing data, irregular intervals between measures and within person dependence [45]. We investigated longitudinal differences in cerebellar WM and GM volumes between VLBW adolescents divided according to diagnostic status during adolescence (persisting/developing diagnosis vs. healthy/becoming healthy) and controls. We also analyzed the relationship between cerebellar WM and GM changes and the longitudinal changes of psychiatric symptoms and function assessed with questionnaires in the VLBW group (CGAS, ASEBA and ADHD-RS-IV).
We studied if there were cerebellar WM and GM volumetric differences between the two VLBW groups and the control group at 15 and 19 years of age by using a GLM, adjusting for age, sex and eICV. Linear regression was used to explore the relationship between cerebellar GM and WM volumes and psychiatric symptoms assessed with questionnaires at both 15 and 19 years of age, adjusting for age, sex and eICV. Normality of residuals was assessed by visual inspection of Q–Q plots. Missing cases were excluded pairwise. These analyses were further adjusted for IQ to elucidate the relationship between psychiatric diagnosis and symptoms, cognitive abilities and the cerebellum. However, results are presented before corrections to avoid shadowing the direct relationship between brain abnormalities and psychiatric symptoms [46].
Two-sided p values <0.05 were taken to indicate statistical significance, and 95 % confidence intervals (CI) are reported where relevant. All p values were corrected for multiple comparisons following the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (50 comparisons) [47]. Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics versions 20 and 22 (SPSS, Chicago, IL) and STATA/IC 13.1 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

Psychiatric and MRI findings

Neonatal and socio-demographic variables are displayed in Table 1. The VLBW group had lower IQ than the control group. There was no statistical significant difference in SES between the two groups. Cerebellar volumes and psychiatric outcome are given in Table 2. Compared with controls, the VLBW group had smaller volume of cerebellar WM at both ages, but cerebellar GM was not significantly different between the two groups at 15 or 19 years. The VLBW group had lower general psycho-social functioning expressed by lower CGAS scores at both ages. Psychiatric symptoms, measured by ASEBA-YSR and -ASR demonstrated no significant differences between the VLBW and the control group. The VLBW group had higher scores on the Inattention subscale of the ADHD-rating scale at 15 and 19 years. Compared with controls, there were more VLBW participants with psychiatric or subclinical diagnoses at both time points. In particular, the VLBW group had higher frequencies of ADHD diagnosis at both ages and higher frequency of anxiety disorders at 19 years.
Table 1
Participants’ neonatal and socio-demographic details
 
Assessed at 15 years
Assessed 19 at years
 
Assessed at both time points
VLBW
Control
VLBW
Control
VLBW
Control
Number of participants
40
56
44
60
 
30
37
 Males (%)
18 (45)
21 (37)
18 (41)
25 (42)
 
11 (37)
14 (38)
Background information
 Birthweight (grams) M (SD)
1204 (236)***
3713 (500)
1212 (234)***
3698 (501)
 
1223 (250)***
3766 (544)
 Gestational age (weeks)
29.18 (2.65)***
39.61 (1.15)
29.25 (2.54)***
39.72 (1.27)
 
29.43 (2.60)***
39.51 (1.17)
 Age (years-months) M (SD)
15–2 (0–6)
15–5 (0–5)
19–7 (0–7)
19–8 (0–6)
Time 1
15–2 (0–6)
15–5 (0–5)
     
Time 2
19–9 (0–8)
19–7 (0–6)
 IQ. M (SD)
  
89.00 (12.54)***
99.85 (10.62)
 
86.33 (13.52)***
100.14 (11.03)
 SES (1–5). M (SD)
3.15 (1.25)
3.59 (1.04)
3.39 (1.38)
3.70 (0.95)
 
3.27 (1.33)
3.65 (0.92)
Linear regression adjusted for age and sex for normal distributed data, else the Mann–Whitney U-test
The unconditional z-pooled test was used to analyze differences in proportions between groups
IQ intelligence quotient, M mean, SD standard deviation, SES socio-economic status, VLBW very low birth weight (birth weight ≤ 1500)
* p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001 (VLBW versus controls)
Table 2
Cerebellar volume and psychiatric outcome in VLBW participants and controls
 
Assessed at 15 years
Assessed 19 at years
VLBW (N = 40)
Control (N = 56)
VLBW (N = 44)
Control (N = 60)
Cerebellar volume (ml) M (SD)
 White matter M (SD)
25.52 (4.05)*
28.93 (3.04)
26.60 (4.03)*
29.83 (3.10)
 Gray matter M (SD)
99.46 (11.02)
103.93 (10.08)
96.59 (11.14)
103.57 (8.85)
Psychiatric questionnaires
 CGAS M (SD)
71.73 (14.48)***
86.96 (6.75)
79.05 (12.75)**
85.78 (7.69)
 ASEBA M (SD)
  Internalizing M (SD)
6.95 (5.27)
7.23 (5.96)
10.00 (9.45)
7.33 (7.25)
  Externalizing M (SD)
7.68 (4.74)
8.14 (5.84)
7.25 (4.99)
6.48 (5.78)
  Total problems. M (SD)
25.16 (14.91)
24.59 (15.81)
32.15 (21.53)
26.90 (20.78)
 ADHD-RS-IV
  Hyperactivity M (SD)
2.78 (3.71)
1.43 (1.78)
2.90 (4.29)
1.34 (1.67)
  Inattention M (SD)
6.39 (5.11)***
2.51 (2.81)
5.45 (5.58)**
1.76 (1.98)
Clinical diagnoses
 Any psychiatric diagnosis M (SD)
12 (30)**
3 (5.36)
11 (25)**
4 (6.67)
  Anxiety disorders M (SD)
5 (12.50)
2 (3.57)
7 (15.91)**
1 (1.67)
  ADHD M (SD)
3 (7.50)*
0 (0)
4 (9.09)*
0 (0)
  Other M (SD)
4 (10)
1 (1.76)
0 (0)
3 (5)
 Any subclinical diagnosis n (%)
11 (27.50)***
1 (1.76)
5 (11.36)
6 (10)
  Anxiety n (%)
3 (7.50)
1 (1.76)
4 (9.09)
2 (3.33)
  ADHD n (%)
8 (20)***
0 (0)
1 (2.27)
3 (5)
  Other n (%)
0 (0)
0 (0)
0 (0)
1 (1.67)
 Diagnostic status n (%)
  Healthy/became healthy n (%)
22 (55)**
46 (82)
25 (57)**
50 (83)
  Persisting/developed diagnosis n (%)
18 (45)**
10 (18)
16 (36)*
9 (15)
Linear regression adjusted for age and sex for normal distributed data, else the Mann–Whitney U-test. Cerebellar volumes adjusted for estimated intracranial volume. The unconditional z-pooled test was used to analyze differences in proportions between groups
ADHD-RS-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder rating scale, ASEBA the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment, YSR (Youth Self Report at 14 years) and ARS (Adult Self Report at 19 years), CGAS children’s global assessment scale, M mean, SD standard deviation, VLBW very low birth weight (birth weight ≤ 1500)
* p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001 (VLBW versus controls)

Relationship between clinical and MRI data

Cerebellar growth rate and psychiatric data

Mixed model linear regression results for the differences in cerebellar growth rate from 15 to 19 years of age between the two VLBW groups and controls are provided in Table 3. We did not find any differences in cerebellar growth between the two VLBW groups and controls (Fig. 2).
Table 3
Cerebellar growth differences between the two VLBW groups and the control group from 15 to 19 years of age
 
Interaction time × group
Coefficient
(95 % CI)
p value
Cerebellar white matter
−0.115
(−0.410 to 0.181)
0.447
Cerebellar gray matter
0.395
(−0.226 to 1.015)
0.213
Mixed linear regressions with groups of severity of diagnosis and time as independent variables and brain volumes (ml) as dependent variable. Adjusted for sex and estimated intracranial volume, but not for IQ
CI confidence interval, IQ intelligence quotient, VLBW very low birth weight
Mixed model linear regression analyses in the VLBW group between cerebellar volumetric changes over time and the development of psychiatric symptoms assessed with questionnaires (Additional file 1: Appendix S1) revealed an association between cerebellar WM volume increases over time and higher ADHD-RS-IV Inattention scores [B = 0.621 (0.0629–1.180) p = 0.029]. However, this association disappeared after corrections for multiple comparisons.

Cerebellar volumes and severity of diagnosis

Comparisons between cerebellar volumes in the two VLBW groups and controls are presented in Fig. 3. At both 15 and 19 years of age, VLBW adolescents who had or developed a psychiatric diagnosis during adolescence had smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes than controls. This VLBW group had also smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes than VLBW adolescents who were or became healthy in this period. After correcting for multiple comparisons, all results remained significant. Significance also remained after adjusting for IQ, except for cerebellar GM differences at 15 years. Detailed results before and after correction for IQ are provided in Additional file 2: Appendix S2A and B, respectively.

Cerebellar volumes and psychiatric symptoms assessed with questionnaires

Linear regression results between cerebellar volumes and psychiatric symptoms and function assessed by questionnaires in the VLBW group are provided in Table 4. Lower CGAS scores, indicating lower psychosocial functioning, were associated with smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes at both 15 and 19 years of age (Fig. 4). These results remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. We did not find any associations between cerebellar volumes and the ASEBA composite scales, nor with ADHD-RS-IV Hyperactivity scores. However, we found that the ADHD-RS-IV Inattention scores were associated with smaller cerebellar GM volumes at both ages and with smaller WM volume at 15 years (Fig. 5). After correcting for multiple testing, significance only remained for cerebellar GM differences at 15 years of age (Fig. 5c). When we further corrected the analyses for IQ, only significant differences between smaller cerebellar GM volumes and poorer psychosocial functioning (CGAS) at 19 years remained. Results with IQ corrections are provided in Additional file 3: Appendix S3.
Table 4
Linear regression with psychiatric data as dependent variable and cerebellar volumes (ml) as independent variable in the VLBW group
 
15 years
19 years
Coefficient
(95 % CI)
p value
Coefficient
(95 % CI)
p value
CGAS (15 years n = 40, 19 years n = 41)
 Cerebellar WM
1.930
(0.841 to 3.020)
0.001 a
1.450
(0.362 to 2.539)
0.010 a
 Cerebellar GM
0.630
(0.187 to 1.072)
0.007 a
0.764
(0.382 to 1.147)
<0.000 a
ASEBA (15 years n = 38, 19 years n = 40)
 Internalizing
  Cerebellar WM
−0.259
(−0.735 to 0.217)
0.276
−0.454
(−1.317 to 0.410)
0.294
  Cerebellar GM
−0.118
(−0.301 to 0.064)
0.196
−0.287
(−0.611 to 0.038)
0.082
 Externalizing
  Cerebellar WM
−0.098
(−0.516 to 0.320)
0.637
−0.223
(−0.70 to 0.254)
0.349
  Cerebellar GM
−0.130
(−0.285 to 0.026)
0.099
−0.099
(−0.283 to 0.085)
0.282
 Total problems
  Cerebellar WM
−0.818
(−2.142 to 0.506)
0.217
−1.190
(−3.201 to 0.820)
0.238
  Cerebellar GM
−0.457
(−0.955 to 0.040)
0.070
−0.683
(−1.441 to 0.074)
0.075
ADHD-RS-IV (15 years n = 36, 19 years n = 29) 
 Hyperactivity
  Cerebellar WM
−0.286
(−0.597 to 0.026)
0.071
0.158
(−0.348 to 0.663)
0.526
  Cerebellar GM
−0.092
(−0.214 to 0.030)
0.135
0.007
(−0.190 to 0.204)
0.940
 Inattention
  Cerebellar WM
−0.528
(−0.950 to 0.105)
0.016
−0.256
(−0.899 to 0.387)
0.420
  Cerebellar GM
−0.222
(−0.382 to 0.061)
0.008 *
−0.243
(−0.473 to 0.012)
0.040
Adjusted for age, sex and estimated intracranial volume, but not for IQ
ADHD-RS-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder rating scale, ASEBA Achenbach system of empirically based assessment, YSR (Youth Self Report at 14 years) and ARS (Adult Self Report at 19 years), CGAS children’s global assessment scale, CI confidence interval, GM gray matter, IQ intelligence quotient, VLBW very low birth weight, WM white matter
a Significant results also when corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure

Discussion

We studied the relationship between cerebellar volumes and psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses at 15 and 19 years of age in adolescents born very preterm and with VLBW. Cerebellar growth trajectories from 15 to 19 years of age were equal between adolescents born with VLBW and controls, regardless of psychiatric morbidity (Fig. 2). However, VLBW adolescents with a persisting/developing diagnosis during adolescence had maintained smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes compared with controls and compared with VLBW adolescents who were healthy or became healthy during this period. Moreover, cerebellar volumes did not differ between VLBW adolescents who were or became healthy from 15 to 19 years of age and controls (Fig. 3). At both 15 and 19 years of age, larger cerebellar WM and GM volumes correlated with better general psychosocial functioning in the VLBW group (Fig. 4).
Smaller cerebellar volumes have been consistently reported in preterm children compared with term-born peers from birth [6, 4851] to childhood [52] and adolescence [2729, 5355]. Even though overall smaller brain volumes are a common trait in children born very preterm and with VLBW [55], some studies suggest that there are not brain growth differences, including the cerebellum, between individuals born preterm and term-born peers [29, 55, 56]. Nonetheless, other studies have found differences in cerebellar trajectories between preterm and term-born children during adolescence. In an MRI longitudinal study, Parker et al. [27] reported total cerebellar volume reduction from 15 to 18 years of age in a cohort of adolescents born very preterm compared with term-born peers. This reduction in cerebellar volume was associated with having more problems in several questionnaire items concerning concentration, feeling useful, decision-making capability, overcoming difficulties, feeling confident and feeling worthless. Abnormal cerebellar growth has also been reported to occur right after birth, even after normal cerebellar ultrasound [6, 48]. Preterm children with the most deviant cerebellar development have higher rates of intraventricular hemorrhage and other associated complications like post-hemorrhagic hydrocephalus and neurosurgical interventions [50, 5759]. However, extreme prematurity has been noted as the most explicative factor for disruptive cerebellar development in VLBW neonates [6, 51, 57, 59, 60]. The causes of deviant cerebellar development in the absence of apparent damage are unknown [9]. We speculate that the smaller cerebellar volumes in VLBW adolescents with persistent/increasing mental health problems might be originated in the perinatal/neonatal period.
In order to properly understand the role of the cerebellum in the appearance and maintenance of psychiatric disorders in children born very preterm and with VLBW, it is important to study its anatomy and how premature birth affects its development. The cerebellum is connected with the whole brain, especially with the cerebral cortex [24, 61, 62]. The vast development of the cerebellum occurs, mainly, in the third trimester of gestation, where the cerebellum increases its volume fourfold [6]. Early disruption of the cerebellar circuitry development has been positively correlated with ASD, attention deficit and emotional problems [25]. In fact, a meta-analysis pointed out cerebellar abnormalities as the most consistently reported structural finding for ADHD [63], but the results whether total cerebellar volume, WM or GM or both are abnormal were inconclusive. ADHD symptom severity has been shown to correlate with overall cerebellar volume [64]. Many studies have found cerebellar abnormalities in ADHD, especially in the vermis [6571]. Our finding of smaller cerebellar GM volume associated with higher inattention symptoms in the VLBW group points out GM as related to their symptoms. However, the lack of correlation with WM volumes might be due to low statistical power. Diffusion tensor imaging studies investigating WM differences in ADHD have reported reduced fractional anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncles [72, 73], and WM in the left cerebellum [73, 74]. A recent fMRI study has reported a link of abnormal cerebro-cerebellar connectivity with ADHD, and particularly for inattention, suggesting that ADHD symptoms are not the direct result of cerebellar abnormalities, but the result of damages in the whole network [75].
Injury confined to the cerebellum has been associated with reduced growth in specific regions of the uninjured contralateral cerebral cortex in preterm born neonates (dorsolateral prefrontal, premotor, sensorimotor, and mid-temporal regions), and this secondary growth reduction has been associated with a higher risk of impaired cognitive, language, behavior, and motor performance [49, 76]. This suggests that injury to the immature cerebellum could affect neurologic function through mechanisms that interfere with later development of remote regions of the cerebral cortex [26]. Projections from the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex constitute the cerebello-thalamo-cortical (CTC) pathway, the main efferent cerebellar projection [23, 24]. The CTC pathway has been linked with deficits in information processing in schizophrenia [77, 78], and disruption in cortico-cerebellar connectivity has been proposed as a major neurobiological mechanism of emotional dysregulation [79]. In preterm infants, thalamo-cortical projections measured with DTI were affected compared with born-at-term controls, suggesting that such pathways might be especially vulnerable to preterm birth [80]. Different fMRI studies have also shown activation of the cerebellum in different emotional tasks, such us viewing pictures of deceased loved ones [81] and viewing pictures and movies evoking both negative and positive feelings [82, 83]. These results suggest that the cerebellum is directly involved in the appraisal of emotional stimuli. The closed-loop circuits between the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex serve as the anatomical substrate by which the cerebellum can modulate activation patterns in distal regions. Based on these interactions, it has been proposed that cerebellar dysfunction or disruption early in development could have major impacts on the structure and function of the cortical regions to which it projects [2]. We hypothesize that deviant cerebellar development, probably occurring around the neonatal period, might cause smaller cerebellar volumes that are carried onto adolescence affecting the cerebellar circuitry and its extensive connections with the brain, and that this injury pattern probably plays an important role in the development and maintenance of psychiatric disorders in this population. Moreover, discriminating VLBW children with significant smaller cerebellar volumes than controls might help to identify which of them are at risk of developing psychiatric problems, and it might aid to take preventive actions at an earlier stage.
In order to clarify the relationship between psychiatric symptoms, general cognitive abilities and cerebellar volumes, we corrected our analyses for IQ. After this, all the results remained significant, except for differences in cerebellar GM volume at 15, the relationship between cerebellar volumes and inattention, and cerebellar WM volumes and psychosocial function at both ages and GM at 15. IQ summarizes psychological processes such as attention, executive functions, and general knowledge [84], while social cognition measures are not properly covered. Having intellectual disability has been linked to higher prevalence of psychiatric disorders [85], and recent research suggests that impaired executive functions (i.e. inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) is a core feature in many mental illnesses. If a deficit in executive functions is a core symptom of psychiatric disorders, correcting for IQ might be an overcorrection in brain imaging studies trying to find the neural basis of mental health illness [86]. Still, the nature of this correlation is not fully understood yet [85, 87]. A cohort study using data from over 900,000 individuals in Sweden has linked higher IQ scores during adolescence with higher risk of developing bipolar disorder later in life, controlling for socioeconomic group and parental education [88]. At the same time, neurocognitive deficits in bipolar disorder have been highlighted as an important determinant of the disruptive nature of this disorder [89]. In preterm born individuals, cognitive abilities might be affected by the same brain mechanisms that affect mental health problems. Thus, if IQ, which is also the result of brain characteristics [90], is a mediating variable in mental health disorders, rather than a core feature, correcting for IQ might shadow the direct relationship between brain abnormalities and psychiatric symptoms [46]. More research is needed to clarify this matter.
The strength of this study is the usage of both questionnaires and a semi-structured diagnostic interview for identifying psychiatric symptoms and disorders, thereby contributing to a comprehensive psychiatric evaluation. Another strength is the combined cross-sectional and longitudinal design with clinical and MRI assessments at both 15 and 19 years of age in this cohort. The participation rate was comparable to other follow-up studies with similar study groups [91] and participants and non-participants did not differ in perinatal variables (gestational age, birth weight, maternal age at birth), making selection bias less likely. Another strength is that the groups did not differ in levels of socio-economic status.
We used a well-known and reliable automated MR segmentation method to measure cerebellar volumes (http://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu/​). However, this method only provides delineation of the total cerebellum, differentiating between WM and GM, but not accounting for the cerebellar vermis. FreeSurfer has been shown to underestimate the cerebellar region [92] and problems to distinguish perfectly between the cerebellum and brain stem [93]. We inspected all processed images for accuracy of the FreeSurfer segmentation and rejected structures with obvious segmentation errors. However, in order to avoid introducing bias and increasing variances into the data set of MRI images, no manual editing was performed. Further studies using MRI with higher field strength or improved algorithms for segmentation that allows more detailed cerebellar segmentations, may be required to support or reject our results.
Another limitation of the study is group size. Due to the relatively small sample, only large differences and strong associations could reach significant levels. Hence, negative findings should be interpreted with caution. We had longitudinal data for a smaller sample than the cross-sectional study groups, which reduced the statistical power and hence, the generalization of the longitudinal results. Studies with larger samples are certainly needed to confirm the findings.
The ASEBA self-report composite scales were not associated with cerebellar volumes, while results from diagnostic evaluation were. This can be explained by a discrepancy in our VLBW group between the ASEBA self-report and the clinical reports at both assessments, suggesting that the VLBW group might be under-reporting problems on questionnaires [4, 94]. Similar discrepancies have been reported in other studies between self-reports and parent-reports in VLBW young adult populations [95, 96]. Therefore, the lack of association with the ASEBA subscales should be interpreted with this in mind.

Conclusion

Cerebellar growth rate was the same for VLBW children and term-born peers regardless of psychiatric status. Still, VLBW participants who had persistent or developed psychiatric problems during adolescence presented smaller cerebellar WM and GM volumes compared with controls, and also compared with VLBW adolescents who were or became healthy during this period. Our results suggest that significant smaller cerebellar volumes may be a biomarker for developing and maintaining psychiatric problems during adolescence in individuals born with VLBW.

Authors’ contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VLB, JS, AKH, AMB, MSI, MM. Performed the experiments: JS, AMB, GCL, MSI, MM. Analyzed the data: VLB, SL, KJB. Wrote the paper: VLB, MM. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The study was funded by The Research Council of Norway (Research Program on Mental Health—no213732) and the Liaison Committee between the Faculty of Medicine, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU) and St Olav University Hospital, Trondheim, Norway. We used the Abel cluster for this work.The cluster is owned by the University of Oslo and the Norwegian metacenter for High-Performance Computing (NOTUR), and operated by the Department for Research Computing at USIT, the University of Oslo IT-department. http://​www.​hpc.​uio.​no.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Blencowe H, Cousens S, Oestergaard MZ, Chou D, Moller AB, Narwal R, et al. National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications. Lancet. 2012;379(9832):2162–72. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60820-4.CrossRefPubMed Blencowe H, Cousens S, Oestergaard MZ, Chou D, Moller AB, Narwal R, et al. National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications. Lancet. 2012;379(9832):2162–72. doi:10.​1016/​S0140-6736(12)60820-4.CrossRefPubMed
2.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Indredavik MS, Vik T, Heyerdahl S, Kulseng S, Brubakk AM. Psychiatric symptoms in low birth weight adolescents, assessed by screening questionnaires. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;14(4):226–36. doi:10.1007/s00787-005-0459-6.CrossRefPubMed Indredavik MS, Vik T, Heyerdahl S, Kulseng S, Brubakk AM. Psychiatric symptoms in low birth weight adolescents, assessed by screening questionnaires. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;14(4):226–36. doi:10.​1007/​s00787-005-0459-6.CrossRefPubMed
6.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Steggerda SJ, Leijser LM, Wiggers-de Bruine FT, van der Grond J, Walther FJ, van Wezel-Meijler G. Cerebellar injury in preterm infants: incidence and findings on US and MR images. Radiology. 2009;252(1):190–9. doi:10.1148/radiol.2521081525.CrossRefPubMed Steggerda SJ, Leijser LM, Wiggers-de Bruine FT, van der Grond J, Walther FJ, van Wezel-Meijler G. Cerebellar injury in preterm infants: incidence and findings on US and MR images. Radiology. 2009;252(1):190–9. doi:10.​1148/​radiol.​2521081525.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Gauvreau K, Robertson RL Jr, Sullivan NR, Benson CB, et al. Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors? Pediatrics. 2007;120(3):584–93. doi:10.1542/peds.2007-1041.CrossRefPubMed Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Gauvreau K, Robertson RL Jr, Sullivan NR, Benson CB, et al. Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors? Pediatrics. 2007;120(3):584–93. doi:10.​1542/​peds.​2007-1041.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Katz DB, Steinmetz JE. Psychological functions of the cerebellum. Behav Cognit Neurosci Rev. 2002;1(3):229–41.CrossRef Katz DB, Steinmetz JE. Psychological functions of the cerebellum. Behav Cognit Neurosci Rev. 2002;1(3):229–41.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey. J Neurosci. 1997;17(1):438–58.PubMed Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey. J Neurosci. 1997;17(1):438–58.PubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Clower DM, West RA, Lynch JC, Strick PL. The inferior parietal lobule is the target of output from the superior colliculus, hippocampus, and cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2001;21(16):6283–91.PubMed Clower DM, West RA, Lynch JC, Strick PL. The inferior parietal lobule is the target of output from the superior colliculus, hippocampus, and cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2001;21(16):6283–91.PubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23(23):8432–44.PubMed Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23(23):8432–44.PubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology. 2001;57(2):245–54.CrossRefPubMed Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology. 2001;57(2):245–54.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Limperopoulos C, Chilingaryan G, Sullivan N, Guizard N, Robertson RL, du Plessis AJ. Injury to the premature cerebellum: outcome is related to remote cortical development. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2014;24(3):728–36. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs354. Limperopoulos C, Chilingaryan G, Sullivan N, Guizard N, Robertson RL, du Plessis AJ. Injury to the premature cerebellum: outcome is related to remote cortical development. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2014;24(3):728–36. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhs354.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker J, Mitchell A, Kalpakidou A, Walshe M, Jung HY, Nosarti C, et al. Cerebellar growth and behavioural & neuropsychological outcome in preterm adolescents. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 5):1344–51. doi:10.1093/brain/awn062.PubMed Parker J, Mitchell A, Kalpakidou A, Walshe M, Jung HY, Nosarti C, et al. Cerebellar growth and behavioural & neuropsychological outcome in preterm adolescents. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 5):1344–51. doi:10.​1093/​brain/​awn062.PubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Martinussen M, Flanders DW, Fischl B, Busa E, Lohaugen GC, Skranes J et al. Segmental brain volumes and cognitive and perceptual correlates in 15-year-old adolescents with low birth weight. J Pediatrics. 2009;155(6):848–53 e1. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.06.015. Martinussen M, Flanders DW, Fischl B, Busa E, Lohaugen GC, Skranes J et al. Segmental brain volumes and cognitive and perceptual correlates in 15-year-old adolescents with low birth weight. J Pediatrics. 2009;155(6):848–53 e1. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jpeds.​2009.​06.​015.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–55.CrossRefPubMed Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–55.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Segonne F, Salat DH et al. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2004;14(1):11–22. Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Segonne F, Salat DH et al. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2004;14(1):11–22.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980–8. doi:10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021.CrossRefPubMed Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980–8. doi:10.​1097/​00004583-199707000-00021.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed, text revision ed. Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed, text revision ed. Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaffer D, Gould M, Brasic J. A children’s global assessment scale (CGAS) (for children 4 to 16 years of age). Psychopharmacollly Bulletin. 1985;21:747–8. Shaffer D, Gould M, Brasic J. A children’s global assessment scale (CGAS) (for children 4 to 16 years of age). Psychopharmacollly Bulletin. 1985;21:747–8.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles: child behavior checklist for ages 6–18, teacher’s report form, youth self-report: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington: ASEBA; 2001. Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles: child behavior checklist for ages 6–18, teacher’s report form, youth self-report: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington: ASEBA; 2001.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a clinical workbook. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1998. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a clinical workbook. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1998.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufman AS, Lichtenberger EO. Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 2005. Kaufman AS, Lichtenberger EO. Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 2005.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Lydersen S, Langaas M, Bakke Ø. The exact unconditional z-pooled test for equality of two binomial probabilities: optimal choice of the berger and boos confidence coefficient. J Stat Comput Simul. 2012;82(9):1311–6.CrossRef Lydersen S, Langaas M, Bakke Ø. The exact unconditional z-pooled test for equality of two binomial probabilities: optimal choice of the berger and boos confidence coefficient. J Stat Comput Simul. 2012;82(9):1311–6.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox J, Weisberg S. An R companion to applied regression. London: Sage; 2010. Fox J, Weisberg S. An R companion to applied regression. London: Sage; 2010.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Gran MG, H. S, Håberg SE, O AO. Causal inference. In: Veierød M, Lydersen S, Laake P, editors. Medical statistics in clinical and epidemiological research. Gyldendal Akademisk. 2012. Gran MG, H. S, Håberg SE, O AO. Causal inference. In: Veierød M, Lydersen S, Laake P, editors. Medical statistics in clinical and epidemiological research. Gyldendal Akademisk. 2012.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Messerschmidt A, Brugger PC, Boltshauser E, Zoder G, Sterniste W, Birnbacher R, et al. Disruption of cerebellar development: potential complication of extreme prematurity. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2005;26(7):1659–67.PubMed Messerschmidt A, Brugger PC, Boltshauser E, Zoder G, Sterniste W, Birnbacher R, et al. Disruption of cerebellar development: potential complication of extreme prematurity. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2005;26(7):1659–67.PubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Limperopoulos C, Soul JS, Haidar H, Huppi PS, Bassan H, Warfield SK, et al. Impaired trophic interactions between the cerebellum and the cerebrum among preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2005;116(4):844–50. doi:10.1542/peds.2004-2282.CrossRefPubMed Limperopoulos C, Soul JS, Haidar H, Huppi PS, Bassan H, Warfield SK, et al. Impaired trophic interactions between the cerebellum and the cerebrum among preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2005;116(4):844–50. doi:10.​1542/​peds.​2004-2282.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Messerschmidt A, Prayer D, Brugger PC, Boltshauser E, Zoder G, Sterniste W, et al. Preterm birth and disruptive cerebellar development: assessment of perinatal risk factors. Eur J Paediatric Neurol. 2008;12(6):455–60. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2007.11.003.CrossRef Messerschmidt A, Prayer D, Brugger PC, Boltshauser E, Zoder G, Sterniste W, et al. Preterm birth and disruptive cerebellar development: assessment of perinatal risk factors. Eur J Paediatric Neurol. 2008;12(6):455–60. doi:10.​1016/​j.​ejpn.​2007.​11.​003.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Bodensteiner JB, Johnsen SD. Cerebellar injury in the extremely premature infant: newly recognized but relatively common outcome. J Child Neurol. 2005;20(2):139–42.CrossRefPubMed Bodensteiner JB, Johnsen SD. Cerebellar injury in the extremely premature infant: newly recognized but relatively common outcome. J Child Neurol. 2005;20(2):139–42.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, Cannistraci CJ, Dolberg A, Schneider KC, et al. Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. JAMA. 2000;284(15):1939–47.CrossRefPubMed Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, Cannistraci CJ, Dolberg A, Schneider KC, et al. Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. JAMA. 2000;284(15):1939–47.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Argyropoulou MI, Xydis V, Drougia A, Argyropoulou PI, Tzoufi M, Bassounas A, et al. MRI measurements of the pons and cerebellum in children born preterm; associations with the severity of periventricular leukomalacia and perinatal risk factors. Neuroradiology. 2003;45(10):730–4. doi:10.1007/s00234-003-1067-0.CrossRefPubMed Argyropoulou MI, Xydis V, Drougia A, Argyropoulou PI, Tzoufi M, Bassounas A, et al. MRI measurements of the pons and cerebellum in children born preterm; associations with the severity of periventricular leukomalacia and perinatal risk factors. Neuroradiology. 2003;45(10):730–4. doi:10.​1007/​s00234-003-1067-0.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Allin M, Matsumoto H, Santhouse AM, Nosarti C, AlAsady MH, Stewart AL, et al. Cognitive and motor function and the size of the cerebellum in adolescents born very pre-term. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 1):60–6.CrossRefPubMed Allin M, Matsumoto H, Santhouse AM, Nosarti C, AlAsady MH, Stewart AL, et al. Cognitive and motor function and the size of the cerebellum in adolescents born very pre-term. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 1):60–6.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnsen SD, Bodensteiner JB, Lotze TE. Frequency and nature of cerebellar injury in the extremely premature survivor with cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol. 2005;20(1):60–4.CrossRefPubMed Johnsen SD, Bodensteiner JB, Lotze TE. Frequency and nature of cerebellar injury in the extremely premature survivor with cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol. 2005;20(1):60–4.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Mercuri E, He J, Curati WL, Dubowitz LM, Cowan FM, Bydder GM. Cerebellar infarction and atrophy in infants and children with a history of premature birth. Pediatr Radiol. 1997;27(2):139–43. doi:10.1007/s002470050085.CrossRefPubMed Mercuri E, He J, Curati WL, Dubowitz LM, Cowan FM, Bydder GM. Cerebellar infarction and atrophy in infants and children with a history of premature birth. Pediatr Radiol. 1997;27(2):139–43. doi:10.​1007/​s002470050085.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Rollins NK, Wen TS, Dominguez R. Crossed cerebellar atrophy in children: a neurologic sequela of extreme prematurity. Pediatr Radiol. 1995;25(Suppl 1):S20–5.PubMed Rollins NK, Wen TS, Dominguez R. Crossed cerebellar atrophy in children: a neurologic sequela of extreme prematurity. Pediatr Radiol. 1995;25(Suppl 1):S20–5.PubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Krageloh-Mann I, Toft P, Lunding J, Andresen J, Pryds O, Lou HC. Brain lesions in preterms: origin, consequences and compensation. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992). 1999;88(8):897–908. Krageloh-Mann I, Toft P, Lunding J, Andresen J, Pryds O, Lou HC. Brain lesions in preterms: origin, consequences and compensation. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992). 1999;88(8):897–908.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Manto M, Gruol DL, Schmahmann J, Koibuchi N, Rossi F, editors. Handbook of the cerebellum and cerebellar disorders. New York: Springer; 2013. Manto M, Gruol DL, Schmahmann J, Koibuchi N, Rossi F, editors. Handbook of the cerebellum and cerebellar disorders. New York: Springer; 2013.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2002;288(14):1740–8.CrossRefPubMed Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2002;288(14):1740–8.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, et al. Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology. 1998;50(4):1087–93.CrossRefPubMed Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, et al. Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology. 1998;50(4):1087–93.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Mostofsky SH, Reiss AL, Lockhart P, Denckla MB. Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol. 1998;13(9):434–9.CrossRefPubMed Mostofsky SH, Reiss AL, Lockhart P, Denckla MB. Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol. 1998;13(9):434–9.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Bussing R, Grudnik J, Mason D, Wasiak M, Leonard C. ADHD and conduct disorder: an MRI study in a community sample. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2002;3(4):216–20.CrossRefPubMed Bussing R, Grudnik J, Mason D, Wasiak M, Leonard C. ADHD and conduct disorder: an MRI study in a community sample. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2002;3(4):216–20.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Berquin PC, Walter JM, Sharp W, Tran T, et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(3):289–95.CrossRefPubMed Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Berquin PC, Walter JM, Sharp W, Tran T, et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(3):289–95.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Hill DE, Yeo RA, Campbell RA, Hart B, Vigil J, Brooks W. Magnetic resonance imaging correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(3):496–506.CrossRefPubMed Hill DE, Yeo RA, Campbell RA, Hart B, Vigil J, Brooks W. Magnetic resonance imaging correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(3):496–506.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Bechtel N, Kobel M, Penner IK, Klarhofer M, Scheffler K, Opwis K, et al. Decreased fractional anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncle in children with epilepsy and/or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a preliminary study. Epilepsy Behav. 2009;15(3):294–8. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.04.005.CrossRefPubMed Bechtel N, Kobel M, Penner IK, Klarhofer M, Scheffler K, Opwis K, et al. Decreased fractional anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncle in children with epilepsy and/or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a preliminary study. Epilepsy Behav. 2009;15(3):294–8. doi:10.​1016/​j.​yebeh.​2009.​04.​005.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Kucyi A, Hove MJ, Biederman J, Van Dijk KRA, Valera EM. Disrupted functional connectivity of cerebellar default network areas in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(9):3373–86. doi:10.1002/hbm.22850.CrossRefPubMed Kucyi A, Hove MJ, Biederman J, Van Dijk KRA, Valera EM. Disrupted functional connectivity of cerebellar default network areas in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(9):3373–86. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​22850.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreasen NC. Schizophrenia: the big questions. Seishin shinkeigaku zasshi = Psychiatria et neurologia Japonica. 2002;104(4):249. Andreasen NC. Schizophrenia: the big questions. Seishin shinkeigaku zasshi = Psychiatria et neurologia Japonica. 2002;104(4):249.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Ponto LL, et al. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93(18):9985–90.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Ponto LL, et al. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93(18):9985–90.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguistics. 2000;13(2):189–214.CrossRef Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguistics. 2000;13(2):189–214.CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Gündel H, O’connor M-F, Littrell L, Fort C, Lane RD. Functional neuroanatomy of grief: an FMRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2014. Gündel H, O’connor M-F, Littrell L, Fort C, Lane RD. Functional neuroanatomy of grief: an FMRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2014.
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):918–25.CrossRefPubMed Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):918–25.CrossRefPubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Liotti M, Mayberg HS, McGinnis S, Brannan SL, Jerabek P. Unmasking disease-specific cerebral blood flow abnormalities: mood challenge in patients with remitted unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2014. Liotti M, Mayberg HS, McGinnis S, Brannan SL, Jerabek P. Unmasking disease-specific cerebral blood flow abnormalities: mood challenge in patients with remitted unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2014.
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children-third edition, Swedish version. Stockholm: Psykologiforlaget AB; 1999. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children-third edition, Swedish version. Stockholm: Psykologiforlaget AB; 1999.
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Ricciardi J. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in individuals with intellectual disability. In: Luiselli JK, Reed DD, DiGennaro Reed FD, editors. Handbook of crisis intervention and developmental disabilities. issues in clinical child psychology. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 213–43.CrossRef Ricciardi J. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in individuals with intellectual disability. In: Luiselli JK, Reed DD, DiGennaro Reed FD, editors. Handbook of crisis intervention and developmental disabilities. issues in clinical child psychology. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 213–43.CrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Etkin A, Gyurak A, O’Hara R. A neurobiological approach to the cognitive deficits of psychiatric disorders. Dialogues Clinical Neurosci. 2013;15(4):419–29. Etkin A, Gyurak A, O’Hara R. A neurobiological approach to the cognitive deficits of psychiatric disorders. Dialogues Clinical Neurosci. 2013;15(4):419–29.
88.
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Bearden CE, Hoffman KM, Cannon TD. The neuropsychology and neuroanatomy of bipolar affective disorder: a critical review. Bipolar disorders. 2001;3(3):106–50; discussion 51–3. Bearden CE, Hoffman KM, Cannon TD. The neuropsychology and neuroanatomy of bipolar affective disorder: a critical review. Bipolar disorders. 2001;3(3):106–50; discussion 51–3.
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Schnack HG, van Haren NE, Brouwer RM, Evans A, Durston S, Boomsma DI et al. Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2015;25(6):1608–17. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht357. Schnack HG, van Haren NE, Brouwer RM, Evans A, Durston S, Boomsma DI et al. Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991). 2015;25(6):1608–17. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bht357.
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Fewtrell MS, Kennedy K, Singhal A, Martin RM, Ness A, Hadders-Algra M, et al. How much loss to follow-up is acceptable in long-term randomised trials and prospective studies? Arch Dis Child. 2008;93(6):458–61. doi:10.1136/adc.2007.127316.CrossRefPubMed Fewtrell MS, Kennedy K, Singhal A, Martin RM, Ness A, Hadders-Algra M, et al. How much loss to follow-up is acceptable in long-term randomised trials and prospective studies? Arch Dis Child. 2008;93(6):458–61. doi:10.​1136/​adc.​2007.​127316.CrossRefPubMed
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee DK, Yoon U, Kwak K, Lee JM. Automated segmentation of cerebellum using brain mask and partial volume estimation map. Computational Math Methods Med. 2015;2015:167489. doi:10.1155/2015/167489.CrossRef Lee DK, Yoon U, Kwak K, Lee JM. Automated segmentation of cerebellum using brain mask and partial volume estimation map. Computational Math Methods Med. 2015;2015:167489. doi:10.​1155/​2015/​167489.CrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Lund LK, Vik T, Lydersen S, Lohaugen GC, Skranes J, Brubakk AM, et al. Mental health, quality of life and social relations in young adults born with low birth weight. Health Quality Life Outcomes. 2012;10:146. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-10-146.CrossRef Lund LK, Vik T, Lydersen S, Lohaugen GC, Skranes J, Brubakk AM, et al. Mental health, quality of life and social relations in young adults born with low birth weight. Health Quality Life Outcomes. 2012;10:146. doi:10.​1186/​1477-7525-10-146.CrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Hack M, Youngstrom EA, Cartar L, Schluchter M, Taylor HG, Flannery D, et al. Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopathology among very low birth weight infants at age 20 years. Pediatrics. 2004;114(4):932–40. doi:10.1542/peds.2003-1017-L.CrossRefPubMed Hack M, Youngstrom EA, Cartar L, Schluchter M, Taylor HG, Flannery D, et al. Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopathology among very low birth weight infants at age 20 years. Pediatrics. 2004;114(4):932–40. doi:10.​1542/​peds.​2003-1017-L.CrossRefPubMed
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Hille ET, Dorrepaal C, Perenboom R, Gravenhorst JB, Brand R, Verloove-Vanhorick SP. Social lifestyle, risk-taking behavior, and psychopathology in young adults born very preterm or with a very low birthweight. The Journal of pediatrics. 2008;152(6):793–800,.e1–4. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.11.041. Hille ET, Dorrepaal C, Perenboom R, Gravenhorst JB, Brand R, Verloove-Vanhorick SP. Social lifestyle, risk-taking behavior, and psychopathology in young adults born very preterm or with a very low birthweight. The Journal of pediatrics. 2008;152(6):793–800,.e1–4. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jpeds.​2007.​11.​041.
Metadaten
Titel
Mental health and cerebellar volume during adolescence in very-low-birth-weight infants: a longitudinal study
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Erschienen in
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1753-2000
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0093-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.