Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Study protocol

Movement cognition and narration of the emotions treatment versus standard speech therapy in the treatment of children with borderline intellectual functioning: a randomized controlled trial

verfasst von: V Blasi, G Baglio, F Baglio, MP Canevini, M Zanette

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

Borderline intellectual functioning (BIF) is defined as a “health meta-condition… characterized by various cognitive dysfunctions associated with an intellectual quotient (IQ) between 71 and 85 which determines a deficit in the individual’s functioning both in the restriction of activities and in the limitation of social participation”. It can be caused by many factors, including a disadvantaged background and prematurity. BIF affects 7–12% of primary school children that show academic difficulties due to poor executive functioning. In many children with BIF, language, movement and social abilities are also affected, making it difficult to take part in daily activities. Dropping out of school and psychological afflictions such as anxiety and depression are common in children with BIF.
This study investigates whether an intensive rehabilitation program that involves all of the areas affected in children with BIF (Movement, Cognition and Narration of emotions, MCNT) is more effective than Standard Speech Therapy (SST).

Methods

This is a multicenter interventional single blind randomized controlled study. Children aged between 6 to 11 years who attend a mainstream primary school and have multiple learning difficulties, behavioral problems and an IQ ranging between 85 to 70 have been enrolled. Participants are randomly allocated to one of three groups. The first group receives individual treatment with SST for 45 min, twice a week for 9 months. The second group receives the experimental treatment MCNT for 3 h per day, 5 days/ week for 9 months and children work in small groups. The third group consists of children on a waiting list for the SST for nine months.

Discussion

BIF is a very frequent condition with no ad hoc treatment. Over the long term, there is a high risk to develop psychiatric disorders in adulthood. Due to its high social impact, we consider it very important to intervene during childhood so as to intercept the remarkable plasticity of the developing brain.

Trial registration

“Study Let them grow: A new intensive and multimodal Treatment for children with borderline intellectual functioning based on Movement, Cognition and Narration of emotions”, retrospectively registered in ISRCTN Register with ISRCTN81710297 at 2017–01-09.
Abkürzungen
ADHD
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
BIF
Borderline Intellectual Functioning
BVN
Neuropsychological Evaluation Battery for developmental age
CBCL
Child Behavior Check List
EQ-i:YV
The Emotional Quotient Inventory-Youth Version
M-ABC
Movement Assessment Battery for Children
MBT
Modified Bells Test
MCNT
Movement Cognition and Narration of the emotions Treatment
MRI
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
SST
Standard Speech Treatment
TAU
Treatment As Usual
TOL
Tower Of London
TROG
Test for Reception Of Grammar
WISC-III
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children

Background

Borderline Intellectual Functioning (BIF) is a complex clinical entity that has received poor scientific investigation [1, 2]. According to the CONFIL consensus group [3], BIF can be defined as a “health meta-condition … characterized by various cognitive dysfunctions associated with an intellectual quotient (IQ) between 71 and 85 which determines a deficit in the individual’s functioning both in the restriction of activities and in the limitation of social participation”. Excluding BIF due to genetic and chromosomal abnormalities syndromes, the mostly frequent causes related to this condition are biological factors, like perinatal complications, and epigenetic factors related to the social environment such as maltreatment, abuse, neglect and high level of maternal stress [1, 47]. It is estimated that the prevalence of “non genetic” BIF ranges between 7 to 12% [2, 3] but the lack of terminological consensus and specific diagnostic code in the DSM-V [8] and ICD10 [9] makes it difficult to have a precise estimate. In DSM-V IQ test scores are removed from the diagnostic description and BIF is mentioned only within V-codes; this makes the BIF condition more difficult to be defined and acknowledged [2, 10].
BIF is associated with several neurodevelopmental disorders, with a high frequency for anxiety and behavioral disorders [1, 2, 1114]. A strong association between BIF and mental disorders is observed in adolescence and in adulthood (e.g. antisocial personality disorder, depression, psychosis, suicide and substance abuse) [12, 15, 16]. The comorbidity with mental disorders is of great relevance in the prognosis: presence of BIF adds further severity to the evolution of psychiatric diseases [17]. Moreover, BIF is a lifelong condition with obvious drawbacks in terms of social and vocational opportunities and health outcomes [1, 13, 15, 18]. A longitudinal study of the impact that mild intellectual deficits, including BIF, have on the life course demonstrated that low-IQ individuals, compared to their higher IQ siblings, completed less schooling, had less prestigious occupations, rated themselves less physically healthy, and reported lower levels of psychological well-being [19].
Furthermore, in childhood, BIF is associated with some degree of motor skills limitation and cognitive difficulties in gaining and generalizing knowledge, in executive functions (i.e. working memory, problem solving, attention and concentration, planning, and inhibition of impulsive responses) and text comprehension [3, 20, 21]. Indeed, children with BIF have a high rate of school failures [18, 22, 23].
Finally, in a recent study [24], we found that children of 7 to 10 years of age with BIF show a delay in the development of social competences. Specifically, they perform poorly on all types of false belief tasks that test the Theory of Mind. Their performance was comparable to children of a younger age (3 to 4 years) who understand that actions are driven by desires, thought, intention and true belief but do not comprehend that behavior can also be justified by misinterpretation, i.e., false belief. These difficulties can result in poor social relations and social disadvantage in adulthood.
Moreover, in a recent study [25] concerning brain morphometry in children with BIF, we detected a delayed development of the parahippocampal, temporal and sensory-motor cortices, important for behavioral, learning and motor abilities. Such delay was highly correlated with IQ indices. Considering the great amount of cerebral modifications that occur in the typical developing brain between 6 to 12 years of age [2629], we believe that an early and effective intervention is essential to improve the clinical course and the brain development trajectory of children with BIF.
For this reason, we developed an innovative treatment for BIF that stems from three main theoretical considerations: 1- Intelligence is not a fixed entity but a multidimensional and dynamic process; 2- the development of emotional, cognitive and motor skills is interrelated; 3-. environmental enrichment (EE) has a high impact on the neurodevelopment.
In relation to the first point, we considered the definition of intelligence of the Mainstream Science on Intelligence [30], “It is a very general mental capability that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly and learn from experience”. According to this definition, intelligence can be considered a modifiable entity that is largely dependent on educational, cultural, socioeconomic, emotional and motivational factors. This notion is supported by both behavioral and neuroimaging data [31] showing that individual’s intellectual capacity can decrease or increase in the teenage years and also correlate with independently measured changes in brain structure of specific regions. We consider intelligence important for the development of truly adaptive abilities that ultimately represents a protective factor from social disadvantage in adulthood.
Point 2 refers to recent findings demonstrating that the level of performance in motor, executive functioning and language skills is highly correlated in both typical development [3237] and in children with BIF [38, 39]. Moreover, the development of the brain areas involved in these functions is closely interrelated [40].
Finally, the third point is related to the well known effect of EE on neurodevelopment: richer and more stimulating environments determine higher rates of synaptogenesis and more complex dendritic arbors, leading to increased brain activity. EE was originally demonstrated in mouse brain [4143] and successively confirmed on humans [4447]. Research in humans has shown that attaining and engaging in higher levels of education and living in cognitively stimulating environments results in greater cognitive reserve [48]. The effects of EE take place primarily during neurodevelopment, but also throughout adulthood to a lesser degree [49, 50]. In humans, the opposite phenomenon has also been demonstrated: lack of stimulation in childhood, a critical period for the neurodevelopment, causes delay and impairment in the cognitive development, as in the case of children growing in Institutions that offer impoverished stimulation [51]. Similarly, we consider important, for the rehabilitation of children with BIF, to include different and multiple levels of stimulation as specified in the Methods section. Moreover, we consider part of the EE the capacity to listen and understand in an empathic way the children, focusing not only on their performances but also on their needs and emotions promoting the use of creative strategies to learn instead of fixed predetermined instructions.
All the above-mentioned considerations have been crucial for our choice of a treatment for the BIF. We introduce an innovative, multimodal and intensive training called the Movement Cognition and Narration of emotions Treatment (MCNT). No specific rehabilitation approach is available at the moment for children with BIF, but due to their poor academic performances they are typically treated with Standard Speech Therapy (SST). The latter is aimed at training specific functions such as writing or reading, and/or with the prescription to use facilitation devices such as the calculator and/or the computer.
The aim of our trial was to determine if the MCNT is more effective than SST in improving global intelligence in children with BIF. Our prediction is that MCNT will prove to be more effective than SST due to its use of a multimodal, empathic and game oriented approach, with the use of interactive technology devices, and a great variety of activities related to cognition, psychomotricity and emotions. The difficulties encountered by BIF children, indeed, cannot be delimited to a single specific domain. Consequently, we consider the SST approach as insufficient in treating a complex condition such as BIF. Furthermore, we predict that children with BIF attending the MCNT treatment will show a greater improvement also in the cognitive abilities and in particular, executive functioning. Finally, we will investigate if the MCNT improves the developmental trajectories of brain regions that we previously identified as having delayed maturation in children with BIF: the parahippocampal, temporal and sensory-motor cortices.

Methods

Study design

This is a multicentre interventional single blind randomized controlled study with three groups of children with BIF: group 1- children treated with SST (conventional treatment; N = 20); group 2-children treated with the MCNT (experimental treatment, N = 20); group 3-children not treated -on the waiting list for SST (no treatment N = 20).
The Study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Don Gnocchi Foundation and of the ASST S. Paolo and S. Carlo Hospital.
All parents signed a written informed consent at the first meeting.
The primary and secondary outcome measures are determined at two time points, within two months prior to the beginning of the treatment (T0) and within two months after the end of the treatment (T1). Evaluations of all children made before and after treatment are performed by two psychologists blind to the type of intervention that the child receives. The flow diagram for study enrollment and randomization, as per Consolidated Standards Of Reporting Trial (CONSORT) guidelines, is shown in Fig. 1.

Sample size

For the power calculation, considering preliminary data from our group, we expect a delta of 8 points (T1 –T0) in the primary outcome measure (WISC III FSIQ) between SST and MCNT groups, and 10 points between MCNT and no treatment group with a standard deviation of 10. With a sample size of 60, the calculation of the effect size is 0.43 and the probability is 0.83.
Standard procedures to maintain participants’ interest in the study and to avoid drop outs will be applied (regular meetings with teachers and parents of the children). Moreover, since the MCNT requires children to attend the treatment four day/week, we organized a shuttle that brings them from their school to our Centre brings, to avoid drop-outs.

Participants

Sixty participants will be consecutively recruited from the Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit of IRCCS Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation and of the ASST S. Paolo and S. Carlo Hospital.
Inclusion criteria are: age range between 6 to 11 years old and attending primary mainstream school; with a Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) score ranging from 70 to 85 determined with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (WISC-III) [52, 53]; presence of learning disabilities assessed with the standardized test battery for developmental dyslexia and dysorthographia (DDE-2) [54] and dyscalculia (AC-MT 6–11) [55]; difficulties in executive functions and or verbal comprehension assessed with Modified Barrage bell test (MBT) [56],Tower of London (TOL) [57, 58], and the Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG 2) [59, 60]; presence of an impact on daily life of the above mentioned difficulties as measured by the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL 6–18) [61, 62] and Vineland 2 [63].
The exclusion criteria are: presence of major neuropsychiatric disorders (such as ADHD and autism spectrum disorder); presence of neurological conditions such as epilepsy, traumatic brain injury, brain malformations and infectious disease involving the central nervous system. Moreover, the presence of systemic diseases such as diabetes or dysimmune disorders, genetic syndromes such as Down syndrome or Fragile X syndrome and a positive history for psychoactive drugs, particularly referring to current or past use of psychostimulants, neuroleptics, antidepressants, benzodiazepines and antiepileptic drugs are also considered exclusion criteria.

Randomization and masking

Randomization in the three groups occurred after screening and baseline assessment (T0). Subjects were randomly assigned to SST or MCNT or no treatment. The randomization process was performed using a computer algorithm (http://​graphpad.​com/​quickcalcs/​randMenu/​) by an independent operator not involved in neither phase of our research. All evaluations in both pre- and post-treatment are conducted by two psychologists blind to group allocation.

Intervention

Our study involves two types of intervention: the MCNT, which represents the experimental intervention; and the SST, the treatment as usual (TAU). Both treatments last for nine months and provide regular meetings between the professionals responsible for the treatment and the families and the teachers of the children. Regular weekly meetings among professionals are also included in both treatments. The MCNT is based on a multidimensional approach with children working in small groups while the SST is domain specific and treatment is one-on-one based.

Experimental treatment: The movement cognition and narration of the emotions treatment (MCNT)

The MCNT is an intervention in which children work in small groups, for 3 h each day, five days a week, Monday through Friday, for nine months. Each day they arrive at 2 pm, with a shuttle that brings them from their school to our Centre. They are divided into 3 groups, for the whole treatment, named Red, Blue and Green, according to their global functioning, grade and/or special educational needs. Upon arrival children are gathered all together for a 15 min long relaxing activity, after which each group starts with one of the three laboratories for 45 min. At the end of the first, the three groups switch the activity until all groups have completed all three laboratories.
MCNT is focused on the three domains affected in children with BIF that are strictly embedded with learning abilities: motor, cognitive and socio-emotional domain. This treatment consists of: a) Movement training to improve motor planning and fine and gross motor abilities with a Game Therapy approach using the Wii and Xbox video game platforms; b) Cognitive training for the empowerment of the executive functions such as working memory, planning abilities, problem solving, reasoning with the use of the multimedia interactive whiteboard (MIW); and c) an emotive training to learn how to Narrate the emotions to help the child to cope with the experiences of her/his daily life.
We chose to use the technology devices mentioned above because they are able to capture the attention and are highly motivating for the child. In addition, the use of the MIW represents a very useful visual aid for a visually-oriented teaching strategy that is effective in fostering verbal comprehension, memory encoding and storage processes. With both devices, moreover, it is possible to promote small competitions between the children which are very effective in motivating the child and improving information speed processing, positive interdependency and frustration tolerance. Finally, to help the child to narrate the emotions, a psychotherapist promotes several activities such as the narration of fairy tales, or drawing and symbolic playing to create a new story, to promote the capacity to think, to recognize and to master the emotions.
The first level concerned the movement execution and planning abilities: the objective is to improve balance, fine and gross motor abilities, hand-eye coordination, impulsive motor response inhibition, praxic abilities and attention. For this intervention, the Nintendo Wii Console with the “Wiimote” and the “balance board”, and the “Xbox 360 Kinetic” platforms were used. A game therapy approach is used to improve the children’s motivation and compliance. In the first weeks of treatment, the focus is on easier visual-motor integration skills, and the children familiarize themselves with the Wiimote while using it as a laser pointer to match a fixed visual input on the screen. A second step involves more complex games whereby the child points the Wiimote to a moving target (Wii Motion Plus game) that requires an increased involvement of attention and higher visual-motor integration ability. A third step involves games such as Wii Sports in which children use both the Wiimote and the Wii Balance board to train balance and coordination of both upper and lower limbs. Also, Wii Music and Wii Party games are used to train rhythm, timing of movement and inhibition of impulsive motor behavior. During the whole process, advanced executive functions, such as planning competence, working memory and inhibitory control are involved and trained so as to render the movements more fluid, economical, quicker and functional. Finally, the Xbox Kinetic console is used to train not only all body segments motricity but also moving in unison with another child. For example, in the Kinetic Adventure game, children maneuver a boat on river rapids in pairs with the movements of their bodies. In this step, the child needs to continuously adjust his/her own position in response to the movements of the other person. This activity facilitates training the awareness of his/her own body in space and relative to the other along with the capacity to master a novel situation in a cooperative manner.
The second domain concerns the cognitive abilities. The cognitive training is focused on several domains that include language comprehension and expression and executive functions, such as deductive and inductive reasoning, working memory, planning and problem solving, attention and concentration, inhibition of impulsive verbal responses. This part of the MCNT involves the use of the MIW. Particular attention is devoted to finding appropriate strategies to solve problems and to the constant training of the abilities to increase the velocity with which the task is executed. The aim is to promote the ability to explore the information, to select salient features which are relevant to solve the problem and finally to take a decision while examining the possible choices. Moreover, children are trained at keeping and sharing their attention with all members of the group for the whole duration of the session by promoting their active participation in the activities. Finally, according to the incremental intelligence theory [64], children are supported in the idea that their intelligence and their capacities can be increased by training and that each child has a different way to learn and to encode sensorial stimuli: some rely more upon visual strategies while others prefer verbal modalities. Indeed, great attention is devoted to the cognitive style and the learning strategies of each child. This approach helps the child to focus on the discovery of his/her personal functioning rather than being worried about the grade. Finally, working in a group promotes prosocial behavior and cooperative learning.
The third domain concerns emotions and social skills. BIF children very often lack adaptive skills and show behavioral, emotional and/or social-relations problems. The focus of this part of the treatment is to help the children to express, recognize and cope with their own emotions. We named this process “alphabetization” of the emotions [65]. The underlying idea stems from the psychoanalytic model of Bion [66, 67] in which the comprehension of the emotional experience is central to thought and learning. The term “alphabetization” refers to the capacity to transform the unknown sensations that precede and accompany any emotion into elements that can be thought and narrated [66, 67]. This process demands that the unknown sensations, which can generate discomfort, can be tolerated and contained in the mind of the subject and/or of the therapist for a sufficient time to be transformed into symbols that can be thought, experienced and communicated [65]. The alphabetization process operates through a symbolization mechanism that implies the “as if” process. Giving a simple name to the emotion is not sufficient, it is necessary to transform it into a metaphor, a story or a play. Such a process is very important for social relations: if the child can express his/her inner states, he/she has no need to act rashly upon them. Finally, the alphabetization process helps the child to “clear” the mind from very unwanted thoughts and sensations, which can ultimately facilitate the cognitive processes. The therapist, a psychologist with a psychotherapy degree, uses different approaches to promote the narration of the emotions: symbolic play, reading, inventing and/or dramatizing a story, drawing and talking. These activities are considered creative processes with a transitional function [68] that enable the children and the therapist to deal with the emotions in an indirect way, preventing the fear of being judged and persecutory feelings. Finally, such a transitional space enables and promotes the “as if” process [68]. The therapist helps the children to associate the feelings and emotions narrated and symbolized through the activities with their own experiences. Within the group, each child can introduce new characters, new scenarios and/or situations to challenge the group to find solutions. The goal is to encourage cooperation and to experience the facilitation effect of the group in thinking and coping with emotions.

Treatment as usual: The standard speech therapy (SST)

STT is an individual treatment consisting of two 45 min sessions each per week for nine months. The focus is on the training of the academic abilities that are compromised in the child as assessed by the neuropsychological evaluation at T0 (pre-treatment). Typically, the skills mostly affected are reading comprehension, word-meaning relationship (semantics); writing problems related to phoneme-grapheme correspondence; arithmetic abilities; and problem solving. To empower these skills, SST uses both pencil/paper tools and specific rehabilitation software (http://​www.​erickson.​it/​). In the event of dyslexia or dysorthographia, the principal aims of SST are to increase processing information speed and transcoding, reducing spelling mistakes and expanding personal vocabulary. One of the approaches used is sub-lexical treatment. For dyscalculia, images are used to reason and solve the problem as, for example, in the analogical method [69]. Moreover, the empowerment of transversal competences such as phonological competences, perception, visual-spatial ability, attention, memory and other executive functions is pursued together with the use of compensative tools. Key criteria are functional re-education and the use of a meta-cognitive approach. The first provides for using alternative/different strategies to cover or partially rehabilitate compromised mechanisms whereas the second is based on strategies to focus the attention on mental processing (input, elaboration and output). Finally, the speech therapist offers pedagogy consultation for both parents and teachers to increase their knowledge concerning learning difficulties and potentially associated behavioral-emotional problems and finally to share effective problem solving strategies.

Assessment design and outcomes measures

The timeline of the study is illustrated in Fig. 2. All children are evaluated at two time points, within two months prior to the beginning of the treatment (T0) and within two months after the end of the treatment (T1). For the no-treatment group the same time interval is kept.
To measure intellectual functioning and motor competences, we used the WISC-III [52, 53] and the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) [70], respectively.
The Emotional Quotient Inventory-Youth Version (EQ-i:YV) [71] was used to assess the emotion competences, while the Child Behavior Check List (CBCL 6–18) [61, 62] and Vineland 2 [63] were employed for investigating adaptive behavior.
A full neuropsychological evaluation was also made with the use of following tests: the Modified Bells Test (selective attention MBT) [56]; the Tower of London (TOL) planning ability and inhibitory control; [57, 58]; the Neuropsychological Evaluation Battery for developmental age 5–11 (BVN5–11, speech fluency, selective word retrieval and Corsi) [72] to assess verbal executive function and verbal and visual short term memory; the Test of Reception of Grammar-2 (TROG2, language comprehension) [59, 60].
Finally, our study includes measures of brain morphometry and function, evaluated by means of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) as surrogate outcome measure. All children participating in the study are evaluated with non-conventional brain MRI to investigate brain morphometry and functionality in the regions that we found abnormally developed in our previous work [25]: the parahippocampal, temporal and sensory-motor cortices.

Outcomes

Primary and secondary outcome are evaluated at T0 and T1.
The primary outcome measures are:1 WISC-III; 2. M-ABC; 3. EQ-i:YV; 4. CBCL 6–18 and Vineland 2.
Secondary outcome measure are: 1. MBT; 2. TOL; 3. BVN5–11; 4. TROG2; and MRI.

Analysis

Statistical analyses on outcome measures will be conducted using R and SPSS. The General Linear Model (GLM) will be used to test the interaction effects between treatment (MCNT vs no treatment, MCNT vs TAU, TAU vs no treatment) and time (2 repeated measures, T0 and T1). Violations of the parametric model assumptions will be tested and explored with ad-hoc techniques. If violations will be detected, robust methods, data manipulation or statistical corrections will be applied accordingly. An intention-to-treat analysis approach will be used. The missing data process will be examined and the proper imputation method will be selected thereafter.
MRI data will be analyzed with specific statistical software: SPM12, (http://​www.​fil.​ion.​ucl.​ac.​uk/​spm/​); and FreeSurfer (https://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu/​) and FSL (https://​fsl.​fmrib.​ox.​ac.​uk/​fsl/​fslwiki/​). Random effect analyses will be done to detect if there is an effect of treatment on the variables measured. The model will comprise a between factor level (group) and a within factor level (time). Demographic characteristics will be used as covariates if associated with the outcome measure.

Discussion

Borderline Intellectual Functioning is a very frequent condition with a high social impact due to poor school achievements and long term high risk to develop psychiatric conditions in adulthood.
In our protocol, we aim to measure the efficacy of an intensive training that, contrary to the TAU, works in parallel on all domains affected in borderline intellectual functioning: movement, cognition and emotions.
As mentioned in the introduction, it is important to intervene during childhood so as to intercept the remarkable plasticity of the developing brain in which dendrites and spines are able to form new synapses in hours and even minutes in response to some experiences [73]. This holds true for both positive and negative experiences, such as effective rehabilitation programs and adverse events, respectively. The last, when occurring early in life, can produce long term modifications in brain functioning associated with a “latent vulnerability” for psychiatric disorders in adulthood [74]. Recent studies, indeed, have consistently shown hyperactivity in the amygdala when children that have been exposed to early adversity or maltreatment are stimulated with threat-related cues [75] [76].
According to these premises, we claim the importance of finding an effective treatment for children with BIF, which is capable of intercepting the great potentiality of the developing brain and to realign the development towards more typical trajectories.

Trial status

The trial is ongoing.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the BIF Group: Belotti D1, Bergsland NP1, Bolognesi E1, Bossi F2 , Cabinio M1, Celani G2, Clerici M3, D’Amico A1, Dalla Longa G1, Giorgetti G1, Guerini FR1, Isola G1, Mancuso R1, Marin A1, Milone L1, Nemni R3, Rumi V2, Ravasio S1, Saresella M1, Simonetti M1, Sironi B2, Spada C2, Valli B2, Villa V1, Walder M2.
1. IRCCS, Fondazione don Carlo Gnocchi ONLUS, Milan
2. ASST S. Paolo and S. Carlo Hospital, Milan, Italy
3. IRCCS, Fondazione don Carlo Gnocchi ONLUS, Milan and Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan

Funding

The study has been funded by the “Regione Lombardia, Italy” within the call of the Ricerca Indipendente of 2012. “This funding source had no role in the design of this study and will not have any role during its execution, analyses, interpretation of the data, or decision to submit results.”Trial Sponsor contact information:Dott.ssa Mariella De Biase, DG Welfare -Struttura Piani e Progetti, email: mariella_de_biase©regione.lombardia.it

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to lack of specific consent in the ethics committee approval but will be available from the corresponding author on reasonable request after the publication of results.

Authors’ contribution

MZ, FB and VB conceived, the study. GB, VB, and MZ initiated the study design and MPC helped with implementation. All authors contributed to refinement of the study protocol and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
We obtained written consent for publication from the parents of the children.
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee the “IRCCS Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation” and of the “ASST S. Paolo and S. Carlo Hospital”. The study is registered on the ISRCTN registry with trial ID ISRCTN81710297.
The Chief Investigator (VB) or the senior researcher (MZ) introduce the trial to the parents of the patients. Parents will also receive information sheets. Informed written consent to participate in the study is obtained by the parents of each child. Model consent form given to participants is available upon request.Participant-level dataset will be available upon request.Although the type of intervention scheduled for this study poses no risk for the participants, a reporting procedure will be in place to ensure that any adverse event is reported to the Chief Investigator. Both sites involved in the trial (Don Gnocchi Foundation and San Paolo Hospital) have an Insurance Policy covering the risks related to the study.All study-related information are stored securely at the study site, (Don Gnocchi Foundation, IRCCS Milan). All participant information are stored in locked file cabinets in areas with limited access. All reports, data collection, process, and administrative forms are identified by a coded ID only to maintain participant confidentiality. All local databases are secured with password-protected access systems.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Peltopuro M, Ahonen T, Kaartinen J, Seppala H, Narhi V. Borderline intellectual functioning: a systematic literature review. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2014;52(6):419–43.CrossRefPubMed Peltopuro M, Ahonen T, Kaartinen J, Seppala H, Narhi V. Borderline intellectual functioning: a systematic literature review. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2014;52(6):419–43.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Hassiotis A. Borderline intellectual functioning and neurodevelopmental disorders: prevalence, comorbidities and treatment approaches. Adv Mental Hlth Intell Disabil. 2015;9(5):275–83.CrossRef Hassiotis A. Borderline intellectual functioning and neurodevelopmental disorders: prevalence, comorbidities and treatment approaches. Adv Mental Hlth Intell Disabil. 2015;9(5):275–83.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Salvador-Carulla L, Garcia-Gutierrez JC, Ruiz Gutierrez-Colosia M, Artigas-Pallares J, Garcia Ibanez J, Gonzalez Perez J, Nadal Pla M, Aguilera Ines F, Isus S, Cereza JM, Poole M, Portero Lazcano G, Monzon P, Leiva M, Parellada M, Garcia Nonell K, Martinez I, Hernandez A, Rigau E, Martinez-Leal R. Borderline intellectual functioning: consensus and good practice guidelines. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2013;6(3):109–20.CrossRefPubMed Salvador-Carulla L, Garcia-Gutierrez JC, Ruiz Gutierrez-Colosia M, Artigas-Pallares J, Garcia Ibanez J, Gonzalez Perez J, Nadal Pla M, Aguilera Ines F, Isus S, Cereza JM, Poole M, Portero Lazcano G, Monzon P, Leiva M, Parellada M, Garcia Nonell K, Martinez I, Hernandez A, Rigau E, Martinez-Leal R. Borderline intellectual functioning: consensus and good practice guidelines. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2013;6(3):109–20.CrossRefPubMed
4.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99.CrossRefPubMed Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Marcus Jenkins JV, Woolley DP, Hooper SR, De Bellis MD. Direct and indirect effects of brain volume, socioeconomic status and family stress on child IQ. J Child Adolesc Behav. 2013;1(2):1000107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marcus Jenkins JV, Woolley DP, Hooper SR, De Bellis MD. Direct and indirect effects of brain volume, socioeconomic status and family stress on child IQ. J Child Adolesc Behav. 2013;1(2):1000107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Fenning RM, Baker JK, Baker BL, Crnic KA. Parenting children with borderline intellectual functioning: a unique risk population. Am J Ment Retard. 2007;112(2):107–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fenning RM, Baker JK, Baker BL, Crnic KA. Parenting children with borderline intellectual functioning: a unique risk population. Am J Ment Retard. 2007;112(2):107–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). In: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). In: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems: 10th revision. 2nd ed. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2004. World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems: 10th revision. 2nd ed. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2004.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Wieland J, Zitman FG. It is time to bring borderline intellectual functioning back into the main fold of classification systems. BJ Psych Bull. 2016;40(4):204–6. Wieland J, Zitman FG. It is time to bring borderline intellectual functioning back into the main fold of classification systems. BJ Psych Bull. 2016;40(4):204–6.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Dekker MC, Nunn RJ, Einfeld SE, Tonge BJ, Koot HM. Assessing emotional and behavioral problems in children with intellectual disability: revisiting the factor structure of the developmental behavior checklist. J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(6):601–10.CrossRefPubMed Dekker MC, Nunn RJ, Einfeld SE, Tonge BJ, Koot HM. Assessing emotional and behavioral problems in children with intellectual disability: revisiting the factor structure of the developmental behavior checklist. J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(6):601–10.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Dekker MC, Koot HM. DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to moderate intellectual disability. II: child and family predictors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(8):923–31.CrossRefPubMed Dekker MC, Koot HM. DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to moderate intellectual disability. II: child and family predictors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(8):923–31.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Emerson E, Einfeld S, Stancliffe RJ. The mental health of young children with intellectual disabilities or borderline intellectual functioning. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(5):579–87.CrossRefPubMed Emerson E, Einfeld S, Stancliffe RJ. The mental health of young children with intellectual disabilities or borderline intellectual functioning. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(5):579–87.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Salvador-Carulla L, Gibert K, Ochoa S. Definition of "functional dependency". Implications for health and social policy. Aten Primaria. 2010;42(6):344–5.CrossRefPubMed Salvador-Carulla L, Gibert K, Ochoa S. Definition of "functional dependency". Implications for health and social policy. Aten Primaria. 2010;42(6):344–5.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Hassiotis A, Strydom A, Hall I, Ali A, Lawrence-Smith G, Meltzer H, Head J, Bebbington P. Psychiatric morbidity and social functioning among adults with borderline intelligence living in private households. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008;52(Pt 2):95–106.PubMed Hassiotis A, Strydom A, Hall I, Ali A, Lawrence-Smith G, Meltzer H, Head J, Bebbington P. Psychiatric morbidity and social functioning among adults with borderline intelligence living in private households. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008;52(Pt 2):95–106.PubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Gigi K, Werbeloff N, Goldberg S, Portuguese S, Reichenberg A, Fruchter E, Weiser M. Borderline intellectual functioning is associated with poor social functioning, increased rates of psychiatric diagnosis and drug use--a cross sectional population based study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24(11):1793–7.CrossRefPubMed Gigi K, Werbeloff N, Goldberg S, Portuguese S, Reichenberg A, Fruchter E, Weiser M. Borderline intellectual functioning is associated with poor social functioning, increased rates of psychiatric diagnosis and drug use--a cross sectional population based study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24(11):1793–7.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaplin R, Barley M, Cooper SJ, Kusel Y, McKendrick J, Stephenson D, Obuaya T, Stockton-Henderson J, O'Brien LS, Burns T. The impact of intellectual functioning on symptoms and service use in schizophrenia. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2006;50(Pt 4):288–94.CrossRefPubMed Chaplin R, Barley M, Cooper SJ, Kusel Y, McKendrick J, Stephenson D, Obuaya T, Stockton-Henderson J, O'Brien LS, Burns T. The impact of intellectual functioning on symptoms and service use in schizophrenia. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2006;50(Pt 4):288–94.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Karande S, Kanchan S, Kulkarni M. Clinical and psychoeducational profile of children with borderline intellectual functioning. Indian J Pediatr. 2008;75(8):795–800.CrossRefPubMed Karande S, Kanchan S, Kulkarni M. Clinical and psychoeducational profile of children with borderline intellectual functioning. Indian J Pediatr. 2008;75(8):795–800.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Orsmond GI, Lounds J. Life course studies of siblings of individuals with developmental disabilities. Ment Retard. 2005;43(5):354–9.PubMed Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Orsmond GI, Lounds J. Life course studies of siblings of individuals with developmental disabilities. Ment Retard. 2005;43(5):354–9.PubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Cornoldi C, Giofre D, Orsini A, Pezzuti L. Differences in the intellectual profile of children with intellectual vs. learning disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(9):2224–30.CrossRefPubMed Cornoldi C, Giofre D, Orsini A, Pezzuti L. Differences in the intellectual profile of children with intellectual vs. learning disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(9):2224–30.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Vuijk PJ, Hartman E, Scherder E, Visscher C. Motor performance of children with mild intellectual disability and borderline intellectual functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(11):955–65.CrossRefPubMed Vuijk PJ, Hartman E, Scherder E, Visscher C. Motor performance of children with mild intellectual disability and borderline intellectual functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(11):955–65.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw SR. An educational programming framework for a subset of students with diverse learning needs: borderline intellectual functioning. Interv Sch Clin. 2008;43(5):291–9.CrossRef Shaw SR. An educational programming framework for a subset of students with diverse learning needs: borderline intellectual functioning. Interv Sch Clin. 2008;43(5):291–9.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw SR. Rescuing students from the slow learner trap. Princ Leadersh. 2010;10(6):12–6. Shaw SR. Rescuing students from the slow learner trap. Princ Leadersh. 2010;10(6):12–6.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Baglio G, Blasi V, Sangiuliano Intra F, Castelli I, Massaro D, Baglio F, Valle A, Zanette M, Marchetti A. Social competence in children with borderline intellectual functioning: delayed development of theory of mind across all complexity levels. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1604.PubMedPubMedCentral Baglio G, Blasi V, Sangiuliano Intra F, Castelli I, Massaro D, Baglio F, Valle A, Zanette M, Marchetti A. Social competence in children with borderline intellectual functioning: delayed development of theory of mind across all complexity levels. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1604.PubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Baglio F, Cabinio M, Ricci C, Baglio G, Lipari S, Griffanti L, Preti MG, Nemni R, Clerici M, Zanette M, Blasi V. Abnormal development of sensory-motor, visual temporal and parahippocampal cortex in children with learning disabilities and borderline intellectual functioning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:806.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Baglio F, Cabinio M, Ricci C, Baglio G, Lipari S, Griffanti L, Preti MG, Nemni R, Clerici M, Zanette M, Blasi V. Abnormal development of sensory-motor, visual temporal and parahippocampal cortex in children with learning disabilities and borderline intellectual functioning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:806.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–3.CrossRefPubMed Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–3.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(6):718–29.CrossRefPubMed Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(6):718–29.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Groeschel S, Vollmer B, King MD, Connelly A. Developmental changes in cerebral grey and white matter volume from infancy to adulthood. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2010;28(6):481–9.CrossRefPubMed Groeschel S, Vollmer B, King MD, Connelly A. Developmental changes in cerebral grey and white matter volume from infancy to adulthood. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2010;28(6):481–9.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Silk TJ, Wood AG. Lessons about neurodevelopment from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32(2):158–68.CrossRefPubMed Silk TJ, Wood AG. Lessons about neurodevelopment from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32(2):158–68.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Gottfredson LS: Mainstream science on intelligence: an editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography 1997, . Gottfredson LS: Mainstream science on intelligence: an editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography 1997, .
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramsden S, Richardson FM, Josse G, Thomas MS, Ellis C, Shakeshaft C, Seghier ML, Price CJ. Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature. 2011;479(7371):113–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ramsden S, Richardson FM, Josse G, Thomas MS, Ellis C, Shakeshaft C, Seghier ML, Price CJ. Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature. 2011;479(7371):113–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Wassenberg R, Feron FJ, Kessels AG, Hendriksen JG, Kalff AC, Kroes M, Hurks PP, Beeren M, Jolles J, Vles JS. Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a large-scale cross-sectional study. Child Dev. 2005;76(5):1092–103.CrossRefPubMed Wassenberg R, Feron FJ, Kessels AG, Hendriksen JG, Kalff AC, Kroes M, Hurks PP, Beeren M, Jolles J, Vles JS. Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a large-scale cross-sectional study. Child Dev. 2005;76(5):1092–103.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. J Child Lang. 2010;37(2):229–61.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. J Child Lang. 2010;37(2):229–61.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Fels IM, Te Wierike SC, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18(6):697–703.CrossRefPubMed van der Fels IM, Te Wierike SC, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18(6):697–703.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Oudgenoeg-Paz O, Volman MC, Leseman PP. First steps into language? Examining the specific longitudinal relations between walking. Explor Linguist Skills Front Psychol. 2016;7:1458. Oudgenoeg-Paz O, Volman MC, Leseman PP. First steps into language? Examining the specific longitudinal relations between walking. Explor Linguist Skills Front Psychol. 2016;7:1458.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Inkster M, Wellsby M, Lloyd E, Pexman PM. Development of embodied word meanings: Sensorimotor effects in Children's lexical processing. Front Psychol. 2016;7:317.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Inkster M, Wellsby M, Lloyd E, Pexman PM. Development of embodied word meanings: Sensorimotor effects in Children's lexical processing. Front Psychol. 2016;7:317.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Ziemke T. The body of knowledge: on the role of the living body in grounding embodied cognition. Biosystems. 2016;148:4–11.CrossRefPubMed Ziemke T. The body of knowledge: on the role of the living body in grounding embodied cognition. Biosystems. 2016;148:4–11.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartman E, Houwen S, Scherder E, Visscher C. On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(5):468–77.CrossRefPubMed Hartman E, Houwen S, Scherder E, Visscher C. On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54(5):468–77.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Houwen S, Visser L, van der Putten A, Vlaskamp C. The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;53-54:19–31.CrossRefPubMed Houwen S, Visser L, van der Putten A, Vlaskamp C. The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;53-54:19–31.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000;71(1):44–56.CrossRefPubMed Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000;71(1):44–56.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman J, Das GD. Autoradiographic examination of the effects of enriched environment on the rate of Glial multiplication in the adult rat brain. Nature. 1964;204:1161–3.CrossRefPubMed Altman J, Das GD. Autoradiographic examination of the effects of enriched environment on the rate of Glial multiplication in the adult rat brain. Nature. 1964;204:1161–3.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Schapiro S, Vukovich KR. Early experience effects upon cortical dendrites: a proposed model for development. Science. 1970;167(3916):292–4.CrossRefPubMed Schapiro S, Vukovich KR. Early experience effects upon cortical dendrites: a proposed model for development. Science. 1970;167(3916):292–4.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Vance DE, Kaur J, Fazeli PL, Talley MH, Yuen HK, Kitchin B, Lin F. Neuroplasticity and successful cognitive aging: a brief overview for nursing. J Neurosci Nurs. 2012;44(4):218–27.CrossRefPubMed Vance DE, Kaur J, Fazeli PL, Talley MH, Yuen HK, Kitchin B, Lin F. Neuroplasticity and successful cognitive aging: a brief overview for nursing. J Neurosci Nurs. 2012;44(4):218–27.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobs B, Schall M, Scheibel AB. A quantitative dendritic analysis of Wernicke's area in humans. II. Gender, hemispheric, and environmental factors. J Comp Neurol. 1993;327(1):97–111.CrossRefPubMed Jacobs B, Schall M, Scheibel AB. A quantitative dendritic analysis of Wernicke's area in humans. II. Gender, hemispheric, and environmental factors. J Comp Neurol. 1993;327(1):97–111.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Winkler J, Buchel C, May A. Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci. 2006;26(23):6314–7.CrossRefPubMed Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Winkler J, Buchel C, May A. Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci. 2006;26(23):6314–7.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A. Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature. 2004;427(6972):311–2.CrossRefPubMed Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A. Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature. 2004;427(6972):311–2.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Ilg R, Wohlschlager AM, Gaser C, Liebau Y, Dauner R, Woller A, Zimmer C, Zihl J, Muhlau M. Gray matter increase induced by practice correlates with task-specific activation: a combined functional and morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2008;28(16):4210–5.CrossRefPubMed Ilg R, Wohlschlager AM, Gaser C, Liebau Y, Dauner R, Woller A, Zimmer C, Zihl J, Muhlau M. Gray matter increase induced by practice correlates with task-specific activation: a combined functional and morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2008;28(16):4210–5.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Speisman RB, Kumar A, Rani A, Pastoriza JM, Severance JE, Foster TC, Ormerod BK. Environmental enrichment restores neurogenesis and rapid acquisition in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(1):263–74.CrossRefPubMed Speisman RB, Kumar A, Rani A, Pastoriza JM, Severance JE, Foster TC, Ormerod BK. Environmental enrichment restores neurogenesis and rapid acquisition in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(1):263–74.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1(3):191–8.CrossRefPubMed van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1(3):191–8.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Clemenson GD, Deng W, Gage FH. Environmental enrichment and neurogenesis: from mice to humans. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;4:56–62.CrossRef Clemenson GD, Deng W, Gage FH. Environmental enrichment and neurogenesis: from mice to humans. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;4:56–62.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, Marrus N, Harms MP, Babb C, Nishino T, Barch D. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(12):1135–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, Marrus N, Harms MP, Babb C, Nishino T, Barch D. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(12):1135–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D: WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children: manual: psychological Corporation; 1991. Wechsler D: WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children: manual: psychological Corporation; 1991.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D, Orsini A, Picone L: WISC-3. Wechsler intelligence scale for children, terza edizione manuale David Wechsler: Firenze: O. S.; 2006. Wechsler D, Orsini A, Picone L: WISC-3. Wechsler intelligence scale for children, terza edizione manuale David Wechsler: Firenze: O. S.; 2006.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Sartori G, Job R, Tressoldi P. DDE-2. Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva (Battery for the assessment of developmental dyslexia and dysorthographia). Giunti OS: Firenze; 2007. Sartori G, Job R, Tressoldi P. DDE-2. Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva (Battery for the assessment of developmental dyslexia and dysorthographia). Giunti OS: Firenze; 2007.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Cornoldi C, Lucangeli D, Bellina M. Ac-Mt 6–11. Test di valutazione delle abilità di calcolo e soluzione dei problemi. Gruppo MT. Con CD-ROM: Edizioni Erickson; 2012. Cornoldi C, Lucangeli D, Bellina M. Ac-Mt 6–11. Test di valutazione delle abilità di calcolo e soluzione dei problemi. Gruppo MT. Con CD-ROM: Edizioni Erickson; 2012.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Biancardi A, Stoppa E. Il test delle campanelle modificato (TCM) una proposta per lo studio dell’attenzione in età evolutiva. Psichiatria Infanzia Adolescenza. 1997;64:73–84. Biancardi A, Stoppa E. Il test delle campanelle modificato (TCM) una proposta per lo studio dell’attenzione in età evolutiva. Psichiatria Infanzia Adolescenza. 1997;64:73–84.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 1982;298(1089):199–209.CrossRef Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 1982;298(1089):199–209.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Sannio Fancello G, Vio C, Cianchetti C. TOL Torre di Londra. In: Test di valutazione delle funzioni esecutive (pianificazione e problem solving). Trento: Erickson; 2006. Sannio Fancello G, Vio C, Cianchetti C. TOL Torre di Londra. In: Test di valutazione delle funzioni esecutive (pianificazione e problem solving). Trento: Erickson; 2006.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Suraniti S, Ferri R, Neri V. Test for Reception of Grammar version 2 (TROG-2). OS: Firenze; 2009. Suraniti S, Ferri R, Neri V. Test for Reception of Grammar version 2 (TROG-2). OS: Firenze; 2009.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Bishop D: The test for Reception of Grammar - version 2: London: psychological Corporation; 2003. Bishop D: The test for Reception of Grammar - version 2: London: psychological Corporation; 2003.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach T, Rescorla L. Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2007. Achenbach T, Rescorla L. Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2007.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Sparrow S, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland–II: survey forms manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2005. Sparrow S, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland–II: survey forms manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2005.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Dweck CS. Self-theories: their role in motivation, personality and development. Philadelphia: Taylor and Francis/Psychology Press ed; 1999. Dweck CS. Self-theories: their role in motivation, personality and development. Philadelphia: Taylor and Francis/Psychology Press ed; 1999.
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Blasi V, Zanette M, Ferro A. Mentalization as alphabetization of the emotions: oscillation between the opening and closing of possible worlds. In: International forum of psychoanalysis; 2016. p. 1–10. Blasi V, Zanette M, Ferro A. Mentalization as alphabetization of the emotions: oscillation between the opening and closing of possible worlds. In: International forum of psychoanalysis; 2016. p. 1–10.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Bion WR: Transformations: London: Mansfield reprints; 1984. Bion WR: Transformations: London: Mansfield reprints; 1984.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Bion WR: Learning from experience: London: Maresfield; 1984. Bion WR: Learning from experience: London: Maresfield; 1984.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Winnicott DW: Playing and reality: Harmondsworth: penguin; 1980. Winnicott DW: Playing and reality: Harmondsworth: penguin; 1980.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Bortolato C. The analogical method. In: The theory of the intuitive learning of mathematic. Trento: Erickson; 2014. Bortolato C. The analogical method. In: The theory of the intuitive learning of mathematic. Trento: Erickson; 2014.
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett AL: Movement assessment battery for children: psychological Corporation London; 1992. Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett AL: Movement assessment battery for children: psychological Corporation London; 1992.
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Bar-On R, Parker JDA: BarOn emotional quotient inventory: Youth version: multi-health system, incorporated; 2000. Bar-On R, Parker JDA: BarOn emotional quotient inventory: Youth version: multi-health system, incorporated; 2000.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Bisiacchi PS, Cendron M, Gugliotta M, Tressoldi PE, Vio C: BVN 5–11: batteria di valutazione neuropsicologica per l'età evolutiva: Centro studi Erickson; 2005. Bisiacchi PS, Cendron M, Gugliotta M, Tressoldi PE, Vio C: BVN 5–11: batteria di valutazione neuropsicologica per l'età evolutiva: Centro studi Erickson; 2005.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Greenough WT, Chang FF. Plasticity of synapse structure and pattern in the cerebral cortex. In: Peters A, Jones EG, editors. Cerebral Cortex, vol. 7. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1989. p. 391–440. Greenough WT, Chang FF. Plasticity of synapse structure and pattern in the cerebral cortex. In: Peters A, Jones EG, editors. Cerebral Cortex, vol. 7. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1989. p. 391–440.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat McCrory EJ, Viding E. The theory of latent vulnerability: Reconceptualizing the link between childhood maltreatment and psychiatric disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(2):493–505.CrossRefPubMed McCrory EJ, Viding E. The theory of latent vulnerability: Reconceptualizing the link between childhood maltreatment and psychiatric disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(2):493–505.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Maheu FS, Dozier M, Guyer AE, Mandell D, Peloso E, Poeth K, Jenness J, Lau JY, Ackerman JP, Pine DS, Ernst M. A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10(1):34–49.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Maheu FS, Dozier M, Guyer AE, Mandell D, Peloso E, Poeth K, Jenness J, Lau JY, Ackerman JP, Pine DS, Ernst M. A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10(1):34–49.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Tottenham N, Hare TA, Millner A, Gilhooly T, Zevin JD, Casey BJ. Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci. 2011;14(2):190–204.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tottenham N, Hare TA, Millner A, Gilhooly T, Zevin JD, Casey BJ. Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci. 2011;14(2):190–204.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Movement cognition and narration of the emotions treatment versus standard speech therapy in the treatment of children with borderline intellectual functioning: a randomized controlled trial
verfasst von
V Blasi
G Baglio
F Baglio
MP Canevini
M Zanette
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1309-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

BMC Psychiatry 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe