Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research

Motor function and perception in children with neuropsychiatric and conduct problems: results from a population based twin study

verfasst von: Peik Gustafsson, Nóra Kerekes, Henrik Anckarsäter, Paul Lichtenstein, Christopher Gillberg, Maria Råstam

Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Background

Children with early symptomatic psychiatric disorders such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have been found to have high rates of motor and/or perception difficulties. However, there have been few large-scale studies reporting on the association between Conduct Disorder (CD) and motor/perception functions. The aim of the present study was to investigate how motor function and perception relate to measures of ADHD, ASD, and CD.

Methods

Parents of 16,994 Swedish twins (ages nine and twelve years) were interviewed using the Autism-Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities inventory (A-TAC), which has been validated as a screening instrument for early onset child psychiatric disorders and symptoms. Associations between categorical variables of scoring above previously validated cut-off values for diagnosing ADHD, ASD, and CD on the one hand and motor and/or perception problems on the other hand were analysed using cross-tabulations, and the Fisher exact test. Associations between the continuous scores for ADHD, ASD, CD, and the subdomains Concentration/Attention, Impulsiveness/Activity, Flexibility, Social Interaction and Language, and the categorical factors age and gender, on the one hand, and the dependent dichotomic variables Motor control and Perception problems, on the other hand, were analysed using binary logistic regression in general estimated equation models.

Results

Male gender was associated with increased risk of Motor control and/or Perception problems. Children scoring above the cut-off for ADHD, ASD, and/or CD, but not those who were ‘CD positive’ but ‘ADHD/ASD negative’, had more Motor control and/or Perception problems, compared with children who were screen-negative for all three diagnoses. In the multivariable model, CD and Impulsiveness/Activity had no positive associations with Motor control and/or Perception problems.

Conclusions

CD symptoms or problems with Impulsiveness/Activity were associated with Motor control or Perception problems only in the presence of ASD symptoms and/or symptoms of inattention. Our results indicate that children with CD but without ASD or inattention do not show a deviant development of motor and perceptual functions. Therefore, all children with CD should be examined concerning motor control and perception. If problems are present, a suspicion of ADHD and/or ASD should be raised.
Hinweise

Competing interests

All authors declare that we have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

PG was responsible for the design of the study, performed statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. NK participated in the design of the study, performed statistical analyses and helped to draft the manuscript. HA, PL, CG and MR came with advice when interpreting and describing the results and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
ADHD
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
ASD
Autism Spectrum Disorder
A-TAC
The Autism-Tics ADHD and other Comorbidities inventory
CATSS
Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden
CD
Conduct Disorder
CI
confidence interval
DAMP
Deficits in Attention Motor control and Perception
DCD
Developmental Coordination Disorder
DSM-IV/5
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition/5th edition
GEE
General Estimated Equation models
MBD
Minimal Brain Dysfunction/Damage
OR
odds ratio
SPECT
Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography.

Background

Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) have been shown to have high rates of motor and perception difficulties in controlled studies[14]. Before the introduction of the concept of ADHD, other diagnostic terms, such as Minimal Brain Dysfunction/Damage (MBD), were used to cover children with attention deficit, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. During the 1950s, MBD was believed to result from multiple small brain lesions and was often considered to be reflected as motor and perception difficulties[5, 6]. MBD[6] was often regarded as a form of ‘minimal cerebral palsy’. The currently used diagnosis of ADHD is defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV)[7]. DSM-IV lists nine criteria for inattention and nine criteria for hyperactivity-impulsivity (Table 1). To be diagnosed with ADHD, six or more of the criteria for inattention and/or six or more of the criteria for hyperactivity-impulsivity should be fulfilled.
Table 1
Criteria for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV)
Inattention:
Often fails to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes in schoolwork, work or other activities
Often has difficulty sustaining attention in tasks or play activities
Often does not seem to listen when spoken to directly
Often does not follow through on instructions and fails to finish schoolwork, chores or duties in the workplace
Often has difficulty organizing tasks and activities
Often avoids, dislikes, or is reluctant to engage in tasks that require sustained mental effort
Often loses things necessary for tasks and activities
Is Often easily distracted by extraneous stimuli
Is Often forgetful in daily activities
Hyperactivity/Impulsivity:
Often fidgets with hands or feet or squirms in seat
Often leaves seat in classroom or in other situations in which remaining seated is expected
Often runs about or climbs excessively in situations in which it is inappropriate
Often has difficulty playing or engaging in leisure activities quietly
Is Often ‘on the go’ or Often acts as if ‘driven by a motor’
Often talks excessively
Often blurts out answers before questions have been completed
Often has difficulty awaiting turn
Often interrupts or intrudes on others
In the 1980s, Gillberg and Gillberg[8] introduced the concept of Deficits in Attention, Motor control and Perception (DAMP) to describe the coexistence/‘comorbidity’ of ADHD and motor and/or perception problems, later often subsumed under the label of Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD)[7]. The diagnostic label of DCD is applied to individuals who are impaired by motor coordination difficulties that interfere significantly with their academic achievements or activities in everyday life. Several Swedish studies have shown that about half of all children with ADHD also meet the criteria for DCD and that these children have poorer outcomes than those with either ADHD only or DCD only[911]. Children with ADHD comorbid with DCD have, for instance, more cognitive (including language) problems and autistic symptoms, than children with ADHD without DCD[12]. Children with ASD have also been shown to have an increased risk of motor dysfunction/DCD and perception dysfunction[1319]. Moruzzi et al.[20] have shown additive genetic influences on clumsiness and autistic traits in a twin study. Sensory-motor problems, specifically perception-action coupling and temporal control in early infant development, have recently been suggested to be a key component in the progression of ASD[21]. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5)[22], sensory items have been added to the criteria for ASD. In a recent review article, Rogers et al.[23] describe a large number of human and animal studies providing support for the hypothesis that individuals with ASD might have cerebellar dysfunction and that the variability in the degree of such dysfunction is likely to be related to the severity of motor deficits in ASD.
Conduct Disorder (CD) is very common among children with ADHD[24] and not uncommon among children with ASD[25]. The most important predictors of CD are environmental factors such as maladaptive punitive parenting and parental antisocial behaviour[2628]. Genetic factors have also been shown to be of importance for CD[29]. CD and ADHD have been shown to be associated[30]. Martin et al.[31] have shown a positive association between hyperactivity and CD. Children with ADHD who develop CD at an early age (mostly boys) are a clinically important group who have a high risk of developing persistent antisocial behaviour during adolescence[8, 24].
Given the relationship between ADHD and CD, and the motor and/or perceptual dysfunction in ADHD, it seems likely that some of these symptoms would be present in CD. However, some questions remain as to whether only those children with co-occurring ADHD and CD would demonstrate these motor and/or perceptual problems, as well as the clinical significance of these symptoms in CD. We have only found a few studies describing the association between CD on one hand and motor and/or perceptual dysfunction on the other. In a population based follow-up study, Moffitt and Silva assessed children of 13 years of age and compared those with and without self-reported delinquency and with and without attention deficit disorder[32]. Children with self-reported delinquency had more visuospatial problems and problems with visuo-motor integration than control children. Among children with self-reported delinquency, the children with attention deficit disorder had more problems with visuo-motor integration than children without attention deficit disorder. Raine et al.[33] studied young adults with early neuromotor measures collected during the first year of life. They found that subjects with a combination of early neuromotor deficits and unstable family environment had the highest risk of showing violent and criminal behaviour, but they did not compare individuals with or without attention deficit disorder. In a later study[34], Raine et al. showed that individuals with persistent antisocial behaviour had a higher frequency of problems with spatial ability assessed at three-years of age compared to controls and individuals with non-persistent antisocial behaviour, and they made a hypothesis that early visuospatial (right hemisphere) impairments could interfere with early attachment, emotion recognition and emotional regulation leading to antisocial behaviour. In a study of criminal offenders, Raine et al.[35] showed that spatial as well as verbal impairments among these individuals were not completely attributable to ADHD. In a study concerning difficulties with executive planning of motor output and verbal ability among boys aged six to twelve years, Nigg et al.[36] found that these difficulties were better explained by ADHD-problems than by CD or reading disability, although they did not examine a pure group with CD without ADHD.
Clearly, more research is needed concerning the association between CD and motor problems/DCD and perception difficulties. Large-scale general population studies provide an excellent vehicle for studying such possible associations. In an earlier published article, we reported results including the heritability of motor problems, perception, ADHD, ASD, and CD from a large Swedish twin study[37].
The objectives of the present study were to examine the distribution of problems regarding motor function and perception and their relation to childhood neuropsychiatric and behavioural problems in a large population-based twin study, taking gender and age into consideration. We wanted to test the hypothesis that there is an association between CD and motor and/or perceptual problems even among children with CD without ADHD or ASD. We also wanted to test the hypothesis that ADHD (with its subtypes attention deficit and hyperactivity/impulsivity) and ASD (with its three components: rigid fixated behaviour, difficulties with social interaction and difficulties with communication) all are associated with motor and/or perceptual problems.

Methods

Subjects

The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS) is an on-going longitudinal twin study targeting all twins born in Sweden since 1 July 1992. Parents of twins are interviewed regarding their children’s somatic and mental health and social environment in connection with the children’s ninth or twelfth birthdays (CATSS-9/12). By January 2010, 8,610 parental interviews concerning 17,220 twins had been completed, with an overall response rate of 80% of all contacted families[37]. If one or both twins in a pair were affected with a severe brain disorder (documented brain damage, prevalence 0.9%, most commonly cerebral palsy) or known chromosomal aberration (prevalence 0.1%, most commonly Down’s syndrome), then that pair was excluded, leaving 16,994 children (8,666 boys and 6,439 girls). Of these, 10,555 were 9-year-olds, and 6,439 were 12-year-olds. Many of the twins were born prematurely; 29% had received neonatal care due to premature birth. Of the children included in the study, 0.5% had a diagnosis of mental retardation and further 0.5% attended a special school for developmentally delayed children.

The Autism-Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities inventory

The Autism-Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities (A-TAC) inventory used in the interviews was developed (originally for use in the Swedish twin study) with a view to screening for all types of child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. The items in the A-TAC are organised in theoretically defined modules based on DSM-IV categories and criteria. For each question the answer ‘no’ is coded as ‘0’ , ‘yes, to some extent’ is coded as ‘0.5’ and ‘yes’ is coded as ‘1’. The A-TAC has been validated in several studies and has been shown to be a sensitive screening tool for ASD, ADHD, tics, learning disorders, and DCD[38, 39]. It has been shown to function equally well in both genders (Kerekes N, Lundstrom S, Chang Z, Tajnia A, Jern P, Lichtenstein P, Nilsson T, Anckarsäter H; Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: neurodevelopmental predictors and genetic background in boys and girls in a nationwide twin study, submitted).
The ‘Motor control problem’ scale of the A-TAC initially comprised five items. In one of the validation studies, the item: ‘does he/she have problems with smooth coordination of movement?’ was found to have the same level of predictive power for the clinical diagnosis of DCD as did all the five items taken as a module score[38]. This single item was therefore used in the present study to screen for ‘Motor control problems’. A cut-off of ≥ 0.5 was used to define ‘DCD caseness’ (yielding a sensitivity of 0.59 and specificity of 0.85 in the validation study)[38].
The Perception scale of the A-TAC comprises five items, organised into one module. These five items are: ‘Does he/she seem disturbed by height differences such as in connection with climbing stairs, and so on?’ , ‘Does he/she have difficulty judging distance or size?’ , ‘Is he/she oversensitive to the touch of tight clothing?’ , ‘Is he/she particularly sensitive to certain sounds/noise?’ , and ‘Is he/she particularly sensitive to certain flavours, smells, or consistencies?’ Thus, the item ‘perception’ in the A-TAC contains questions concerning visual, auditory, sensory, olfactory and gustatory perception and, to some degree, proprio-sensibility (climbing stairs). To define ‘Perception disorder caseness’ , a cut-off of ≥ 2.5 points was used (yielding a sensitivity of 0.62 and a specificity of 0.93 in the validation study[38]). The internal consistency for the perception scale measured by Cronbach’s α has been shown to be 0.62.
The ADHD scale of the A-TAC consists of 19 items organised into two modules/subdomains: Concentration/Attention and Impulsiveness/Activity. The scale has strong internal consistency[37] and excellent predictive ability[38]. ‘ADHD caseness’ was defined by the validated cut-off ≥ 6.0 points on the ADHD scale (yielding a sensitivity of 0.98 and a specificity of 0.81 in the validation study). The internal consistency for the ADHD scale (Cronbach’s α) is 0.92.
The ASD scale of the A-TAC consists of 17 items organised into three modules/subdomains: Language, Social Interaction and Flexibility. The scale has both excellent internal consistency and predictive ability[37, 38]. ‘ASD caseness’ was defined in accordance with the validated cut-off ≥ 4.5 points on the ASD scale (yielding a sensitivity of 0.96 and a specificity of 0.88[38]). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) for the ASD scale is 0.86.
The CD scale of the A-TAC consists of five items, with good internal consistency[37] and excellent predictive ability. ‘CD caseness’ was defined here as a score of ≥ 2.0 (with a sensitivity of 0.55 and a specificity of 0.98) (Kerekes N, Lundstrom S, Chang Z, Tajnia A, Jern P, Lichtenstein P, Nilsson T, Anckarsäter H; Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: neurodevelopmental predictors and genetic background in boys and girls in a nationwide twin study, submitted). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) of the CD scale is 0.61.
The main modules/subdomains of the A-TAC scales were also analysed as continuous scale variables.

Statistical analysis

The associations of ADHD, ASD, or CD caseness on the one hand, and DCD and Perception disorder caseness on the other hand, were analysed using the Fisher exact test (two-way, three-way and four-way cross tabulations). The associations of defined continuous covariates and categorical cofactors with DCD and Perception disorder were studied using binary logistic regression analyses in General Estimated Equation (GEE) models, and the relationship between the twins in each pair was taken into consideration. DCD and Perception disorder constituted the dichotomous dependent variables. Gender as a co-factor had significant odds ratios (ORs) for DCD and Perception disorder; hence GEE models were analysed by gender. The Fisher r-to-z transformation was used to test the significance of the difference between the Spearman correlations for prematurely born and full term children.

Ethical consideration

The CATSS-9/12 study has ethical approval from the Karolinska Institute Ethical Review Board: Dnr 03–672 and 2010/507-31/1.

Results

Prevalence of DCD and Perception disorder caseness by ADHD, ASD and CD status

ADHD, ASD and, CD caseness was associated with much higher rates of DCD and Perception disorder caseness (Table 2). CD caseness in itself (without concomitant ADHD or ASD caseness) was not associated with DCD or Perception problems (Table 3). DCD caseness and Perception disorder caseness were much more common in ADHD plus ASD caseness (55% and 24%) than in ADHD caseness ‘only’ (18% and 3%, P < 0.001). Perception disorder caseness was also somewhat more common in ADHD caseness plus CD caseness than in ADHD caseness ‘only’ (5% versus 3%, P < 0.05).
Table 2
Cross tables showing the frequencies of Motor problems and Perception problems among children screen-positive for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Conduct Disorder (CD)
 
ADHD screen-negative cases
ADHD screen-positive cases
ASD screen-negative cases
ASD screen-positive cases
CD screen-negative cases
CD screen-positive cases
No Motor problems
14,449
1,218
15,426
249
15,143
530
94.4%
73.1%
93.7%
47.6%
92.80%
79.70%
Motor problems
854
448
1,034
274
1,177
135
5.6%
26.9%
6.3%
52.4%
7.2%
20.3%
Total
15,303
1,666
16,460
523
16,320
665
100%
100%
100%
100%
100%
100%
Cross-table significance
P < 0.001
P < 0.001
P < 0.001
 
ADHD screen-negative cases
ADHD screen-positive cases
ASD screen-negative cases
ASD screen-positive cases
CD screen-negative cases
CD screen-positive cases
No Perception problems
15,230
1,478
16,369
350
16,135
585
 
99.5%
88.7%
99.4%
66.9%
98.9%
88.0%
Perception problems
73
188
91
173
185
80
0.5%
11.3%
0.6%
33.1%
1.1%
12.0%
Total
15,303
1,666
16,460
523
16,320
665
100%
100%
100%
100%
100%
100%
Cross-table significance
P < 0.001
P < 0.001
P < 0.001
Column percentages are shown. Significances have been calculated according to Fisher’s exact test.
Table 3
Cross tables showing the frequencies of Motor and Perception problems among children in the following four categories: 1. Cases screen-negative for Conduct Disorder (CD); 2. Cases screen-positive for CD; 3. Cases screen-negative for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and CD, and 4. Cases screen-negative for both ADHD and ASD but screen-positive for CD
 
CD screen-negative cases
CD screen-positive cases
Cases screen-negative for ADHD, ASD and CD
Cases screen-negative for ADHD and ASD but screen-positive for CD
No Motor problems
15,143
530
14,101
271
92.8%
79.7%
94.7%
94.8%
Motor problems
1,177
135
787
15
7.2%
20.3%
5.3%
5.2%
Total
16,320
665
14,888
286
100%
100%
100%
100%
Cross-table significance
P < 0.001
P = 1
No Perception problems
16,135
585
14,839
283
98.9%
88.0%
99.7%
99.0%
Perception problems
185
80
49
3
1.1%
12.0%
0.3%
1.0%
Total
16,320
665
14,888
286
100%
100%
100%
100%
Cross-table significance
P < 0.001
P = 0.075
Column percentages are shown. Cross-table significances have been calculated according to Fisher’s exact test.
Children born prematurely have been shown to have a higher frequency of motor delays and motor difficulties than children born full term[40]. Since many children were born prematurely (29%) we compared the correlations between motor/perceptual problems and caseness for ADHD, ASD and CD for children born prematurely and children born full term. The correlation coefficients are given in Table 4. The only significant differences found between the correlation coefficients were those concerning the correlations between Perceptual disorder caseness and ASD caseness (P < 0.001), DCD caseness and CD caseness (P = 0.003) and Perceptual disorder caseness and CD caseness (P < 0.001). For perceptual disorder caseness we found that the correlation was stronger for full term children than for those born prematurely. Thus omitting prematurely born children from the analysis would strengthen the association between perceptual problems and ASD caseness. This shows that the strong correlations between caseness for ADHD and ASD and having motor and/or perceptual problems are not explicable only by the fact that many of the children were born prematurely.
Table 4
Spearman correlations between caseness for Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) and perception disorder on one hand and caseness for the different diagnoses (Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Conduct Disorder (CD)) on the other for children born preterm and full term born children
Correlations between caseness for:
Children born preterm
Full term born children
P
DCD/ADHD
rho = 0.25, n = 4,906
rho = 0.22, n = 12,016
0.12
DCD/ASD
rho = 0.30, n = 4,912
rho = 0.30, n = 12,024
0.92
DCD/CD
rho = 0.059, n = 4,912
rho = 0.11, n = 12,026
0.003
Perception disorder/ADHD
rho = 0.25, n = 4,906
rho = 0.26, n = 12,016
0.41
Perception disorder/ASD
rho = 0.42, n = 4,912
rho = 0.48, n = 12,024
< 0.001
Perception disorder/CD
rho = 0.078, n = 4,912
rho = 0.21, n = 12,026
< 0.001
P = significance of the difference between the correlations for children born preterm and full term born children.

Neurodevelopmental and behavioural problems associated with motor or perception dysfunction

Dimensional analyses with continuous A-TAC scores were used in the binary logistic regression analyses in GEE models and the dependence of each twin in a pair was taken into consideration. Male versus female gender was associated with an odds ratio (OR) of 1.86 (95% CI = 1.64 to 2.10) and 2.34 (95% CI = 1.77 to 3.09), in the prediction of Motor control and Perception problems respectively. Age was not associated with the risk of Motor control or Perception problems.
In univariable models, the A-TAC scores of ADHD, ASD, and CD, analysed as continuous scale variables, were significantly (P < 0.05) positively associated with both Motor control and Perception problems in both genders (Table 5). In the multivariable model, the CD-score no longer showed significant positive associations with Motor control or Perception problems in either gender.
Table 5
Results according to the General Estimated Equation (GEE) model concerning the associations between Motor problems and Perception problems and scores of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Conduct Disorder (CD)
BOYS
Motor problems
Perception problems
Variable
Crude measures
Univariable model
Multivariable modela
Univariable model
Multivariable model
 
N
Min to Max
Mean
SD.
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
ADHD
8,643
0 to 19
2.34
3.32
1.22d
1.20 to 1.24
1.12d
1.09 to 1.15
1.38d
1.34 to 1.42
1.12d
1.06 to 1.18
0.20d
0.18 to 0.22
0.11d
0.09 to 0.14
0.32d
0.30 to 0.35
0.11d
0.06 to 0.16
ASD
8,643
0 to 17
0.91
1.71
1.53d
1.46 to 1.60
1.38d
1.30 to 1.46
1.92d
1.81 to 2.04
1.74d
1.60 to 1.89
0.42d
0.38 to 0.47
0.32d
0.26 to 0.38
0.65d
0.59 to 0.71
0.55d
0.47 to 0.64
CD
8,643
0 to 5
0.11
0.38
1.86d
1.62 to 2.14
0.66d
0.54 to 0.80
3.12d
2.50 to 3.90
0.84
0.63 to 1.14
0.62d
0.48 to 0.76
-0.42d
-0.61 to -0.23
1.14d
0.92 to 1.36
-0.17
-0.47 to 0.13
GIRLS
Motor problems
Perception problems
ADHD
8,310
0 to 19
1.45
2.51
1.26d
1.23 to 1.29
1.15d
1.10 to 1.19
1.43d
1.37 to 1.49
1.22d
1.13 to 1.32
0.23d
0.21 to 0.26
0.14d
0.10 to 0.18
0.36d
0.32 to 0.40
0.20d
0.12 to 0.28
ASD
8,310
0 to 14.5
0.55
1.14
1.70d
1.60 to 1.82
1.48d
1.36 to 1.62
2.00d
1.82 to 2.20
1.62d
1.42 to 1.86
0.53d
0.47 to 0.60
0.40d
0.31 to 0.48
0.69d
0.60 to 0.79
0.49d
0.35 to 0.62
CD
8,310
0 to 4.5
0.06
0.28
2.22d
1.80 to 2.72
0.60c
0.45 to 0.81
3.70d
2.79 to 4.90
0.69
0.42 to 1.13
     
0.80d
0.59 to 1.00
-0.50c
-0.80 to -0.21
1.31d
1.03 to 1.59
-0.38
-0.87 to 0.12
aMultivariable models are controlled for age which in univariable models was not significantly associated with the dependent variables.
B coefficients and odds ratios (OR) for the univariable model and adjusted B coefficients and odds ratios for the multivariable model with their 95% confidence intervals (CI) are shown.
b0.01 ≤ P < 0.05; c0.001 ≤ P < 0.01; dP < 0.001.
ADHD = Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; ASD = Autism Spectrum Disorder; CD = Conduct Disorder.
The subdomains of ADHD (Concentration/Attention, Impulsiveness/Activity) and the subdomains of ASD (Language, Social Interaction and Flexibility) were analysed as continuous scale variables in a multivariable model together with the continuous CD-score (Table 6). In this analysis, the score of Impulsiveness/Activity and the score of CD had no significant positive associations with Motor control or Perception problems in either gender, and the score of Social Interaction had a significant (P < 0.05) association with Motor control problems only in boys. The strongest positive significant association in both genders was that between Flexibility and Motor control or Perception problems (Table 6).
Table 6
Results according to the General Estimated Equation (GEE) model concerning the associations between motor problems and perception problems and scores of Conduct Disorder (CD) and of the subdomains of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)
BOYS
Motor problems
Perception problems
Variable
Crude measures
Univariable model
Multivariable model
Univariable model
Multivariable model
 
N
Min to Max
Mean
 
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
    
SD
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
B
95% CI
Concentration and Attention
8,631
0 to 9
1.24
1.89
1.47d
1.43 to 1.52
1.33d
1.27 to 1.39
1.82d
1.73 to 1.92
1.30d
1.18 to 1.43
     
0.39d
0.36 to 0.42
0.28d
0.24 to 0.33
0.60d
0.55 to 0.65
0.26d
0.16 to 0.36
Impulsiveness and Activity
8,631
0 to 10
1.09
1.79
1.30d
1.27 to 1.35
0.93b
0.88 to 0.98
1.63d
1.55 to 1.71
0.97
0.88 to 1.08
     
0.27d
0.78 to 0.95
-0.07b
-0.12 to 0.02
0.49d
0.44 to 0.53
-0.03
-0.13 to 0.08
Flexibility
8,631
0 to 5
0.31
0.67
2.38d
2.19 to 2.60
1.34d
1.18 to 1.53
4.85d
4.23 to 5.56
2.33d
1.92 to 2.83
     
0.87d
0.78 to 0.95
0.30d
0.17 to 0.43
1.58d
1.44 to 1.72
0.84d
0.65 to 1.04
Social Interaction
8,631
0 to 6
0.31
0.67
2.35d
2.14 to 2.58
1.18b
1.01 to 1.37
3.99d
3.50 to 4.56
1.42c
1.15 to 1.75
     
0.85d
0.76 to 0.95
0.16b
0.01 to 0.31
1.39d
1.25 to 1.52
0.35c
0.14 to 0.56
Language
8,631
0 to 6
0.29
0.66
2.68d
2.44 to 2.95
1.63d
1.44 to 1.84
3.98d
3.49 to 4.53
1.49d
1.23 to 1.81
     
0.99d
0.89 to 1.08
0.49d
0.36 to 0.61
1.38d
1.25 to 1.51
0.40d
0.21 to 0.60
CD
8,631
0 to 5
0.11
0.38
1.86d
1.62 to 2.15
0.77b
0.64 to 0.94
3.16d
2.53 to 3.94
0.94
0.70 to 1.26
     
0.62d
0.48 to 0.76
-0.26b
-0.45 to -0.06
1.15d
0.93 to 1.37
-0.06
-0.36 to 0.23
GIRLS
Motor problems
Perception problems
Concentration and Attention
8,296
0 to 9
0.73
1.43
1.54d
1.47 to 1.60
1.38d
1.29 to 1.47
1.97d
1.82 to 2.12
1.57d
1.38 to 1.77
     
0.43d
0.38 to 0.47
0.32d
0.25 to 0.38
0.68
0.60 to 0.75
0.45
0.33 to 0.57
Impulsiveness and Activity
8,296
0 to 10
0.71
1.40
1.36d
1.30 to 1.42
0.95
0.88 to 1.03
1.68d
1.56 to 1.80
0.96
0.82 to 1.12
     
0.31d
0.26 to 0.35
-0.05
-0.13 to 0.03
0.52
0.45 to 0.59
-0.04
-0.20 to 0.11
Flexibility
8,296
0 to 5
0.16
0.45
2.99d
2.62 to 3.42
1.79d
1.50 to 2.15
4.70d
3.87 to 5.70
1.78d
1.33 to 2.37
     
1.10d
0.96 to 1.23
0.58d
0.40 to 0.77
1.55
1.35 to 1.74
0.58
0.29 to 0.86
Social Interaction
8,296
0 to 5.5
0.20
0.50
2.59d
2.28 to 2.93
1.17
0.95 to 1.44
4.30d
3.62 to 5.10
1.54c
1.11 to 2.14
     
0.95
0.83 to 1.08
0.16
-0.06 to 0.37
1.46
1.29 to 1.63
0.43
0.11 to 0.76
Language
8296
0 to 5
0.18
0.46
2.88d
2.52 to 3.30
1.54d
1.29 to 1.83
4.22d
3.42 to 5.21
1.49b
1.06 to 2.09
     
1.06d
0.92 to 1.19
0.43d
0.26 to 0.60
1.44
1.23 to 1.65
0.40
0.06 to 0.74
CD
8296
0 to 4.5
0.06
0.28
2.19d
1.78 to 2.69
0.65c
0.48 to 0.87
3.68d
2.77 to 4.89
0.74
0.44 to 1.22
     
0.78d
0.58 to 0.99
-0.43c
-0.73 to -0.14
1.30
1.02 to 1.59
-0.31
-0.81 to 0.20
aMultivariable models are controlled for age which in univariable models was not significantly associated with the dependent variables.
B coefficients and ORs for the univariable model and adjusted B coefficients and ORs for the multivariable model with their 95% CI are shown.
b0.01 ≤ P < 0.05; c0.001 ≤ P < 0.01; dP < 0.001.
CD = Conduct Disorder.

Discussion

This is probably the largest study ever performed on the relationship between symptoms relating to commonly diagnosed problems in child and adolescent psychiatry - ADHD, ASD, and, CD - and motor control and perception problems - commonly encountered but often not separately diagnosed, either as DCD or under any other label. ADHD and ASD, as ascertained on the basis of parent telephone interviews, showed strong and independent associations with such DCD-related problems, but CD without ‘comorbid’ ADHD or ASD did not. Associations were established both when phenomena were studied on a categorical basis and when they were considered from the point of view of continuous distributions. Parents were interviewed using the A-TAC, an instrument which is used for easy and quick detection of childhood psychiatric problems[41]. Data from 16,994 parent interviews were used, which gives the study good statistical power.
The hypothesis that there is an association between CD and motor and/or perceptual problems was not supported by our study. The results seem to indicate that there are different forms of CD. One type of CD is associated with attention deficit and/or autistic symptoms and these children have a high frequency of motor and/or perceptual problems indicating a more ‘organic’ kind of disturbance. Another type of CD is associated with hyperactive and impulsive behaviour and children in this group have a low frequency of motor and/or perceptual problems and neither attention deficit nor autistic symptoms. Children with CD without either ADHD-symptoms or autistic symptoms also have a low frequency of motor and/or perceptual problems. Thus motor and/or perceptual problems among children with CD should lead a clinician to suspect comorbid attention deficit and/or autistic symptoms which might be of importance in efforts to find the right treatment and support.
ADHD is often argued to be a heterogeneous condition with different subtypes; for example, the DAMP subtype of ADHD with motor and perception problems[3]. Lambek et al.[42] have described one group of children with lower IQ and an executive function deficit, and another group with normal IQ and no signs of an executive function deficit but with delay aversion (difficulty waiting for a reward), which can be regarded as a kind of lack of impulse control. Our study seems to indicate the existence of one group of children with high scores of ADHD but low scores of ASD and without Motor control or Perception problems, some of them with high scores of Impulsiveness/Activity and CD; and another group of children with high scores of ADHD and ASD but with low scores of Impulsiveness/Activity and CD and with Motor control and Perception problems. ASD has also been suggested to be heterogeneous in the sense that the three symptom groups in the ‘triad’ of ASD, that is social interaction difficulties, communication difficulties, and behaviour symptoms, have been shown to have limited genetic and environmental overlap, the so called ‘fractionable autism triad hypothesis’[43]. Robinson et al.[43] have performed a twin study where they could show that the majority of genetic influences on autism-like behaviours are distinct to each phenotypical component, thus giving support to the ‘fractionable autism triad hypothesis’. Different components of the ASD phenotype might be associated with different etiologic factors. Since many studies indicate that both ADHD and ASD can be regarded as heterogeneous conditions, we decided to analyse the different symptoms of ADHD and ASD separately. Of the two subdomains of ADHD (Concentration/Attention and Impulsiveness/Activity) only Concentration/Attention had significant associations with Motor control and Perception problems, whereas all three subdomains of ASD (Flexibility, Social Interaction and Language) were associated with perceptual problems in both boys and girls and with motor problems in boys and girls, with the exception of Social Interaction in girls.
Cerebellar function is of importance for motor function and seems to be of importance in ASD as well as in ADHD[23, 44]. The association between CD and cerebellar function among children without comorbid ADHD or ASD has not been studied. A Swedish study of regional cerebral blood flow using SPECT (Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography) in children with ADHD showed two different non-correlated patterns; one associated with motor impairment and cognitive function (low blood flow in the cerebellum and temporal lobes relative to the basal ganglia and thalamus) and one associated with deviant behaviour (low blood flow in the frontal and parietal areas)[45]. One might hypothesise that the children in our study with a high score of ADHD, a low score of ASD and who had no Motor control problems may have cerebellar function more comparable with children without a neurodevelopmental disorder, whereas the other group with high scores of ADHD and ASD and with Motor control and Perception problems may have relative cerebellar dysfunction. The first group may share some characteristics with the group without an executive function deficit described by Lambek et al.[42], while the other group may share characteristics with the group with lower IQ and an executive function deficit.

Limitations

The assessment of Motor control and Perception problems and of screen positivity for ADHD, ASD and, CD was based on information from parent interviews. In this study, we could not examine the children or obtain information from the teacher as in a thorough clinical assessment. Depending on only one subjective source of information is a weakness. The parents’ description of the child’s motor and perception functions and the result of a neurological examination of the child have been shown to have a low to moderate degree of correlation[46]. On the other hand, the parental interview used in the present study, the A-TAC, has shown excellent diagnostic utility for identifying ADHD and fair diagnostic utility for diagnosing DCD[39]. The study is a part of a national twin study. Although twins are not completely representative of the normal population in terms of mental health problems and psychomotor development, and some bias may be introduced by this, most studies that have examined this issue have found no or very small differences between twins and singletons[47].

Conclusions

Meeting criteria for CD caseness was positively associated with Motor control and/or Perception problems only in individuals who simultaneously met criteria for ADHD caseness and/or ASD caseness. High scores of the continuous variables Impulsiveness/Activity or CD in the absence of high scores of any of the variables Concentration/Attention, Flexibility, Social Interaction, or Language were not associated with Motor control and/or Perception problems. Thus, CD symptoms or problems with Impulsiveness/Activity seem to be associated with Motor control or Perception problems only in the presence of ASD symptoms and/or symptoms of inattention. This seems to indicate that children with CD without ASD or inattention do not show a deviant development of motor and perceptual functions. Children with CD should be examined concerning motor function and perception, and a suspicion of attention deficit disorder and/or ASD should be raised if the child is found to have motor and/or perceptual problems.
Written informed consent was obtained from the patient’s guardian/parent/next of kin for the publication of this report.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited.

Competing interests

All authors declare that we have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

PG was responsible for the design of the study, performed statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. NK participated in the design of the study, performed statistical analyses and helped to draft the manuscript. HA, PL, CG and MR came with advice when interpreting and describing the results and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Rasmussen P, Gillberg C, Waldenström E, Svenson B: Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children: neurological and neurodevelopmental aspects. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1983, 25: 315-333.CrossRefPubMed Rasmussen P, Gillberg C, Waldenström E, Svenson B: Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children: neurological and neurodevelopmental aspects. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1983, 25: 315-333.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartsough CS, Lambert NM: Medical factors in hyperactive and normal children: prenatal, developmental, and health history findings. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1985, 55: 190-201.CrossRefPubMed Hartsough CS, Lambert NM: Medical factors in hyperactive and normal children: prenatal, developmental, and health history findings. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1985, 55: 190-201.CrossRefPubMed
3.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillberg C: The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations. Res Dev Disabil. 2010, 31: 1543-1551. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002.CrossRefPubMed Gillberg C: The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations. Res Dev Disabil. 2010, 31: 1543-1551. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Strauss AA, Lehtinen LE: Psychopathology and Education of the Brain-Injured Child. 1947, New York: Grune and Stratton Strauss AA, Lehtinen LE: Psychopathology and Education of the Brain-Injured Child. 1947, New York: Grune and Stratton
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Knobloch H, Rider R, Harper P, Pasamanick B: Neuropsychiatric sequelae of prematurity; a longitudinal study. JAMA. 1956, 161: 581-585. 10.1001/jama.1956.02970070013004.CrossRef Knobloch H, Rider R, Harper P, Pasamanick B: Neuropsychiatric sequelae of prematurity; a longitudinal study. JAMA. 1956, 161: 581-585. 10.1001/jama.1956.02970070013004.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat APA: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1994, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 4 APA: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1994, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 4
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillberg IC, Gillberg C: Children with deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP): need for specialist treatment. Acta Paediatr Scand. 1988, 77: 450-451. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1988.tb10678.x.CrossRefPubMed Gillberg IC, Gillberg C: Children with deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP): need for specialist treatment. Acta Paediatr Scand. 1988, 77: 450-451. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1988.tb10678.x.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kadesjö B, Gillberg C: Developmental Coordination Disorder in Swedish 7-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 38: 820-828. 10.1097/00004583-199907000-00011.CrossRefPubMed Kadesjö B, Gillberg C: Developmental Coordination Disorder in Swedish 7-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 38: 820-828. 10.1097/00004583-199907000-00011.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillberg C, Rasmussen P, Carlström G, Svenson B, Waldenström E: Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in six-year-old children. Epidemiological aspects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1982, 23: 131-144. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1982.tb00058.x.CrossRefPubMed Gillberg C, Rasmussen P, Carlström G, Svenson B, Waldenström E: Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in six-year-old children. Epidemiological aspects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1982, 23: 131-144. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1982.tb00058.x.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Landgren M, Pettersson R, Kjellman B, Gillberg C: ADHD, DAMP and other neurodevelopmental/neuropsychiatric disorders in six-year-old children. Epidemiology and comorbidity. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1996, 38: 891-906.CrossRefPubMed Landgren M, Pettersson R, Kjellman B, Gillberg C: ADHD, DAMP and other neurodevelopmental/neuropsychiatric disorders in six-year-old children. Epidemiology and comorbidity. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1996, 38: 891-906.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillberg C, Kadesjo B: Why bother about clumsiness? The implications of having developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Neural Plast. 2003, 10: 59-68.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Gillberg C, Kadesjo B: Why bother about clumsiness? The implications of having developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Neural Plast. 2003, 10: 59-68.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghaziuddin M, Butler E, Tsai L, Ghaziuddin N: Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger syndrome?. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1994, 38: 519-527.CrossRefPubMed Ghaziuddin M, Butler E, Tsai L, Ghaziuddin N: Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger syndrome?. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1994, 38: 519-527.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Manjiviona J, Prior M: Comparison of Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autistic children on a test of motor impairment. J Autism Dev Disord. 1995, 25: 23-39. 10.1007/BF02178165.CrossRefPubMed Manjiviona J, Prior M: Comparison of Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autistic children on a test of motor impairment. J Autism Dev Disord. 1995, 25: 23-39. 10.1007/BF02178165.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghaziuddin M, Weidmer-Mikhail E, Ghaziuddin N: Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome: a preliminary report. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1998, 42: 279-283.CrossRefPubMed Ghaziuddin M, Weidmer-Mikhail E, Ghaziuddin N: Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome: a preliminary report. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1998, 42: 279-283.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Noterdaeme M, Mildenberger K, Minow F, Amorosa H: Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002, 11: 219-225.CrossRefPubMed Noterdaeme M, Mildenberger K, Minow F, Amorosa H: Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002, 11: 219-225.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Freitag CM, Kleser C, Schneider M, von Gontard A: Quantitative assessment of neuromotor function in adolescents with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37: 948-959. 10.1007/s10803-006-0235-6.CrossRefPubMed Freitag CM, Kleser C, Schneider M, von Gontard A: Quantitative assessment of neuromotor function in adolescents with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37: 948-959. 10.1007/s10803-006-0235-6.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Mostofsky SH, Powell SK, Simmonds DJ, Goldberg MC, Caffo B, Pekar JJ: Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor task performance. Brain. 2009, 132: 2413-2425. 10.1093/brain/awp088.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Mostofsky SH, Powell SK, Simmonds DJ, Goldberg MC, Caffo B, Pekar JJ: Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor task performance. Brain. 2009, 132: 2413-2425. 10.1093/brain/awp088.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Moruzzi S, Ogliari A, Ronald A, Happé F, Battaglia M: The nature of covariation between autistic traits and clumsiness: a twin study in a general population sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011, 41: 1665-1674. 10.1007/s10803-011-1199-8.CrossRefPubMed Moruzzi S, Ogliari A, Ronald A, Happé F, Battaglia M: The nature of covariation between autistic traits and clumsiness: a twin study in a general population sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011, 41: 1665-1674. 10.1007/s10803-011-1199-8.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Whyatt C, Craig C: Sensory-motor problems in Autism. Front Integr Neuroscience. 2013, 7 (51): 1-12. Whyatt C, Craig C: Sensory-motor problems in Autism. Front Integr Neuroscience. 2013, 7 (51): 1-12.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat APA: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2013, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 5 APA: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2013, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 5
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers TD, McKimm E, Dickson PE, Goldowitz D, Blaha CD, Mittleman G: Is autism a disease of the cerebellum? An integration of clinical and pre-clinical research. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013, 7: 15-e-pub ahead of print.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Rogers TD, McKimm E, Dickson PE, Goldowitz D, Blaha CD, Mittleman G: Is autism a disease of the cerebellum? An integration of clinical and pre-clinical research. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013, 7: 15-e-pub ahead of print.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Mick E: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. Ambul Pediatr. 2007, 7 (Suppl 1): 73-81.CrossRefPubMed Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Mick E: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. Ambul Pediatr. 2007, 7 (Suppl 1): 73-81.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Anckarsäter H, Larson T, Hansson SL, Carlström E, Ståhlberg O, Gillberg CI, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Lichtenstein P: Child neurodevelopmental and behavioural problems are intercorrelated and dimensionally distributed in the general population. Open Psychiatr J. 2008, 2: 5-11. 10.2174/1874354400802010005.CrossRef Anckarsäter H, Larson T, Hansson SL, Carlström E, Ståhlberg O, Gillberg CI, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Lichtenstein P: Child neurodevelopmental and behavioural problems are intercorrelated and dimensionally distributed in the general population. Open Psychiatr J. 2008, 2: 5-11. 10.2174/1874354400802010005.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi A, Moffit TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Tully L, Jacobs C, Kim-Cohen J, Polo-Tomas M: Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behaviour problems: using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioural development. Dev Psychol. 2004, 40: 149-161.CrossRefPubMed Caspi A, Moffit TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Tully L, Jacobs C, Kim-Cohen J, Polo-Tomas M: Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behaviour problems: using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioural development. Dev Psychol. 2004, 40: 149-161.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Stanger C, Dumenci L, Kamon J, Burstein M: Parenting and children’s externalizing problems in substance-abusing families. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004, 33: 590-600. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3303_16.CrossRefPubMed Stanger C, Dumenci L, Kamon J, Burstein M: Parenting and children’s externalizing problems in substance-abusing families. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004, 33: 590-600. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3303_16.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M: Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. Crime and Justice, Volume 7. Edited by: Tonry M, Morris N. 1986, Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press, 219-339. Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M: Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. Crime and Justice, Volume 7. Edited by: Tonry M, Morris N. 1986, Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press, 219-339.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Rhee SH, Waldman ID: Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviour: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128: 490-529.CrossRefPubMed Rhee SH, Waldman ID: Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviour: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128: 490-529.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Levy F, Hay D, Bennett K, McStephen M: Gender differences in ADHD subtype comorbidity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005, 44: 368-376. 10.1097/01.chi.0000153232.64968.c1.CrossRefPubMed Levy F, Hay D, Bennett K, McStephen M: Gender differences in ADHD subtype comorbidity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005, 44: 368-376. 10.1097/01.chi.0000153232.64968.c1.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin NC, Levy F, Piek J, Hay DA: A genetic study of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder and reading disability: aetiological overlaps and implications. Int J Dis Dev Ed. 2006, 53: 21-34. 10.1080/10349120500509992.CrossRef Martin NC, Levy F, Piek J, Hay DA: A genetic study of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder and reading disability: aetiological overlaps and implications. Int J Dis Dev Ed. 2006, 53: 21-34. 10.1080/10349120500509992.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Moffitt TE, Silva PA: Self-reported delinquency, neuropsychological deficit, and history of attention deficit disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1988, 16: 553-569. 10.1007/BF00914266.CrossRefPubMed Moffitt TE, Silva PA: Self-reported delinquency, neuropsychological deficit, and history of attention deficit disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1988, 16: 553-569. 10.1007/BF00914266.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Raine A, Brennan P, Mednick B, Mednick SA: High rates of violence, crime, academic problems, and behavioral problems in males with both early neuromotor deficits and unstable family environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53: 544-549. 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830060090012.CrossRefPubMed Raine A, Brennan P, Mednick B, Mednick SA: High rates of violence, crime, academic problems, and behavioral problems in males with both early neuromotor deficits and unstable family environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53: 544-549. 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830060090012.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Raine A, Yaralian PS, Reynolds C, Venables PH, Mednick SA: Spatial but not verbal cognitive deficits at age 3 years in persistently antisocial individuals. Dev Psychopathol. 2002, 14: 25-44. 10.1017/S0954579402001025.CrossRefPubMed Raine A, Yaralian PS, Reynolds C, Venables PH, Mednick SA: Spatial but not verbal cognitive deficits at age 3 years in persistently antisocial individuals. Dev Psychopathol. 2002, 14: 25-44. 10.1017/S0954579402001025.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Raine A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Lynam D: Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial path. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005, 114: 38-49.CrossRefPubMed Raine A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Lynam D: Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial path. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005, 114: 38-49.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Nigg JT, Hinshaw SP, Carte ET, Treating JJ: Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: explainable by comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems?. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998, 107: 468-480.CrossRefPubMed Nigg JT, Hinshaw SP, Carte ET, Treating JJ: Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: explainable by comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems?. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998, 107: 468-480.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Anckarsäter H, Lundström S, Kollberg L, Kerekes N, Palm C, Carlström E, Långström N, Magnusson PK, Halldner L, Bölte S, Gillberg C, Gumpert C, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P: The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011, 14: 495-508. 10.1375/twin.14.6.495.CrossRefPubMed Anckarsäter H, Lundström S, Kollberg L, Kerekes N, Palm C, Carlström E, Långström N, Magnusson PK, Halldner L, Bölte S, Gillberg C, Gumpert C, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P: The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011, 14: 495-508. 10.1375/twin.14.6.495.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Larson T, Anckarsäter H, Gillberg C, Ståhlberg O, Carlström E, Kadesjö B, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P, Gillberg C: The autism-tics, AD/HD and other comorbidities inventory (A-TAC): further validation of a telephone interview for epidemiological research. BMC Psychiatry. 2010, 10: 1-10.1186/1471-244X-10-1.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Larson T, Anckarsäter H, Gillberg C, Ståhlberg O, Carlström E, Kadesjö B, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P, Gillberg C: The autism-tics, AD/HD and other comorbidities inventory (A-TAC): further validation of a telephone interview for epidemiological research. BMC Psychiatry. 2010, 10: 1-10.1186/1471-244X-10-1.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Cubo E, Sáez Velasco S, Delgado Benito V, Ausin Villaverde V, Garcia Soto XR, Trejo Gabriel Y, Galan JM, Martin Santidrian A, Macarron JV, Cordero Guevara J, Benito-Leon J, Louis ED: Psychometric attributes of the Spanish version of A-TAC screening scale for autism spectrum disorders. Ann Pediatr. 2011, 75: 40-50. 10.1016/j.anpedi.2011.01.008.CrossRef Cubo E, Sáez Velasco S, Delgado Benito V, Ausin Villaverde V, Garcia Soto XR, Trejo Gabriel Y, Galan JM, Martin Santidrian A, Macarron JV, Cordero Guevara J, Benito-Leon J, Louis ED: Psychometric attributes of the Spanish version of A-TAC screening scale for autism spectrum disorders. Ann Pediatr. 2011, 75: 40-50. 10.1016/j.anpedi.2011.01.008.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Connors Lenke M: Motor outcomes in premature infants. Newborn Infant Nurs Rev. 2003, 3: 104-109. 10.1016/S1527-3369(03)00032-1.CrossRef Connors Lenke M: Motor outcomes in premature infants. Newborn Infant Nurs Rev. 2003, 3: 104-109. 10.1016/S1527-3369(03)00032-1.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Hansson SL, Svanström Röjvall A, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Gillberg C, Anckarsäter H: Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism - tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC): preliminary reliability and validity. Br J Psychiatry. 2005, 187: 262-267. 10.1192/bjp.187.3.262.CrossRefPubMed Hansson SL, Svanström Röjvall A, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Gillberg C, Anckarsäter H: Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism - tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC): preliminary reliability and validity. Br J Psychiatry. 2005, 187: 262-267. 10.1192/bjp.187.3.262.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Lambek R, Tannock R, Dalsgaard S, Trillingsgaard A, Damm D, Thomsen PH: Validating neuropsychological subtypes of ADHD: how do children with and without an executive function deficit differ?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010, 51: 895-904. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02248.x.CrossRefPubMed Lambek R, Tannock R, Dalsgaard S, Trillingsgaard A, Damm D, Thomsen PH: Validating neuropsychological subtypes of ADHD: how do children with and without an executive function deficit differ?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010, 51: 895-904. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02248.x.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson EB, Koenen KC, McCormick MC, Munir K, Hallett V, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A: A multivariate twin study of autistic traits in 12-year-olds: testing the fractionable autism triad hypothesis. Behav Genet. 2012, 42: 245-255. 10.1007/s10519-011-9500-3.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Robinson EB, Koenen KC, McCormick MC, Munir K, Hallett V, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A: A multivariate twin study of autistic traits in 12-year-olds: testing the fractionable autism triad hypothesis. Behav Genet. 2012, 42: 245-255. 10.1007/s10519-011-9500-3.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Fatemi SH, Aldinger KA, Ashwood P, Bauman ML, Blaha CD, Blatt GJ, Chauhan A, Chauhan V, Dager SR, Dickson PE, Estes AM, Goldowitz D, Heck DH, Kemper TL, King BH, Martin LA, Millen KJ, Mittleman G, Mosconi MW, Persico AM, Sweeney JA, Webb SJ, Welsh JP, Schneider M, von Gontard A: Consensus paper: pathological role of the cerebellum in autism. Cerebellum. 2012, 11: 777-807. 10.1007/s12311-012-0355-9.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Fatemi SH, Aldinger KA, Ashwood P, Bauman ML, Blaha CD, Blatt GJ, Chauhan A, Chauhan V, Dager SR, Dickson PE, Estes AM, Goldowitz D, Heck DH, Kemper TL, King BH, Martin LA, Millen KJ, Mittleman G, Mosconi MW, Persico AM, Sweeney JA, Webb SJ, Welsh JP, Schneider M, von Gontard A: Consensus paper: pathological role of the cerebellum in autism. Cerebellum. 2012, 11: 777-807. 10.1007/s12311-012-0355-9.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gustafsson P, Thernlund G, Ryding E, Rosén I, Cederblad M: Associations between cerebral blood-flow measured by single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), electro-encephalogram (EEG), behaviour symptoms, cognition and neurological soft signs in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Acta Paediatr. 2000, 89: 830-835.PubMed Gustafsson P, Thernlund G, Ryding E, Rosén I, Cederblad M: Associations between cerebral blood-flow measured by single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), electro-encephalogram (EEG), behaviour symptoms, cognition and neurological soft signs in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Acta Paediatr. 2000, 89: 830-835.PubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Gustafsson P, Svedin CG, Ericsson I, Lindén C, Karlsson MK, Thernlund G: Reliability and validity of the assessment of neurological soft-signs in children with and without attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010, 52: 364-370. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03407.x.CrossRefPubMed Gustafsson P, Svedin CG, Ericsson I, Lindén C, Karlsson MK, Thernlund G: Reliability and validity of the assessment of neurological soft-signs in children with and without attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010, 52: 364-370. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03407.x.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans DM, Martin NG: The validity of twin studies. GeneScreen. 2000, 1: 77-79. 10.1046/j.1466-9218.2000.00027.x.CrossRef Evans DM, Martin NG: The validity of twin studies. GeneScreen. 2000, 1: 77-79. 10.1046/j.1466-9218.2000.00027.x.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Motor function and perception in children with neuropsychiatric and conduct problems: results from a population based twin study
verfasst von
Peik Gustafsson
Nóra Kerekes
Henrik Anckarsäter
Paul Lichtenstein
Christopher Gillberg
Maria Råstam
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders / Ausgabe 1/2014
Print ISSN: 1866-1947
Elektronische ISSN: 1866-1955
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-11

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe