Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Study protocol

Mobility may impact attention abilities in healthy term or prematurely born children at 7-years of age: protocol for an intervention controlled trial

verfasst von: Hadrien Ceyte, Joëlle Rosenbaum, Isabelle Hamon, Maëlle Wirth, Sébastien Caudron, Jean-Michel Hascoët

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Seven years of age is a milestone for learning basic knowledge that is strongly related to attention abilities such as Alerting, Orienting, and Inhibition function, allowing for appropriate adaptation to primary school. These attention abilities are also influenced by gestational age at birth in a complex manner, indicating an area of weakness in prematurely born children. Furthermore, recent studies suggest that allowing children to have freedom of movement during learning may improve their attention level and school performance. The purpose of the present study is to determine the influence of mobility on the attentional components that may impact learning abilities in children aged 7-years who were born at term and prematurely.

Methods

This prospective, randomized, controlled trial will focus on psychometric testing of attentional abilities assessed with the Attention Network Test for Child (Child ANT) and involves a mixed measurement design. Forty-eight children aged 7-years, half of whom were premature at birth and in their expected grade without learning difficulties will be included after parental consent. They will be equipped with a head-mounted display in which the Child ANT will be presented. The association of different flankers and pre-cues will allow the measurement of the development level of Alerting, Orienting, and Inhibition function. The task will be composed of one experimental block of trials randomly performed per posture: seated, standing, or free.

Discussion

This study will assess the contribution of mobility in specific attentional contexts that are usually present during fundamental learning in children. New pedagogical formats of teaching could consider these findings, and new pedagogical tools enabling free spontaneous child mobility might be designed. Moreover, a small percentage of children integrating into the educational system are born prematurely. These children, often considered immature and hyperactive, could benefit from educational innovations that enhance their attention abilities, thereby improving their adaptation to primary school.

Trial registration

This trial is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT03125447).

Background

Five to 7-years of age is a milestone in children’s development. At this age, they begin school and acquire the basics of fundamental learning such as reading, writing, and calculating. In a general way, these lessons are strongly related to attentional abilities and executive functions such as working memory and inhibitory control in children [18].
Since 1990, Posner’s work highlighted three different attentional networks that are thought to relate to the activation of different brain areas [9]. These cerebral networks are related to three components of attention: sustained attention or alerting (maintaining vigilance abilities), selective attention or orienting (ability to shift the attention), and inhibition function (ability to focus on one feature of a stimulus and ignore other interfering features). Fan et al. [10] developed an integrated Attention Network Test (ANT) based on a flanker task [11] in order to independently measure the efficiency of these three networks. This test was validated in adults, where alerting was induced by warning signals given prior to a target event, orienting facilitated by explicit spatial cues prior to a target event, and inhibition function evaluated by introducing incongruent flankers around the target. The adaptation of this test for children born at term at the ages of 4 to 10-years [12] showed an independence in the development of these three attentional systems. Alerting and orienting components may mature at up to 6-years of age then stabilize, while the inhibition function may improve up to the age of 7-years then remain stable after this age [12].
Little research has assessed the three attention components in children born prematurely. Studies suggest that prematurity may induce delays in maturation for the three attentional networks throughout the preschool years rather than lead to a persistent impairment [13, 14]. These attention components are influenced by age at assessment and gestational age at birth in a complex manner, indicating an area of weakness in children born prematurely [13]. On one hand, the risk for deficits in these attention components increases with decreasing gestational age. On the other hand, the development of these attention components might follow different developmental trajectories in children who were born preterm. For alerting, studies did not show any difference between children born at term versus those born preterm at the age of about 8-years [15, 16]. For orienting, the adult level may be reached at 8-years of age in children born prematurely [15]. A developmental delay of about one year has been observed between children born at term versus those born preterm [1720]. Finally, data suggest that the inhibition function is still affected by prematurity at the age of 7-years [1517] and up to 11-years of age in some children [21]. However, many different experimental assessments have been used to study the inhibition function (Tapping Test, Go No-Go Test, Stroop Color World test, Continuous Performance Test, Test Everyday Attention for Children, etc), which have failed to determine consensual and accurate developmental delays for this attentional component.
In general, the attention level of children is considered to decrease when they are moving. The poor attentional performance in those born prematurely as compared to children born at term is also attributed to an impulsivity [16]. Moreover, the urge for mobility is frequently observed in school age children and is often described as “hyperactivity” [22]. However, this is a loaded concept because it implies an attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). This concept is one of the most extensively studied childhood psychiatric disorders and has a precise definition [2325]. The core of ADHD-hyperactive symptoms are poor sustained attention, deficient impulse control (impulsivity), and excessive activity level [24, 2629]. Thus, this qualification of “hyperactive” is excessive due to the absence of primary attention problems in many of these children, raising the fundamental question of the role of their apparent excessive mobility.
Extensive neuroimaging data highlight the interconnection between cognitive capacities and the sensorimotor state [30]. Human posture and/or mobility governs both neurophysiological arousal [3133] and cognitive performance [3438]. In healthy adults, Barra et al. [39] showed that increased body swaying related to imposed postures improved the alerting performance without modulating the orienting and inhibition function. Therefore, contrary to common thinking, mobility does not always seem to be a source of distraction leading to a lack of concentration. For instance, Janssen et al. [40] showed that the implementation of a moderate intensity physical activity break during the school day enhances attention levels, thereby improving school performance. Beyond this exercise-facilitated cognition, several studies suggest that children working in classrooms equipped with desks that allow standing and movement during class time led to significant improvement in their attention, executive control, and working memory [4144].

