Skip to main content
Erschienen in:

Open Access 01.06.2019 | Original Paper

Neocerebellar Crus I Abnormalities Associated with a Speech and Language Disorder Due to a Mutation in FOXP2

verfasst von: G. P. D. Argyropoulos, K. E. Watkins, E. Belton-Pagnamenta, F. Liégeois, K. S. Saleem, M. Mishkin, F. Vargha-Khadem

Erschienen in: The Cerebellum | Ausgabe 3/2019

Abstract

Bilateral volume reduction in the caudate nucleus has been established as a prominent brain abnormality associated with a FOXP2 mutation in affected members of the ‘KE family’, who present with developmental orofacial and verbal dyspraxia in conjunction with pervasive language deficits. Despite the gene’s early and prominent expression in the cerebellum and the evidence for reciprocal cerebellum-basal ganglia connectivity, very little is known about cerebellar abnormalities in affected KE members. Using cerebellum-specific voxel-based morphometry (VBM) and volumetry, we provide converging evidence from subsets of affected KE members scanned at three time points for grey matter (GM) volume reduction bilaterally in neocerebellar lobule VIIa Crus I compared with unaffected members and unrelated controls. We also show that right Crus I volume correlates with left and total caudate nucleus volumes in affected KE members, and that right and total Crus I volumes predict the performance of affected members in non-word repetition and non-verbal orofacial praxis. Crus I also shows bilateral hypo-activation in functional MRI in the affected KE members relative to controls during non-word repetition. The association of Crus I with key aspects of the behavioural phenotype of this FOXP2 point mutation is consistent with recent evidence of cerebellar involvement in complex motor sequencing. For the first time, specific cerebello-basal ganglia loops are implicated in the execution of complex oromotor sequences needed for human speech.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12311-018-0989-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

A dominantly inherited constellation of speech and language deficits in half the members of the multi-generational ‘KE family’ [13] has been linked to a mutation in FOXP2 [4], the first gene to be implicated in speech and language [5]. Neural and genetic properties of this disorder may enhance our understanding of the foundations of human speech [3, 6].
Based on the neural expression pattern of the FOXP2/Foxp2 protein, Vargha-Khadem and colleagues [6] formulated a model whereby normal speech relies primarily on the modulation of activity in the ventral motor cortex via cortico-cortical pathways, as well as two major cortico-subcortical pathways, one fronto-striatal and the other fronto-cerebellar. Although abnormalities in the fronto-striatal circuit of affected KE members are well-documented [711], the significance of the abnormal fronto-cerebellar loops remains unexplored. The need for their detailed study is highlighted by the strikingly early and prominent expression of Foxp2/FoxP2/FOXP2 in rodent, avian and human cerebella, respectively, compared with other structures [1219]. Moreover, the cerebellum shows massive computational power (the adult male human cerebellum contains 80% of brain neurons [20]) and an equally striking evolutionary expansion of its hemispheres [21], in concert with their cerebral association input/output areas [22, 23]. Importantly, recent evidence in mice [24] suggests that the cerebellum modulates striatal activity and cortico-striatal plasticity via a short-latency, disynaptic cerebello-striatal pathway. Likewise, the segregated, reciprocal basal ganglia-cerebellum connectivity found in non-human primates [25, 26] suggests an interplay between cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar circuits in motor sequence learning in the adult human brain [27, 28].
Further, neuroimaging evidence from the adult brain implicates cerebellar lobules HVI/HVIIa Crus I,1 left inferior frontal gyrus, premotor and supplementary motor cortex in articulatory rehearsal during verbal working memory encoding [3033] and increased speech complexity [34]. This is in line with recent findings associating the impaired phonological working memory of affected KE members with deficits in subvocal rehearsal of speech-based material [35]. Lobules HVI/HVIIa Crus I also show somatotopically organized responses for complex movements in healthy adults [36].
In view of the above, we used advanced methods to conduct spatially precise analyses of cerebellar MRIs [37] in order to identify structural and functional abnormalities in affected KE members. We predicted that HVI/HVIIa Crus I would show the largest cerebellar structural abnormality in affected KE members relative to both unaffected members and unrelated controls. We expected that these abnormalities would be bilateral, in accordance with our previous findings on early onset speech and language disorders, where unilateral abnormality offers greater opportunity for compensation in the developing brain [38, 39]. This pattern would also be consistent with the bilateral reduction in GM volume in the caudate nucleus, and the increase in the putamen [8], as well as the bilateral HVI/HVIIa Crus I activations for verbal working memory [40] and complex movements [36]. Furthermore, HVI/HVIIa Crus I were expected to show pronounced functional abnormalities (fMRI) during non-word repetition. Problems in this task provide a reliable marker of speech and language impairment, and performance is strongly predicted by oromotor praxis in neurotypical development [41]. We also expected that the asymmetry and/or volume of HVI/HVIIa Crus I would correlate with accuracy in non-word repetition and non-verbal orofacial praxis, two key aspects of the behavioural phenotype of this mutation [42]. Finally, we examined the structural covariance of these lobules with the caudate nucleus in affected KE members, in light of the engagement of cerebellar-basal ganglia circuitry in finely timed motor control [24].

Materials and Methods

We used structural MRI datasets acquired from different subsets of affected KE members at three time points and reported previously (time-point 1: [79, 43]; time-point 2: [44]; time-point 3: [11]) and fMRI data for non-word repetition [11].

Participants

Demographic details of participants in each of the three time points are outlined in Table 1. All of the affected KE members who were available and eligible for brain imaging had been originally scanned at time point 1 (n = 10). At time points 2 (delay from time point 1: mean = 9.17; SD = 0.45 years) and 3 (delay from time point 1: mean = 11.50; SD = 0.58 years), subsets of those individuals (time point 2: n = 6; time-point 3: n = 4) were available for recruitment.
Table 1
Details of affected (‘A’), unaffected (‘U’) KE members or unrelated controls (‘C’) scanned at three time points
Participants
Time point
Groups
N
Age (years)
Sex (n females)
Mean
Min
Max
1
Affected (A1–A10)
10
29.30
9
77
5
Unaffected (U1–U5)
5
15.20
9
21
2
Controls (C1–C9)
9
33.67
21
77
5
2
Affected (A1–A6)
6
33.00
19
57
3
Controls* (C10–C15)
6
34.21
20
63
3
Unaffected (U1–U4, U6–U7)
6
26.02
21
29
3
Controls* (C16–C21)
6
27.16
23
31
3
3
Affected (A1–A4)
4
32.00
22
53
2
Controls* (C22–C25)
4
31.75
22
52
2
*Controls were individually matched for handedness, age (± 6 years), and sex with affected/unaffected members
Time point 1 = four affected and three unaffected members were 9–18 years of age. All others were adults. No overt focal abnormalities were detectable. Unrelated controls and unaffected members had no known history of speech-language, neurological, hearing or developmental impairment. All were native English speakers

Data Acquisition

MRI (Time Point 1–3)

Details of structural MRI acquisition at each time point are reported in Table S1.

fMRI (Time Point 3)

Further details on this study can be found in [11]. Briefly, here, two runs of 60 volumes were collected for each participant at time point 3 (see Table 1), with five task/baseline blocks (one block = 6 volumes) per run. During the task period (non-word repetition), each non-word was presented via headphones and was immediately repeated aloud by the participant. During baseline, white noise bursts were presented.

Cerebellar Morphometry and Lobular Volumetry (Time Points 1–3)

We combined cerebellum-specific voxel-based morphometry (VBM) and lobular volumetry using SUIT (v. 3.1; http://www.icn.ucl.ac.uk/motorcontrol/imaging/suit.htm; [37]) in SPM12 (v. 6225; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) running in Matlab 2015a (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Compared with normalization to the MNI whole-brain template, SUIT provides stronger contrast for the cerebellum, improving fissure overlap among subjects by reducing spatial variance to 1/3.

Cerebellum-Specific VBM (Time Points 1–3)

T1-weighted images were re-orientated so that the origin coordinates lay over the anterior commissure and segmented into grey matter (GM), white matter (WM) and CSF, using the unified segmentation procedure [45]. The cerebellum and brainstem were isolated and a mask was created per scan, which was manually corrected using MRICron [46] (http://​www.​mccauslandcenter​.​sc.​edu/​mricro/​mricron) as non-cerebellar regions (e.g. transverse sinus, bone marrow) are occasionally misclassified as cerebellar GM [37]. Using SUIT’s DARTEL-algorithm, the cerebellum was deformed to fit the probability maps of cortical GM and WM to an atlas template. Nonlinear deformation was applied to GM segmentation maps, which were modulated to compensate for volume changes during normalization, by multiplying the intensity value in each voxel with Jacobian determinants. The amount of GM signal in normalized images was thus preserved, with VBM statistics reflecting GM volume differences [47, 48]. Images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM), in line with previous cerebellar VBM studies [49, 50]. GM differences between groups were assessed by voxel-wise t test analyses. Whenever participants in the groups were not individually matched, sex and age were entered as between-subjects covariates (ANCOVA). Comparisons were conducted separately for each subset/time point. Given the small smoothing kernels employed for spatial precision in these analyses, we applied stringent corrections for multiple comparisons (voxel peak- or cluster-level familywise error (FWE)-correction: p < .005) and non-stationary smoothness [51] over an individual voxel threshold of p < .001.

Cerebellar Lobular Volumetry (Time Points 1–3)

The procedure involved cropping and isolating the cerebellum, SUIT normalization, re-slicing the cerebellar atlas into subject space using the deformation parameters from normalization and calculating the number of voxels in each lobule in the re-sliced images. One of the authors (GPDA) corrected the cerebellar isolation masks and the re-sliced cerebellar atlas while blind to the participant’s identity. This process resulted in volumetric measurements of cerebellar lobules (left, right I–IV, V; left, medial [vermal], or right hemispheric VI, VIIa Crus I, VIIa Crus II, VIIb, VIIIa, VIIIb, IX and X). For all comparisons, volumes were expressed both in cc and as a proportion of cerebellar cortical volume. As in VBM, ANCOVAs were used with age and sex as covariates. We predicted a main effect of group for Crus I volume, and a group × lobule interaction. Comparisons were conducted separately for each time-point. Significance for two-tailed tests was set at p < .05, using Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) to correct for multiple comparisons. Violations of the sphericity assumption (tested by Mauchly’s W) were followed by a Huynh-Feldt correction of degrees of freedom. Hemispheric asymmetry was calculated for Crus I using the formula (right − left/right + left hemisphere) [52].

Structure-Function Relationships (Time Point 1)

We examined the correlation (Pearson’s r; SPSS, v. 22) of Crus I volume and asymmetry with the behavioural measures reported to fully dissociate affected from unaffected members [42] and to correlate with the affected members’ caudate volume (time point 1; [8]): (i) Non-word repetition: Participants heard and repeated 40 non-words [53], half of which contained consonant clusters (‘complex’ non-words). The dependent measure was the number of complex non-words accurately repeated; (ii) Non-verbal orofacial praxis: Α rating scale was used to assess the performance of orofacial movements [42]. We predicted that the number of accurately repeated complex non-words and the orofacial movement ratings would correlate with Crus I volumetric measures from the same time-point.

fMRI Analysis (Time Point 3)

After realignment, preprocessing of EPIs followed the same steps as in cerebellar VBM, apart from modulation. We compared task (non-word repetition) vs. baseline (listening to white noise bursts), using the fixed-effects analysis employed in [11], reporting regions active in the group as a whole (time point 3). We identified regions that were both activated in controls (n = 4; inclusive mask threshold for ‘task > baseline: p < .01) and less/more active in affected members (n = 4). The same stringent FWE corrections were applied as those in our VBM analyses.

Structural Covariance of VIIa Crus I and Caudate Nucleus (Time Point 1)

In order to explore the structural covariance of the caudate nucleus with lobules that were structurally/functionally abnormal in affected KE family members, we first examined the correlation of the previously measured volumes of the caudate nuclei corrected for total intracranial volume (TICV) from time point 1 [8] with GM volume across the whole brain (VBM regression). The pre-processing pipeline was the same as that for cerebellar VBM, with the exception that GM images were normalized to MNI space by generating a group-specific whole-brain template (DARTEL), and an 8-mm3 FWHM smoothing kernel was used. Age and sex were entered as covariates and total caudate volume as a main effect regressor. We expected to identify clusters in the caudate nuclei bilaterally, given that volumetry and VBM measure the same effects [54], but also in structures the volume of which would covary with the caudate nucleus, probably due to common experience-related plasticity or mutually trophic influences, driven by environmental and genetic factors [48]. We subsequently focused on bivariate correlations (Pearson’s r) between TICV-corrected left/right/total caudate nucleus volumes and TICV-corrected left/right hemispheric/medial/total volumes of lobules that showed structural/functional abnormalities consistently across time-points.

Results

Groups did not differ in TICV, whole-brain, or cerebellar cortical GM volume (between-groups comparisons across time points: p ≥ .10; Table 2).
Table 2
TICV, whole-brain GM and cerebellar cortical GM for affected, unaffected KE family members, and unrelated controls in each of the three time points; F and p values pertain to between-subjects ANOVAs
Time point
Group
TICV (cc)
Whole-brain GM (cc)
Cerebellar cortex GM (cc)
Mean (SD)
F (p)
Mean (SD)
F (p)
Mean (SD)
F (p)
1
Affected
1465.63 (116.49)
0.42 (0.66)
1043.30 (65.10)
1.13 (0.34)
122.62 (9.85)
2.22 (0.13)
Unaffected
1531.15 (153.92)
1101.56 (129.95)
135.95 (13.19)
Controls
1504.50 (151.35)
1009.64 (135.60)
131.95 (15.31)
2
Affected
1353.00 (84.52)
1.11 (0.35)
979.61 (67.72)
0.71 (0.51)
120.94 (11.34)
2.58 (0.10)
Unaffected
1447.02 (157.93)
1046.18 (106.40)
134.40 (12.45)
Controls
1456.45 (156.60)
1032.54 (117.80)
140.90 (21.47)
3
Affected
1479.96 (93.92)
2.64 (0.16)
1010.78 (27.92)
0.06 (0.81)
107.29 (7.76)
2.72 (0.15)
Controls
1578.07 (75.77)
1024.14 (100.52)
119.28 (12.29)

Cerebellar Lobular Volumetry (Time Points 1–3)

Across time points, affected members showed smaller Crus I volume relative to unaffected members and unrelated controls (Fig. 1; Table 3; time point 1 = − 18%; time point 2 = − 21%; time point 3 = − 21%), but did not differ consistently in any other lobule (Table S2). Results were replicated with (a) age and sex as between-subjects covariates (Table S3); (b) when left, right hemispheric and medial volumes were compared separately (Fig. S1); (c) when volume was expressed as percent cerebellar cortex (Table S3; Fig. S2). Unaffected KE members did not differ from unrelated controls in any lobule (time points 1 and 2).
Table 3
All post hoc tests were HSD-corrected for multiple comparisons; ‘affected’ = affected KE members; ‘unaffected’ = unaffected KE members; ‘controls’ = unrelated controls; the dependent measure is volume expressed in cc
Time points
Group comparisons
1
Between-subjects ANOVAs
Group: VIIa Crus I: F(2,21) = 6.18, p = .008
(Group: affected, unaffected, controls)
(Affected vs. controls: p = .033; affected vs. unaffected: p = .014)
Mixed-effects ANOVA
Group × lobule: F(4.71, 49.41) = 4.03, p = .004
(Group: affected, unaffected, controls; lobule: I–X)
2
Pairwise t tests (affected vs. controls*)
VIIa Crus I: t(5) = − 3.89, p = .012
Repeated measures ANOVA
Group × lobule: F(1.80, 9.00) = 9.65, p = .007
(Group: affected, controls*; lobule: I–X)
Between-subjects ANOVA
Group: VIIa Crus I: F(2,15) = 7.66, p = .005
(Group: affected, unaffected, controls)
(Affected vs. controls: p = .008; affected vs. unaffected: p = .014)
Mixed-effects ANOVA
Group × lobule: F(4.95,37.11) = 4.00, p = .005
(Group: affected, unaffected, controls; lobule: I–X)
3
Paired samples t test (affected vs. controls*)
VIIa Crus I: t(3) = −5.12, p = .014
Repeated-measures ANOVA
Group × lobule: F(5.76, 17.27) = 14.73, p = .000007
(Group: affected, controls*; lobule: I–X)
*Controls were individually matched for handedness, age (± 6 years), and sex with affected/unaffected members

Cerebellum-Specific VBM (Time Points 1–3)

The VBM analyses showed a number of regions with reduced GM volume in affected KE members relative to unrelated controls and unaffected KE members. These included medial IV–VI and hemispheric portions of VIIb–VIIIb. Nevertheless, the only discrepancy consistent across time points and comparison groups was the GM volume reduction in HVIIa Crus I of affected members (Fig. 2; Tables S46; Fig. S3), shown to be bilateral in most comparisons.

Structure-Function Analysis (Tim Point 1)

The number of accurately repeated complex non-words and the orofacial praxis ratings correlated negatively with VIIa Crus I volumetric measures (Fig. 3), but not with any other lobular volumes (all ps, p > .15). The same negative correlations have already been reported for the right and total caudate nucleus volume [8]. Using a series of partial correlation analyses, we thus sought to examine whether right/total HVIIa Crus I volumes would correlate with these scores above and beyond right/total caudate nucleus volumes, and vice versa. As expected, no significant partial correlation was shown, given the strong positive volumetric correlations of these two structures in affected KE members (Table S7).

fMRI Analysis (Time Point 3)

There was reduced activity in HVI/HVIIa Crus I bilaterally in affected KE members relative to matched controls during non-word repetition compared with the noise-burst baseline (Fig. 4).

Structural Covariance of HVIIa Crus I and Caudate Nucleus (Time Point 1)

Crus I and caudate nuclei showed structural covariance in affected KE members. This was seen in whole-brain VBM regression, where total caudate volume expectedly correlated bilaterally with GM volume in the caudate nuclei (Fig. S4), but also with regions in the right HVIIa Crus I and the left supplementary motor area (SMA) (Fig. 5). The same relationship was seen between the previously measured volumes of the caudate nuclei [8] and those of Crus I (Fig. 6).

Discussion

The speech and language deficits in half the members of the KE family are associated with a point mutation in FOXP2. Its neural and behavioural phenotype may shed light on the ontogenetic and phylogenetic foundations of articulate speech. The structural and functional abnormalities in the fronto-striatal circuitry of affected KE members have been well documented. In particular, the caudate volume reduction bilaterally represents a fundamental component of this neural phenotype that is associated with key aspects of the behavioural phenotype of this mutation. Nevertheless, very little has been known so far about the fronto-cerebellar circuits, despite the very early expression pattern of the FOXP2/Foxp2 protein in the cerebellum across species.
In this study, we identified a pronounced volume reduction (≈ 20% relative to unaffected members and unrelated controls) bilaterally in cerebellar VIIa Crus I in affected KE family members at all three of the different time points of MRI data collection. Their right hemispheric and total cerebellar Crus I volume correlated with their impaired performance in complex non-word repetition and non-verbal orofacial praxis. These two test scores reflect the core of the behavioural phenotype of this mutation [42]. Consistent with these structure-function relationships, the same lobule also showed hypoactivation bilaterally in non-word repetition. Importantly, the right hemispheric Crus I volume of affected members positively correlated with that of their left caudate nucleus, showing the same negative correlation with non-word repetition accuracy as that observed for the right (and total) caudate nucleus [8].
Our findings may thus reflect the presence of abnormalities in a cerebellar-striatal loop comprising HVIIa Crus I and the caudate nucleus. This proposal is based on evidence for reciprocal cerebellar-striatal connectivity in non-human primates [25, 26] and its role in finely timed motor control and learning in rodents [24]. It is also in line with human brain imaging studies that demonstrate resting-state functional connectivity of Crus I with the caudate nuclei [56] and the interplay of cortico-striatal with cortico-cerebellar circuits in motor sequence learning (e.g. [27, 28]. This is further supported by recent evidence for GM reduction in the caudate nucleus in patients with cerebellar atrophy [57], as well as findings highlighting the involvement of cerebellar pathology in disorders of the basal ganglia [5860].
There are at least two possible explanations of the abnormalities described here. Firstly, Crus I regions may support speech motor sequencing across the lifespan. Studies on neurotypical adults suggest that HVI/HVIIa Crus I support motor speech sequencing [6163], and are somatotopically organized selectively for the production of complex motor sequences [36]. While cerebellar damage is associated with ataxic dysarthria [64], the diminished sequence length effects on speech reaction times noted in cases of ataxic dysarthria have been held to reflect impaired ‘motor programming’ [65]. Interestingly, a recent study has shown effects of non-invasive stimulation of the right HVIIa Crus I/II on phonological errors in speech production (addition, deletion, transposition of phonemes) in neurotypical adults [66]. Crossed cerebellar diaschisis-related phenomena may also play an important role in apraxias of speech [6771]. Indeed, Broca’s area (44/45), which also shows structural and functional abnormalities in affected KE members [811], is embedded in common functional networks with the adult HVI/HVIIa Crus I [72]. This circuitry is activated during verbal working memory encoding, motor rehearsal [30, 3234], and modulated by difficulty in overt non-word reading [61].
A second explanation is that Crus I regions are selectively important in the pre-automatic stage of motor learning in speech acquisition. Evidence that apraxia of speech occurs as a cerebellar syndrome is quite limited [73]. There is, however, strong support for a cerebellar role in the acquisition of complex motor sequences [7476]. With extended practice, cerebellar cortical activation decreases, paralleled by increases in cortico-striatal circuits [27], with regions close to SMA ultimately representing the automatized sequence [77]. Characteristically, the monkey pre-SMA, which, unlike SMA, is reciprocally connected with Crus I/II [78], is engaged in early motor sequence learning [79]. Similarly, pre-automatic processing of motor sequences is associated with activity in prefrontal cortex and HVI/HVIIa Crus I [80]. Regions in the caudate nucleus and Crus I are embedded within the ‘fronto-parietal control network’ [72, 81], which may be engaged during initial motor sequence learning (e.g. [82]). This dovetails with proposals that cerebellar integrity is of greater importance in earlier developmental stages [83]. Similar proposals have been made for the striatum in speech acquisition [84, 85].
Further research is required to assess these explanations. This would include larger group sizes to examine the speech-related functional abnormalities observed in Crus I for the affected KE members, given the small sample sizes analysed here. T1- and T2-weighted MRI should also be conducted at higher field strength, in order to examine structural and functional abnormalities in the dentate nucleus, to which Purkinje cells of Crus I project. Moreover, our findings do not contradict the VIIb and VIIIb volume reductions in affected KE members reported earlier [8, 9], as some contrasts here disclosed reductions in medial IV–VI and hemispheric VIIb–VIIIb. The former are the loci for the somatotopic representation of the orofacial musculature [86]; the latter may be associated with the involvement of lobule VIII in auditory [87] and somatosensory feedback processing [88]. Finally, considering the inverse correlations of non-word repetition accuracy with both Crus I and right caudate volumes, it is likely that neurodevelopmental compensatory mechanisms, functional reorganization for caudate/Crus I reduction (see discussion in [8]), and additional disruptions in synaptic pruning could be involved.

Conclusion

Consistent with the early, homologous expression pattern of FOXP2/Foxp2 in the human/rodent cerebellar cortical Purkinje cells, the neurodevelopmental abnormalities that we identify in lobule HVIIa Crus I of the affected members of the KE family may compromise the capacity of cerebellar-striatal circuitry to execute complex oromotor sequences. Our findings thus point towards abnormality in a circuit linking cerebellar lobule HVIIa Crus I with the caudate nucleus. This loop may be a fundamental component of the neural apparatus that enables the finely timed coordination of complex oromotor sequences needed for human speech. Research on the neurobiology of speech and language acquisition and processing needs to examine the complex interplay between cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar circuits, rather than their contributions in isolation from each other.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the members of the KE family and all the other participants for their continued cooperation. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Ethics Committee of the Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Trust, the Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Fußnoten
1
We follow the nomenclature used in the ‘MRI Atlas of the Human Cerebellum’ [29]: cerebellar lobules are labelled ‘I-X’ from the anterior-superior border, through posterior-superior, posterior-inferior, to the anterior-inferior border. The nomenclature is based on that of Larsell and Jansen [21], where cerebellar hemispheres are distinguished from the vermis with the ‘H’ prefix. The atlas of Schmahmann and colleagues [29] uses this prefix to refer specifically to the hemispheres, and the adjective ‘vermal’ to refer to vermal compartments of a lobule. No prefix is used when referring to the entire lobule.
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Hurst JA, Baraitser M, Auger E, Graham F, Norell S. An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1990;32:352–5.CrossRefPubMed Hurst JA, Baraitser M, Auger E, Graham F, Norell S. An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1990;32:352–5.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Passingham RE. Speech and language defects. Nature. 1990;346:226.CrossRef Vargha-Khadem F, Passingham RE. Speech and language defects. Nature. 1990;346:226.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R. Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:930–3.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R. Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:930–3.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder, vol. 413. London: Nature Nature Publishing Group; 2001. p. 519–23. Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder, vol. 413. London: Nature Nature Publishing Group; 2001. p. 519–23.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:131–8.CrossRefPubMed Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:131–8.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Price CJ, Ashburner J, Alcock KJ, Connelly A, et al. Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:12695–700.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Price CJ, Ashburner J, Alcock KJ, Connelly A, et al. Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:12695–700.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, Friston KJ, et al. MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain. 2002;125:465–78.CrossRefPubMed Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, Friston KJ, et al. MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain. 2002;125:465–78.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Belton E, Salmond CH, Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG. Bilateral brain abnormalities associated with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18:194–200.CrossRefPubMed Belton E, Salmond CH, Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG. Bilateral brain abnormalities associated with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18:194–200.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Liégeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1230–7.CrossRefPubMed Liégeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1230–7.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Liégeois F, Morgan AT, Connelly A, Vargha-Khadem F. Endophenotypes of FOXP2: dysfunction within the human articulatory network. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2011;15:283–8.CrossRefPubMed Liégeois F, Morgan AT, Connelly A, Vargha-Khadem F. Endophenotypes of FOXP2: dysfunction within the human articulatory network. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2011;15:283–8.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lai CSL, Gerrelli D, Monaco AP, Fisher SE, Copp AJ. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain. 2003;126:2455–62.CrossRefPubMed Lai CSL, Gerrelli D, Monaco AP, Fisher SE, Copp AJ. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain. 2003;126:2455–62.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferland RJ, Cherry TJ, Preware PO, Morrisey EE, Walsh CA. Characterization of Foxp2 and Foxp1 mRNA and protein in the developing and mature brain. J Comp Neurol. 2003;460:266–79. Ferland RJ, Cherry TJ, Preware PO, Morrisey EE, Walsh CA. Characterization of Foxp2 and Foxp1 mRNA and protein in the developing and mature brain. J Comp Neurol. 2003;460:266–79.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Shu W, Cho JY, Jiang Y, Zhang M, Weisz D, Elder GA, et al. Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9643–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Shu W, Cho JY, Jiang Y, Zhang M, Weisz D, Elder GA, et al. Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9643–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Shiota A, Ueda M, Suwa K, Momoi MY, et al. Ultrasonic vocalization impairment of Foxp2 (R552H) knockin mice related to speech-language disorder and abnormality of Purkinje cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:3117–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Shiota A, Ueda M, Suwa K, Momoi MY, et al. Ultrasonic vocalization impairment of Foxp2 (R552H) knockin mice related to speech-language disorder and abnormality of Purkinje cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:3117–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Momoi MY, Momoi T. Cntnap2 expression in the cerebellum of Foxp2(R552H) mice, with a mutation related to speech-language disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2012;506:277–80.CrossRefPubMed Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Momoi MY, Momoi T. Cntnap2 expression in the cerebellum of Foxp2(R552H) mice, with a mutation related to speech-language disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2012;506:277–80.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi K, Liu FC, Hirokawa K, Takahashi H. Expression of Foxp2, a gene involved in speech and language, in the developing and adult striatum. J Neurosci Res. 2003;73:61–72.CrossRefPubMed Takahashi K, Liu FC, Hirokawa K, Takahashi H. Expression of Foxp2, a gene involved in speech and language, in the developing and adult striatum. J Neurosci Res. 2003;73:61–72.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell AJ, Lyne L, Brown PJ, Launchbury RJ, Bignone P, Chi J, et al. Aberrant expression of the neuronal transcription factor FOXP2 in neoplastic plasma cells. Br J Haematol. 2010;149:221–30.CrossRefPubMed Campbell AJ, Lyne L, Brown PJ, Launchbury RJ, Bignone P, Chi J, et al. Aberrant expression of the neuronal transcription factor FOXP2 in neoplastic plasma cells. Br J Haematol. 2010;149:221–30.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA. Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3152–63.CrossRefPubMed Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA. Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3152–63.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Azevedo FAC, Carvalho LRB, Grinberg LT, Farfel JM, Ferretti REL, Leite REP, et al. Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. J Comp Neurol. 2009;513:532–41.CrossRefPubMed Azevedo FAC, Carvalho LRB, Grinberg LT, Farfel JM, Ferretti REL, Leite REP, et al. Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. J Comp Neurol. 2009;513:532–41.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsell O, Jansen J. The comparative anatomy and histology of the cerebellum. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press; 1967. Larsell O, Jansen J. The comparative anatomy and histology of the cerebellum. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press; 1967.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS. Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:443–54.CrossRefPubMed Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS. Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:443–54.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Whiting B, Barton R. The evolution of the cortico-cerebellar complex in primates: anatomical connections predict patterns of correlated evolution. J Hum Evol. 2003;44:3–10.CrossRefPubMed Whiting B, Barton R. The evolution of the cortico-cerebellar complex in primates: anatomical connections predict patterns of correlated evolution. J Hum Evol. 2003;44:3–10.CrossRefPubMed
24.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoshi E, Tremblay L, Féger J, Carras PL, Strick PL. The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1491–3.CrossRefPubMed Hoshi E, Tremblay L, Féger J, Carras PL, Strick PL. The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1491–3.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Doyon J, Song AW, Karni A, Lalonde F, Adams MM, Ungerleider LG. Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:1017–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Doyon J, Song AW, Karni A, Lalonde F, Adams MM, Ungerleider LG. Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:1017–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Tzvi E, Stoldt A, Witt K, Krämer UM. Striatal-cerebellar networks mediate consolidation in a motor sequence learning task: an fMRI study using dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2015;122:52–64.CrossRefPubMed Tzvi E, Stoldt A, Witt K, Krämer UM. Striatal-cerebellar networks mediate consolidation in a motor sequence learning task: an fMRI study using dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2015;122:52–64.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD, Doyon J, Toga AW, Petrides M, Evans A. MRI atlas of the human cerebellum. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000. Schmahmann JD, Doyon J, Toga AW, Petrides M, Evans A. MRI atlas of the human cerebellum. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Desmond JE, Gabrieli JD, Wagner AD, Ginier BL, Glover GH. Lobular patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working-memory and finger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI. J Neurosci. 1997;17:9675–85.CrossRefPubMed Desmond JE, Gabrieli JD, Wagner AD, Ginier BL, Glover GH. Lobular patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working-memory and finger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI. J Neurosci. 1997;17:9675–85.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Chein JM, Fiez JA. Dissociation of verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1003–14.CrossRefPubMed Chein JM, Fiez JA. Dissociation of verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1003–14.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen SHA, Desmond JE. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. NeuroImage. 2005;24:332–8.CrossRefPubMed Chen SHA, Desmond JE. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. NeuroImage. 2005;24:332–8.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen SHA, Desmond JE. Temporal dynamics of cerebro-cerebellar network recruitment during a cognitive task. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1227–37.CrossRefPubMed Chen SHA, Desmond JE. Temporal dynamics of cerebro-cerebellar network recruitment during a cognitive task. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1227–37.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Marvel CL, Desmond JE. The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex. 2010;46:880–95.CrossRefPubMed Marvel CL, Desmond JE. The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex. 2010;46:880–95.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulze K, Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Phonological working memory and FOXP2. Neuropsychol Pergamon. 2018;108:147–52.CrossRef Schulze K, Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Phonological working memory and FOXP2. Neuropsychol Pergamon. 2018;108:147–52.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Schlerf JE, Verstynen TD, Ivry RB, Spencer RMC. Evidence of a novel somatopic map in the human neocerebellum during complex actions. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:3330–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schlerf JE, Verstynen TD, Ivry RB, Spencer RMC. Evidence of a novel somatopic map in the human neocerebellum during complex actions. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:3330–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Diedrichsen J. A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2006;33:127–38.CrossRefPubMed Diedrichsen J. A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2006;33:127–38.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Speech and language outcome after hemispherectomy in childhood. In: Tuxhorn I, Holthausen H, Boenigk HE, editors. Paediatr epilepsy Syndr their Surg treat. Montrouge: John Libbey & Co. Ltd. Medical Books; 1997. p. 774–84. Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Speech and language outcome after hemispherectomy in childhood. In: Tuxhorn I, Holthausen H, Boenigk HE, editors. Paediatr epilepsy Syndr their Surg treat. Montrouge: John Libbey & Co. Ltd. Medical Books; 1997. p. 774–84.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Carr LJ, Isaacs E, Brett E, Adams C, Mishkin M. Onset of speech after left hemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy. Brain. 1997;120:159–82.CrossRefPubMed Vargha-Khadem F, Carr LJ, Isaacs E, Brett E, Adams C, Mishkin M. Onset of speech after left hemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy. Brain. 1997;120:159–82.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1560–70.CrossRefPubMed Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1560–70.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Krishnan S, Alcock KJ, Carey D, Bergström L, Karmiloff-Smith A, Dick F. Fractionating nonword repetition: The contributions of short-term memory and oromotor praxis are different. PLoS One. Public Libr Sci; 2017;12:e0178356. Krishnan S, Alcock KJ, Carey D, Bergström L, Karmiloff-Smith A, Dick F. Fractionating nonword repetition: The contributions of short-term memory and oromotor praxis are different. PLoS One. Public Libr Sci; 2017;12:e0178356.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Watkins KE, Dronkers NF, Vargha-Khadem F. Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain. 2002;125:452–64.CrossRefPubMed Watkins KE, Dronkers NF, Vargha-Khadem F. Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain. 2002;125:452–64.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Watkins KE, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F. Functional and structural brain abnormalities associated with a genetic disorder of speech and language. Am J Hum Genet. 1999;65:1215–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Watkins KE, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F. Functional and structural brain abnormalities associated with a genetic disorder of speech and language. Am J Hum Genet. 1999;65:1215–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Belton E. The neural correlates of developmental speech and language disorders: a neuropsychological and neuroimaging investigation. University College London: Institute of Child Health; 2004. Belton E. The neural correlates of developmental speech and language disorders: a neuropsychological and neuroimaging investigation. University College London: Institute of Child Health; 2004.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Rorden C, Karnath H-OO, Bonilha L. Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1081–8.CrossRefPubMed Rorden C, Karnath H-OO, Bonilha L. Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1081–8.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36.CrossRef Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ashburner J. Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Curr Med Imaging Rev. 2005;1:105–13.CrossRef Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ashburner J. Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Curr Med Imaging Rev. 2005;1:105–13.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat D’Agata F, Caroppo P, Baudino B, Caglio M, Croce M, Bergui M, et al. The recognition of facial emotions in spinocerebellar ataxia patients. Cerebellum. 2011;10:600–10.CrossRefPubMed D’Agata F, Caroppo P, Baudino B, Caglio M, Croce M, Bergui M, et al. The recognition of facial emotions in spinocerebellar ataxia patients. Cerebellum. 2011;10:600–10.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Peterburs J, Thürling M, Rustemeier M, Göricke S, Suchan B, Timmann D, et al. A cerebellar role in performance monitoring—evidence from EEG and voxel-based morphometry in patients with cerebellar degenerative disease. Neuropsychologia. 2015;68:139–47.CrossRefPubMed Peterburs J, Thürling M, Rustemeier M, Göricke S, Suchan B, Timmann D, et al. A cerebellar role in performance monitoring—evidence from EEG and voxel-based morphometry in patients with cerebellar degenerative disease. Neuropsychologia. 2015;68:139–47.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayasaka S, Phan KLL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE. Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. NeuroImage. 2004;22:676–87.CrossRefPubMed Hayasaka S, Phan KLL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE. Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. NeuroImage. 2004;22:676–87.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Snyder PJ, Bilder RM, Wu H, Bogerts B, Lieberman JA. Cerebellar volume asymmetries are related to handedness: a quantitative MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:407–19.CrossRefPubMed Snyder PJ, Bilder RM, Wu H, Bogerts B, Lieberman JA. Cerebellar volume asymmetries are related to handedness: a quantitative MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:407–19.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. J Mem Lang. 1989;28:200–13.CrossRef Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. J Mem Lang. 1989;28:200–13.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Focke NK, Trost S, Paulus W, Falkai P, Gruber O. Do manual and voxel-based morphometry measure the same? A proof of concept study. Front Psychiatry Frontiers. 2014;5:39. Focke NK, Trost S, Paulus W, Falkai P, Gruber O. Do manual and voxel-based morphometry measure the same? A proof of concept study. Front Psychiatry Frontiers. 2014;5:39.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Sang L, Qin W, Liu Y, Han W, Zhang Y, Jiang T, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity of the vermal and hemispheric subregions of the cerebellum with both the cerebral cortical networks and subcortical structures. NeuroImage. 2012;61:1213–25.CrossRefPubMed Sang L, Qin W, Liu Y, Han W, Zhang Y, Jiang T, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity of the vermal and hemispheric subregions of the cerebellum with both the cerebral cortical networks and subcortical structures. NeuroImage. 2012;61:1213–25.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Dayan M, Olivito G, Molinari M, Cercignani M, Bozzali M, Leggio M. Impact of cerebellar atrophy on cortical gray matter and cerebellar peduncles as assessed by voxel-based morphometry and high angular resolution diffusion imaging. Funct Neurol. 2016;31:239–48.PubMed Dayan M, Olivito G, Molinari M, Cercignani M, Bozzali M, Leggio M. Impact of cerebellar atrophy on cortical gray matter and cerebellar peduncles as assessed by voxel-based morphometry and high angular resolution diffusion imaging. Funct Neurol. 2016;31:239–48.PubMed
58.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Dirkx MF, den Ouden HEM, Aarts E, Timmer MHM, Bloem BR, Toni I, et al. Dopamine controls Parkinson’s tremor by inhibiting the cerebellar thalamus. Brain. Oxford University Press; 2017;83:aww331. Dirkx MF, den Ouden HEM, Aarts E, Timmer MHM, Bloem BR, Toni I, et al. Dopamine controls Parkinson’s tremor by inhibiting the cerebellar thalamus. Brain. Oxford University Press; 2017;83:aww331.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Callaghan C, Hornberger M, Balsters JH, Halliday GM, Lewis SJG, Shine JM. Cerebellar atrophy in Parkinson’s disease and its implication for network connectivity. Brain. 2016;139:845–55.CrossRefPubMed O’Callaghan C, Hornberger M, Balsters JH, Halliday GM, Lewis SJG, Shine JM. Cerebellar atrophy in Parkinson’s disease and its implication for network connectivity. Brain. 2016;139:845–55.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Peeva MG, Guenther FH, Tourville JA, Nieto-Castanon A, Anton J-LL, Nazarian B, et al. Distinct representations of phonemes, syllables, and supra-syllabic sequences in the speech production network. Neuroimage. Elsevier Inc.; 2010;50:626–38. Peeva MG, Guenther FH, Tourville JA, Nieto-Castanon A, Anton J-LL, Nazarian B, et al. Distinct representations of phonemes, syllables, and supra-syllabic sequences in the speech production network. Neuroimage. Elsevier Inc.; 2010;50:626–38.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Fulbright RK, Jenner AR, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, et al. The cerebellum’s role in reading: a functional MR imaging study. Am J Neuroradiol. 1999;20:1925–30.PubMed Fulbright RK, Jenner AR, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, et al. The cerebellum’s role in reading: a functional MR imaging study. Am J Neuroradiol. 1999;20:1925–30.PubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Bohland JW, Guenther FH. An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. NeuroImage. 2006;32:821–41.CrossRefPubMed Bohland JW, Guenther FH. An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. NeuroImage. 2006;32:821–41.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan AT, Liégeois F, Liederkerke C, Vogel AP, Hayward R, Harkness W, et al. Role of cerebellum in fine speech control in childhood: persistent dysarthria after surgical treatment for posterior fossa tumour. Brain Lang. 2011;117:69–76.CrossRefPubMed Morgan AT, Liégeois F, Liederkerke C, Vogel AP, Hayward R, Harkness W, et al. Role of cerebellum in fine speech control in childhood: persistent dysarthria after surgical treatment for posterior fossa tumour. Brain Lang. 2011;117:69–76.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer KA, Rogers MA. Speech motor programming in hypokinetic and ataxic dysarthria. Brain Lang. 2005;94:347–66.CrossRefPubMed Spencer KA, Rogers MA. Speech motor programming in hypokinetic and ataxic dysarthria. Brain Lang. 2005;94:347–66.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Runnqvist E, Bonnard M, Gauvin HS, Attarian S, Trébuchon A, Hartsuiker RJ, et al. Internal modeling of upcoming speech: a causal role of the right posterior cerebellum in non-motor aspects of language production. Cortex. 2016;81:203–14.CrossRefPubMed Runnqvist E, Bonnard M, Gauvin HS, Attarian S, Trébuchon A, Hartsuiker RJ, et al. Internal modeling of upcoming speech: a causal role of the right posterior cerebellum in non-motor aspects of language production. Cortex. 2016;81:203–14.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Mariën P, Engelborghs S, Fabbro F, De Deyn PP. The lateralized linguistic cerebellum: a review and a new hypothesis. Brain Lang. 2001;79:580–600.CrossRefPubMed Mariën P, Engelborghs S, Fabbro F, De Deyn PP. The lateralized linguistic cerebellum: a review and a new hypothesis. Brain Lang. 2001;79:580–600.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Mariën P, Verhoeven J, Engelborghs S, Rooker S, Pickut BA, De Deyn PP. A role for the cerebellum in motor speech planning: evidence from foreign accent syndrome. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2006;108:518–22.CrossRefPubMed Mariën P, Verhoeven J, Engelborghs S, Rooker S, Pickut BA, De Deyn PP. A role for the cerebellum in motor speech planning: evidence from foreign accent syndrome. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2006;108:518–22.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Mariën P, Verhoeven J. Cerebellar involvement in motor speech planning: some further evidence from foreign accent syndrome. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2007;59:210–7.CrossRefPubMed Mariën P, Verhoeven J. Cerebellar involvement in motor speech planning: some further evidence from foreign accent syndrome. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2007;59:210–7.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen DA, Kurowski K, Steven MS, Blumstein SE, Pascual-Leone A. Paradoxical facilitation: the resolution of foreign accent syndrome after cerebellar stroke. Neurology. 2009;73:566–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cohen DA, Kurowski K, Steven MS, Blumstein SE, Pascual-Leone A. Paradoxical facilitation: the resolution of foreign accent syndrome after cerebellar stroke. Neurology. 2009;73:566–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Keulen S, Mariën P, Wackenier P, Jonkers R, Bastiaanse R, Verhoeven J. Developmental foreign accent syndrome: report of a new case. Front Hum Neurosci Front. 2016;10:65. Keulen S, Mariën P, Wackenier P, Jonkers R, Bastiaanse R, Verhoeven J. Developmental foreign accent syndrome: report of a new case. Front Hum Neurosci Front. 2016;10:65.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:2322–45.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:2322–45.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Ziegler W. The phonetic cerebellum: cerebellar involvement in speech sound production. In: Mariën P, Manto M, editors. Linguist Cerebellum. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2016. p. 1–32. Ziegler W. The phonetic cerebellum: cerebellar involvement in speech sound production. In: Mariën P, Manto M, editors. Linguist Cerebellum. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2016. p. 1–32.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Carrera E, Tononi G, Abe O, Okubo T, Hayashi N, Saito N, et al. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain. 2014;137:2408–22.CrossRefPubMed Carrera E, Tononi G, Abe O, Okubo T, Hayashi N, Saito N, et al. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain. 2014;137:2408–22.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Laforce R, Doyon J. Distinct contribution of the striatum and cerebellum to motor learning. Brain Cogn. 2001;45:189–211.CrossRefPubMed Laforce R, Doyon J. Distinct contribution of the striatum and cerebellum to motor learning. Brain Cogn. 2001;45:189–211.CrossRefPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang CE, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar damage impairs automaticity of a recently practiced movement. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1336–47.CrossRefPubMed Lang CE, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar damage impairs automaticity of a recently practiced movement. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1336–47.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanji J, Shima K. Supplementary motor cortex in organization of movement. Eur Neurol. 1996;36:13–9.CrossRefPubMed Tanji J, Shima K. Supplementary motor cortex in organization of movement. Eur Neurol. 1996;36:13–9.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8432–44.CrossRefPubMed Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8432–44.CrossRefPubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakamura K, Sakai K, Hikosaka O. Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:2671–87.CrossRefPubMed Nakamura K, Sakai K, Hikosaka O. Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:2671–87.CrossRefPubMed
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Sakai K, Hikosaka O, Nakamura K. Emergence of rhythm during motor learning. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:547–53.CrossRefPubMed Sakai K, Hikosaka O, Nakamura K. Emergence of rhythm during motor learning. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:547–53.CrossRefPubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Choi EY, Yeo BTT, Buckner RL. The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:2242–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Choi EY, Yeo BTT, Buckner RL. The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:2242–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Jenkins IH, Brooks DJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RS, Passingham RE. Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J Neurosci. 1994;14:3775–90.CrossRefPubMed Jenkins IH, Brooks DJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RS, Passingham RE. Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J Neurosci. 1994;14:3775–90.CrossRefPubMed
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Ullman MT. Is Broca’s area part of a basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit? Cortex. 2006;42:480–5.CrossRefPubMed Ullman MT. Is Broca’s area part of a basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit? Cortex. 2006;42:480–5.CrossRefPubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Ackermann H, Hage SR, Ziegler W. Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: an evolutionary perspective. Behav Brain Sci. 2014;37:529–46.CrossRefPubMed Ackermann H, Hage SR, Ziegler W. Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: an evolutionary perspective. Behav Brain Sci. 2014;37:529–46.CrossRefPubMed
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Schoch B, Dimitrova A, Gizewski ER, Timmann D. Functional localization in the human cerebellum based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 90 patients. NeuroImage. 2006;30:36–51.CrossRefPubMed Schoch B, Dimitrova A, Gizewski ER, Timmann D. Functional localization in the human cerebellum based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 90 patients. NeuroImage. 2006;30:36–51.CrossRefPubMed
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Tourville JA, Reilly KJ, Guenther FH. Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1429–43.CrossRefPubMed Tourville JA, Reilly KJ, Guenther FH. Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1429–43.CrossRefPubMed
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Golfinopoulos E, Tourville JA, Bohland JW, Ghosh SS, Nieto-Castanon A, Guenther FH. fMRI investigation of unexpected somatosensory feedback perturbation during speech. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1324–38.CrossRefPubMed Golfinopoulos E, Tourville JA, Bohland JW, Ghosh SS, Nieto-Castanon A, Guenther FH. fMRI investigation of unexpected somatosensory feedback perturbation during speech. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1324–38.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Neocerebellar Crus I Abnormalities Associated with a Speech and Language Disorder Due to a Mutation in FOXP2
verfasst von
G. P. D. Argyropoulos
K. E. Watkins
E. Belton-Pagnamenta
F. Liégeois
K. S. Saleem
M. Mishkin
F. Vargha-Khadem
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2019
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
The Cerebellum / Ausgabe 3/2019
Print ISSN: 1473-4222
Elektronische ISSN: 1473-4230
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-018-0989-3

Kompaktes Leitlinien-Wissen Neurologie (Link öffnet in neuem Fenster)

Mit medbee Pocketcards schnell und sicher entscheiden.
Leitlinien-Wissen kostenlos und immer griffbereit auf ihrem Desktop, Handy oder Tablet.

Neu im Fachgebiet Neurologie

Stumme Schlaganfälle − ein häufiger Nebenbefund im Kopf-CT?

In 4% der in der Notfallambulanz initiierten zerebralen Bildgebung sind „alte“ Schlaganfälle zu erkennen. Gar nicht so selten handelt es sich laut einer aktuellen Studie dabei um unbemerkte Insulte. Bietet sich hier womöglich die Chance auf ein effektives opportunistisches Screening?

CGRP-Antikörper auch bei älteren Migränekranken sicher

Beginnen ältere Migränekranke eine Prophylaxe mit CGRP-Antikörpern, kommt es anschließend nicht häufiger zu kardiovaskulären Problemen als unter einer Prophylaxe mit Botulinumtoxin. Darauf deutet eine US-Analyse von Medicare-Versicherten.

Frühwarnzeichen für multiple Sklerose bei Kindern und Jugendlichen

Ein Forschungsteam aus Deutschland und Kanada hat eine Reihe metabolischer, okulärer, muskuloskelettaler, gastrointestinaler und kardiovaskulärer Symptome identifiziert, die bei Kindern und Jugendlichen der Diagnose einer multiplen Sklerose (MS) vorausgehen können.

Migräne verstehen und psychotherapeutisch behandeln

Das Wissen über die Mechanismen, die im Gehirn bei einer Migräneattacke ablaufen, und mögliche Auslöser wird immer breiter. Der psychologische Psychotherapeut Dr. Dipl.-Psych. Timo Klan fasst den aktuellen Erkenntnisstand zusammen. Und er gibt Tipps für eine differenzierte, individuelle Diagnostik auch von Begleiterkrankungen und beschreibt erfolgreiche psychotherapeutische Interventionen.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.