Aim of the study

The purpose of the present study is to determine the influence of mobility on the attention components that may impact learning in healthy children aged 7-years born at term or prematurely. We hypothesize that the absence of mobility constraints may improve alerting performance by increasing arousal in children born at term, without influencing orienting or inhibition function. We also speculate that considering mobility in children born prematurely might help improve some of their attention abilities.

Methods and design

This prospective, randomized, controlled trial will focus on psychometric testing of the attention components and will involve a mixed measurement design. The study will take place in the Maternité Régionale of CHRU Nancy. It will be conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. It was approved by the Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud-Est III Ethics Committee (2017–010 B) and registered in the clinicaltrial.​gov registry (NCT 03125447). Because the participants will be children, the signed consent of their parents will be requested after they have received written information related to the study. The children will also be asked for their oral consent. Data collected will be analyzed anonymously.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Children aged 7-years will be included in the study. Half will be children who were born prematurely and the other half will be children born at term. For the preterm group, children were born prematurely at or before 34 weeks gestation. They were born and cared for at our level III institution and involved in our routine follow-up program. At the time of the 7 years follow-up visit parents and child will be informed about the study and asked for participation. When they agree to participate an appointment will be taken for the study. For the term group, the children will be recruited using an information leaflet displayed at Lorraine University.
All children will have a clinical examination performed by trained pediatricians. General information on children’s health, socio-demographic data, behavioral problems, vision screening at the time of the test as well as perinatal information will be recorded. These features will be taken into account as potential confounding factors. All children with visual, cognitive, or motor disabilities that would prevent the realization of the test will be excluded. Also, infants with ADHD-inattentive problems will not be part of this study.

Materials

The Child ANT [12] will be generated by the software, E-Prime (version 3.0 professional; Psychological Software Tools®, Sharpsburg PA, USA) and presented through a head-mounted display (Wear Video Headphones, The Way In®, Vuzix Corporation, New York, USA).

Design and setting of the study

The head-mounted display will be used to keep the distance between the eyes and the visual stimuli constant across 3 experimental conditions (seated, standing, and free position). The visual target stimulus consists of a yellow fish placed in the center of the visual blue background that is oriented toward the left or right side (Fig.1). This target will be presented either above or below a fixed cross and with or without flanker stimuli. The target will appear either alone (neutral condition) or in the center of a horizontal row of five yellow flanking fishes who will be oriented in the same direction (congruent condition) or in the opposite direction (incongruent condition). Children will be instructed to identify, as quickly and accurately as possible, the direction of the central fish by pressing the right or the left mouse button whatever the direction of the possible flanking fishes. The children will use their preferred hand. Each fish is subtended 1.6 degrees of visual angle and the contours of adjacent fish are separated by 0.2 degrees. The five fish are subtended a total of 8.8 degrees. The target will be presented about 1 degree above or below fixation.
Each target will be preceded by one of the following four warning cues (asterisk) conditions, as illustrated in Fig. 1: (1) no cue with only the fixation cross displayed; (2) a center cue presented at the location of the fixation cross; (3) a double cue, appearing simultaneously 1 degree above and 1 degree below the fixation cross then the target appears at the level of only one of these two cues; or (4) a spatial cue, appearing 1 degree above or 1 degree below the fixation cross, then the target appears at the location of the cue. Each trial will begin with a fixation period of random duration (400–1600 ms). After that fixation period, the warning cue will be presented for 100 ms and will be followed by another fixation period of 400 ms. subsequently, the target and flankers will appear simultaneously. They will be presented until the child responds. The maximal response time allowed will be 2500 ms. After the response, the target and flankers will disappear, and there will be a last fixation period of 3500 ms minus the response time (RT). Then, the next trial can begin.
To test the influence of mobility on the level of the three components of attention, we will ask the children to complete the experimental task in three random positions: (a) in a fixed seated position corresponding to a posture with very low mobility similar to the demand of sitting in a school environment; (b) standing in an upright position corresponding to the human’s reference posture, requiring real balance control due to the natural body sway; (c) in a free position where the children will be able to move and change their position whenever and as often as they want.
To check the children’s understanding of ANT, a 12-trial practice block, lasting less than 2 min, will be executed in the seated position. The children will receive feedback on their success. After this practice block, they will execute 48 trials in each position (with a 1-min 30-s break after 24 trials): 4 cue conditions × 2 target locations (up, down) × 2 target directions (left, right) × 3 flanker conditions (neutral, congruent, incongruent). The order of the trials will be randomized. Overall, each experimental block will last less than 3 min. Between each experimental block, the children will have a 3-min break. During these breaks, they will rate the subjective dimension of their arousal based on the adapted Self-Assessment Manikin scale [45]. They will point to one of five figures on a teddy bears’ scale (Fig.2), or between any two figures, which results in a 9-point rating scale. Overall, the experiment will last about 30 min.

Data acquisition

To control for the position instructions, the experiment will be video recorded and an observation sheet completed for each experimental block.
During the Child ANT, the success and RT will be recorded for each trial. According to Fan et al. [10], the level of each attention components in each position will be computed from the RT difference of correct responses between pairs of specific trials. The alerting effect will be evaluated by subtracting the median RT of all double cue conditions for each child from the median RT of the no cue condition across the flanker conditions. The orienting effect will be evaluated by subtracting the median RT of all spatial cue conditions from the median RT of all center cue conditions across the flanker conditions. The inhibition function effect will be evaluated by subtracting the median RT of congruent flanking conditions from the median RT of incongruent flanking conditions across cue conditions.

Statistical analyses

To determine the number of children to include, we relied upon Rueda et al. [12], showing a global sitting performance of an overall RT of 931 ± 42 ms in 6-years-old children born at term and 833 ± 123 ms in 7-years-old children born at term. Because children born prematurely are usually considered to have about a 1-year delay for learning abilities, we calculated that to demonstrate a catch-up related to the mobility condition, sitting being the reference, for each attention component, with an alpha risk of 0.00625 (Bonferroni correction for the number of tests) and a power of 0.80, 24 children would be needed in each group (Power and Precision™ V4, Biostat Inc., Englewood, NJ, USA 2001).
Thus, we will first compare the overall RT of the children born at term versus those born prematurely. Then, we will compare separately the mean scores (± standard deviation) obtained for each of the attention components. After having verified the required assumptions about data distributions (normality of attentional scores, homoscedasticity and sphericity), the level of each attention component will be analyzed by the means of three mixed analyses of variance, with position condition (seated, standing, free) as a within-subject factor and gestational age (children born preterm vs. children born at term) as a between-subject factor. For all analyses, post hoc tests will be conducted using Tukey’s honestly significant difference method when needed.
To evaluate the subjective arousal level of children between the positions, the Friedman test will be performed on each score on the Self-Assessment Manikin scale with position (seated, upright, and free) as a within-subject factor.
The statistical thresholds for significance will be set to 0.05 for the remaining analyses.

Discussion

The consequences of child mobility during learning are a recurrent concern for parents and teachers. In general, a behavior with a high level of mobility is perceived as the expression of a lack of concentration, and consequently a lack of performance. This study will reassess the contribution of mobility expression in specific attentional contexts that are usually present during fundamental learning in children aged 7-years.
Numerous findings suggest that mobility is not always a source of distraction [39, 4649]. The work of Stoffregen’s team [48, 49] suggests that during the accomplishment of a supra-postural cognitive task such as calculating or memorizing, the organism may generate a spontaneous body sway to facilitate the performance of the associated supra-postural task. The modulation of self-generated body motions may correspond to unintentional attempts to increase arousal. This would be enabled by the increase in physiological parameters leading to greater cerebral activation, hence facilitating information processing [40, 5052]. This heuristic assumption results from the U-inverted model of Yerkes and Dodson [53], which proposes a progressive improvement in cognitive performance with a moderate increase in the arousal level until reaching a threshold of this energetic solicitation, when the cognitive performance progressively decreases.
Furthermore, the behavioral strategies in children, especially their mobility, should be considered in the analysis of their difficulties during class time. In other words, we speculate that the spontaneous mobility often observed in school children may reflect a behavioral strategy when he/she is engaged in learning activities with attentional overload. This possible reassessment of child mobility has potentially important implications for educational practices in order to facilitate the attentional performance in children. A new pedagogical format of teaching could be proposed, taking into account the child’s mobility. Also, new pedagogical tools that allow the child to have free mobility could be designed such as stand-biased school desks [4144]. Simple environmental changes in classrooms could enhance children’s cognitive functioning, driving their cognitive development and impacting educational outcomes. This could significantly improve learning abilities in children who were born preterm. These children are known to have poor or delayed development levels of attention. From the outcomes of this trial, educational innovations may be developed to help improve the adaptation to primary school in vulnerable children.

Acknowledgements

We are sincerely grateful to all the children and their parents for their participation. We would like to give special thanks to A.-F. André, C. Sauge and N. Tecquert for managing the children’s files. We also thank Professor N. Thilly from the Methodology, Data management and Statistics Unit of the PARC of Nancy University Hospital for her help and supervision of the study design.

Funding

This research is funded by the Université de Lorraine “Soutien à des Actions de Recherches – Crédits SC-UL 2017”.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.
This study will be conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. It was approved by the Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud-Est III Ethics Committee (2017–010 B) and registered on the clinicaltrial.​gov registry (NCT 03125447). Written informed consent will be obtained from the parents according to the protocol approved by the ethics committees. In this study, no child will be exposed to painful situations.
The individual will give consent to allow data from the registers to be used for research publication purposes before participation. Data collected will be treated confidentially. No individual details will be included in the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78:647–63.CrossRefPubMed Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78:647–63.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Bull R, Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;19:273–93.CrossRefPubMed Bull R, Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;19:273–93.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Clair-Thompson HLS, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2006;59:745–59.CrossRef Clair-Thompson HLS, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2006;59:745–59.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2004;18:1–16.CrossRef Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2004;18:1–16.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lorsbach TC, Wilson S, Reimer JF. Memory for relevant and irrelevant information: evidence for deficient inhibitory processes in language/learning disabled children. Contemp Educ Psychol. 1996;21:447–66.CrossRefPubMed Lorsbach TC, Wilson S, Reimer JF. Memory for relevant and irrelevant information: evidence for deficient inhibitory processes in language/learning disabled children. Contemp Educ Psychol. 1996;21:447–66.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Jensen J. Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:171–7.CrossRefPubMed Ozonoff S, Jensen J. Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:171–7.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Swanson HL. Reading comprehension and working memory in learning-disabled readers: is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;72:1–31.CrossRefPubMed Swanson HL. Reading comprehension and working memory in learning-disabled readers: is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;72:1–31.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42.CrossRefPubMed Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340–7.CrossRefPubMed Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340–7.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Atten Percept Psychophys. 1974;16:143–9.CrossRef Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Atten Percept Psychophys. 1974;16:143–9.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1029–40.CrossRefPubMed Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1029–40.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N. Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:393–421.CrossRefPubMed Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N. Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:393–421.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat van de Weijer-Bergsma E, Wijnroks L, Jongmans MJ. Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: a review. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31:333–51.CrossRefPubMed van de Weijer-Bergsma E, Wijnroks L, Jongmans MJ. Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: a review. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31:333–51.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayless S, Stevenson J. Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83:247–54.CrossRefPubMed Bayless S, Stevenson J. Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83:247–54.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Katz KS, Dubowitz LM, Henderson S, Jongmans M, Kay GG, Nolte CA, et al. Effect of cerebral lesions on continuous performance test responses of school age children born prematurely. J Pediatr Psychol. 1996;21:841–55.CrossRefPubMed Katz KS, Dubowitz LM, Henderson S, Jongmans M, Kay GG, Nolte CA, et al. Effect of cerebral lesions on continuous performance test responses of school age children born prematurely. J Pediatr Psychol. 1996;21:841–55.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Atkinson J, Braddick O. Visual and visuocognitive development in children born very prematurely. In: Rosander C von H and K, editor. Prog. Brain Res. Elsevier; 2007. p. 123–49. Atkinson J, Braddick O. Visual and visuocognitive development in children born very prematurely. In: Rosander C von H and K, editor. Prog. Brain Res. Elsevier; 2007. p. 123–49.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Foreman N, Fielder A, Minshell C, Hurrion E, Sergienko E. Visual search, perception, and visual–motor skill in “healthy” children born at 27–32 weeks’ gestation. J Exp Child Psychol. 1997;64:27–41.CrossRefPubMed Foreman N, Fielder A, Minshell C, Hurrion E, Sergienko E. Visual search, perception, and visual–motor skill in “healthy” children born at 27–32 weeks’ gestation. J Exp Child Psychol. 1997;64:27–41.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D. Others. Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2007;120:793–804.CrossRefPubMed Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D. Others. Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2007;120:793–804.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Pasman JW, Rotteveel JJ, Maassen B. Neurodevelopmental profile in low-risk preterm infants at 5 years of age. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 1998;2:7–17.CrossRefPubMed Pasman JW, Rotteveel JJ, Maassen B. Neurodevelopmental profile in low-risk preterm infants at 5 years of age. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 1998;2:7–17.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Leclercq V, Jambaqué I, Picard A, Bricout L, Siéroff É. Trouble du contrôle attentionnel et prématurité. Rev Neuropsychol. 2006;16:41–64. Leclercq V, Jambaqué I, Picard A, Bricout L, Siéroff É. Trouble du contrôle attentionnel et prématurité. Rev Neuropsychol. 2006;16:41–64.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkley RA, Poillion MJ. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Behav Disord. 1994;19:150–2.CrossRef Barkley RA, Poillion MJ. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Behav Disord. 1994;19:150–2.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Barkley RA, Mash EJ, editors. Treatment of childhood disorders. New York: The Guilford Press; 1998. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Barkley RA, Mash EJ, editors. Treatment of childhood disorders. New York: The Guilford Press; 1998.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkley RA. Hyperactive children: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Guilford press New York; 1981. Barkley RA. Hyperactive children: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Guilford press New York; 1981.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkley RA. The ecological validity of laboratory and analogue assessment methods of ADHD symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1991;19:149–78.CrossRefPubMed Barkley RA. The ecological validity of laboratory and analogue assessment methods of ADHD symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1991;19:149–78.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Douglas VI. Stop, look and listen: the problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Can J Behav Sci Can Sci Comport. 1972;4:259.CrossRef Douglas VI. Stop, look and listen: the problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Can J Behav Sci Can Sci Comport. 1972;4:259.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Douglas VI, Peters KG. Toward a clearer definition of the attentional deficit of hyperactive children. Atten. Cogn. Dev. Springer; 1979. p. 173–247. Douglas VI, Peters KG. Toward a clearer definition of the attentional deficit of hyperactive children. Atten. Cogn. Dev. Springer; 1979. p. 173–247.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Ross DM, Ross SA. Hyperactivity: Current issues, research, and theory. John Wiley & Sons; 1982. Ross DM, Ross SA. Hyperactivity: Current issues, research, and theory. John Wiley & Sons; 1982.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd Ed Revis. Wash. DC. 1987. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd Ed Revis. Wash. DC. 1987.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Thibault RT, Raz A. Imaging Posture Veils Neural Signals. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10 Thibault RT, Raz A. Imaging Posture Veils Neural Signals. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Rice JK, Rorden C, Little JS, Parra LC. Subject position affects EEG magnitudes. NeuroImage. 2013;64:476–84.CrossRefPubMed Rice JK, Rorden C, Little JS, Parra LC. Subject position affects EEG magnitudes. NeuroImage. 2013;64:476–84.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Spironelli C, Busenello J, Angrilli A. Supine posture inhibits cortical activity: Evidence from Delta and alpha EEG bands. Neuropsychologia. 2016;89:125–31.CrossRefPubMed Spironelli C, Busenello J, Angrilli A. Supine posture inhibits cortical activity: Evidence from Delta and alpha EEG bands. Neuropsychologia. 2016;89:125–31.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Thibault RT, Lifshitz M, Raz A. Body position alters human resting-state: insights from multi-postural magnetoencephalography. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;10:772–80.CrossRef Thibault RT, Lifshitz M, Raz A. Body position alters human resting-state: insights from multi-postural magnetoencephalography. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;10:772–80.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodenough DR, Oltman PK, Sigman E, Cox PW. The rod-and-frame illusion in erect and supine observers. Atten Percept Psychophys. 1981;29:365–70.CrossRef Goodenough DR, Oltman PK, Sigman E, Cox PW. The rod-and-frame illusion in erect and supine observers. Atten Percept Psychophys. 1981;29:365–70.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Lipnicki DM, Byrne DG. An effect of posture on anticipatory anxiety. Int J Neurosci. 2008;118:227–37.CrossRefPubMed Lipnicki DM, Byrne DG. An effect of posture on anticipatory anxiety. Int J Neurosci. 2008;118:227–37.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Mast FW, Ganis G, Christie S, Kosslyn SM. Four types of visual mental imagery processing in upright and tilted observers. Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:238–47.CrossRef Mast FW, Ganis G, Christie S, Kosslyn SM. Four types of visual mental imagery processing in upright and tilted observers. Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:238–47.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Messerotti Benvenuti S, Bianchin M, Angrilli A. Posture affects emotional responses: a head down bed rest and ERP study. Brain Cogn. 2013;82:313–8.CrossRefPubMed Messerotti Benvenuti S, Bianchin M, Angrilli A. Posture affects emotional responses: a head down bed rest and ERP study. Brain Cogn. 2013;82:313–8.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Muehlhan M, Marxen M, Landsiedel J, Malberg H, Zaunseder S. The effect of body posture on cognitive performance: a question of sleep quality. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:171. Muehlhan M, Marxen M, Landsiedel J, Malberg H, Zaunseder S. The effect of body posture on cognitive performance: a question of sleep quality. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:171.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Barra J, Auclair L, Charvillat A, Vidal M, Pérennou D. Postural control system influences intrinsic alerting state. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:226.CrossRefPubMed Barra J, Auclair L, Charvillat A, Vidal M, Pérennou D. Postural control system influences intrinsic alerting state. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:226.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Janssen M, Chinapaw MJM, Rauh SP, Toussaint HM, van Mechelen W, Verhagen EALM. A short physical activity break from cognitive tasks increases selective attention in primary school children aged 10–11. Ment Health Phys Act. 2014;7:129–34.CrossRef Janssen M, Chinapaw MJM, Rauh SP, Toussaint HM, van Mechelen W, Verhagen EALM. A short physical activity break from cognitive tasks increases selective attention in primary school children aged 10–11. Ment Health Phys Act. 2014;7:129–34.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Blake JJ, Benden ME, Wendel ML. Using stand/sit workstations in classrooms: lessons learned from a pilot study in Texas. J Public Health Manag Pract JPHMP. 2012;18:412–5.CrossRefPubMed Blake JJ, Benden ME, Wendel ML. Using stand/sit workstations in classrooms: lessons learned from a pilot study in Texas. J Public Health Manag Pract JPHMP. 2012;18:412–5.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Dornhecker M, Blake JJ, Benden M, Zhao H, Wendel M. The effect of stand-biased desks on academic engagement: an exploratory study. Int J Health Promot Educ. 2015;53:271–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Dornhecker M, Blake JJ, Benden M, Zhao H, Wendel M. The effect of stand-biased desks on academic engagement: an exploratory study. Int J Health Promot Educ. 2015;53:271–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Koepp GA, Snedden BJ, Flynn L, Puccinelli D, Huntsman B, Levine JA. Feasibility analysis of standing desks for sixth graders. ICAN Infant Child Adolesc Nutr. 2012;4:89–92.CrossRef Koepp GA, Snedden BJ, Flynn L, Puccinelli D, Huntsman B, Levine JA. Feasibility analysis of standing desks for sixth graders. ICAN Infant Child Adolesc Nutr. 2012;4:89–92.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Mehta RK, Shortz AE, Benden ME. Standing up for learning: a pilot investigation on the neurocognitive benefits of stand-biased school desks. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;13:59.CrossRefPubMedCentral Mehta RK, Shortz AE, Benden ME. Standing up for learning: a pilot investigation on the neurocognitive benefits of stand-biased school desks. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;13:59.CrossRefPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59.CrossRefPubMed Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Ceyte H, Lion A, Caudron S, Kriem B, Perrin PP, Gauchard GC. Does calculating impair postural stabilization allowed by visual cues? Exp Brain Res. 2014;232:2221–8.CrossRefPubMed Ceyte H, Lion A, Caudron S, Kriem B, Perrin PP, Gauchard GC. Does calculating impair postural stabilization allowed by visual cues? Exp Brain Res. 2014;232:2221–8.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Stins JF, Beek PJ. A critical evaluation of the cognitive penetrability of posture. Exp Aging Res. 2012;38:208–19.CrossRefPubMed Stins JF, Beek PJ. A critical evaluation of the cognitive penetrability of posture. Exp Aging Res. 2012;38:208–19.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoffregen TA, Hove P, Bardy BG, Riley M, Bonnet CT. Postural stabilization of perceptual but not cognitive performance. J Mot Behav. 2007;39:126–38.CrossRefPubMed Stoffregen TA, Hove P, Bardy BG, Riley M, Bonnet CT. Postural stabilization of perceptual but not cognitive performance. J Mot Behav. 2007;39:126–38.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ, Bardy BG, Pagulayan RJ. Postural stabilization of looking. J Exp Psychol. 1999;25:1641–58. Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ, Bardy BG, Pagulayan RJ. Postural stabilization of looking. J Exp Psychol. 1999;25:1641–58.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Caldwell JA, Prazinko B, Caldwell JL. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:23–31.CrossRefPubMed Caldwell JA, Prazinko B, Caldwell JL. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:23–31.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Brisswalter J, Legros P. Interactions entre les processus physiologiques et cognitifs: modèles théoriques et approche méthodologique. Sci Sports. 1996;11:71–80.CrossRef Brisswalter J, Legros P. Interactions entre les processus physiologiques et cognitifs: modèles théoriques et approche méthodologique. Sci Sports. 1996;11:71–80.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65.CrossRefPubMed Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Yerkes RM, Dodson JD. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol. 1908;18:459–82.CrossRef Yerkes RM, Dodson JD. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol. 1908;18:459–82.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Mobility may impact attention abilities in healthy term or prematurely born children at 7-years of age: protocol for an intervention controlled trial
verfasst von
Hadrien Ceyte
Joëlle Rosenbaum
Isabelle Hamon
Maëlle Wirth
Sébastien Caudron
Jean-Michel Hascoët
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1229-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

BMC Pediatrics 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.