Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Annals of Behavioral Medicine 1/2017

Open Access 22.09.2016 | Original Article

Personality and Risk of Frailty: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

verfasst von: Catharine R Gale, PhD, René Mõttus, PhD, Ian J Deary, PhD, Cyrus Cooper, DM, Avan Aihie Sayer, PhD

Erschienen in: Annals of Behavioral Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

There is evidence that the personality traits conscientiousness, extraversion and neuroticism are associated with health behaviours and with risk of various health outcomes. We hypothesised that people who are lower in conscientiousness or extraversion or higher in neuroticism may be at greater risk of frailty in later life.

Methods

We used general linear models to examine the prospective relation between personality, assessed using the Midlife Development Inventory, and change in frailty, modelled by a frailty index, in 5314 men and women aged 60 to over 90 years from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing.

Results

Men and women with higher levels of neuroticism or lower levels of extraversion or conscientiousness had an increased frailty index score at follow-up. After adjustment for potential confounding or mediating variables, including frailty index score at baseline, the frailty index score at follow-up—which potentially ranges from 0 to 1—was higher by 0.035 (95 % confidence interval 0.018, 0.052) for a standard deviation increase in neuroticism and lower by 0.061 (0.031, 0.091) or 0.045 (0.020, 0.071) for a standard deviation increase in extraversion or conscientiousness, respectively. There was some evidence that the association between extraversion and frailty may be due to reverse causation whereby poorer health affected responses to items in the personality inventory.

Conclusions

Higher levels of neuroticism or lower levels of conscientiousness or extraversion may be risk factors for the onset or progression of frailty. Future studies need to replicate these observations in other populations and explore the mechanisms underlying these associations.

Introduction

Frailty is a clinical syndrome observed in older people whose core feature is an increased vulnerability to stressors due to impairments in multiple, inter-related systems, decreased physiological reserves and a decline in the ability to maintain homeostasis [1]. It is increasingly common at older ages [2] and raises the risk of numerous adverse consequences, including disability, falls, morbidity, hospitalisation, institutionalisation and death. Its causes are complex and are likely to involve not only just biomedical but also social and psychological mechanisms [3].
There are two established models for frailty [1]. The frailty phenotype model—devised by Fried and colleagues using data from the Cardiovascular Health Study [4]—defines frailty as the presence of three or more components: unintentional weight loss, self-reported exhaustion, low-energy expenditure, slow walking speed and weak grip strength. The frailty index, or cumulative deficit model—originally developed by Rockwood and colleagues using data from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging [5, 6]—defines frailty in terms of the accumulation of ‘deficits’ (symptoms, signs, diseases and disabilities), whereby an individual’s frailty index score reflects the proportion of potential deficits present in that individual and indicates the likelihood that frailty is present [7]. While both models of frailty have predictive validity for adverse outcomes, there is evidence that the continuously distributed frailty index may be better at discriminating between moderately and severely frail individuals [8].
One psychological factor that may influence the risk of becoming frail in later life is personality—a largely stable set of traits and characteristics that influence behaviour, thoughts and feelings. Personality starts developing during early childhood and demonstrates increasing continuity with increasing age [9]. It has the potential to influence health via several mechanisms [10], including engagement in health-damaging or health-enhancing behaviours, illness behaviour in response to symptom perception or diagnosis of illness, or susceptibility to stress-induced physiological arousal, which may contribute to the development or progression of illness. There is evidence that levels of personality traits may change in adulthood in some people, if not in most, possibly in response to biological or environmental factors [1113]. Such changes have been linked with later adverse health outcomes, including mortality [14]. This raises the possibility that personality traits themselves could, in the future, be targets for intervention.
The five-factor model of personality [15], consisting of the major personality traits of neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness, has been widely used to study the relationship between personality and health outcomes and behaviours [16]. To our knowledge, no previous study has investigated the relationship between these personality traits and frailty, but there is evidence that certain personality traits are associated with health behaviours or conditions that have been linked with risk of frailty, including smoking [17], physical activity [18], cardiovascular disease [19, 20], diabetes [21, 22] obesity [23, 24] and poorer cognitive function [25, 26]. Of the five major traits, conscientiousness—the tendency to be organised, responsible, industrious and disciplined—is the personality trait that has been most consistently linked with healthier behaviour [27] and with longevity [2833]. People who are higher in conscientiousness also have a reduced risk of dying from cardiovascular disease [3436]; are less likely to develop or die of diabetes [37]; and have a lower risk of obesity [38], cognitive decline [39] and dementia [40]. Neuroticism—the tendency to experience negative emotions—has been linked with smoking and low physical activity [41]. People who are higher in neuroticism have an increased risk of cognitive decline [39] and dementia [40], but findings on their risk of cardiovascular disease have been mixed, with some studies finding an increased risk [34, 42] and others finding no association [28, 43]. Extraversion—the tendency to be sociable, outgoing and energetic—has consistently been associated with being less physically active [41]. These findings on personality led us to hypothesise that older people who are lower in conscientiousness or extraversion or higher in neuroticism may be at greater risk of frailty. We had no a priori hypotheses about the relationship between the other major personality traits, agreeableness—the tendency to be kind, warm, tolerant and affable—and openness to experience—the tendency to be curious, creative, open to new ideas and intellectual—and risk of frailty.
The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is a large population-based study of older men and women. We used these data to investigate the prospective relationship between the five major personality traits and frailty in people aged 60 to over 90 years.

Methods

Participants

The data for this study come from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). The initial sample for ELSA was based on people aged ≥50 years who had participated in the Health Survey for England in 1998, 1999 or 2001 [44]. It was drawn by postcode sector and stratified by health authority and proportion of households in non-manual socioeconomic groups. The initial survey took place in 2002–2003. Subsequent waves of data collection have taken place at 2-year intervals. Refreshment samples drawn from the Health Survey for England were added at waves 3 and 4 to maintain the representation of people aged 50–75. The current study uses data from waves 5 (2010–2011) and 6 (2012–2013). Ethical approval was obtained from the Multicentre Research and Ethics Committee. Participants gave written informed consent.

Measures

Frailty

We used a frailty index to assess frailty status at baseline (wave 5) and follow-up (wave 6). A frailty index can be derived from different numbers or types of variables, thereby facilitating comparison between datasets [7]. The criteria for inclusion are that the variables are associated with health status, represent conditions that become more common with age—though not ubiquitous (e.g. presbyopia)—and cover a range of systems [45]. If a frailty index is to be used at two or more time points on the same individuals, the items used to derive the index at each point in time need to be the same [45]. In ELSA, our frailty index was made up of 44 deficits, including sensory and functional impairments, a score on a composite measure of cognitive function that was in the lowest 10 % of the distribution, and self-reported comorbidities (see Supplementary Table 1 for details of the deficits included). The frailty index is constructed by summing the number of deficits present for each individual and dividing by the total number of deficits considered, which gives a range from 0 to 1. Higher values indicate greater frailty.
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of the study sample and their rank order correlations with frailty index scores at follow-up (n = 5314)
Baseline characteristics
Mean (SD), median (IQR) or number (%)
Correlation with frailty index at follow-up
Age (years), mean (SD)
70.0 (7.36)
0.3012***
Female, no. (%)
2982 (54.4)
0.128***
Personality traits, mean (SD)
 Extraversion
3.15 (0.55)
−0.249***
 Agreeableness
3.51 (0.48)
0.015
 Openness
2.86 (0.56)
−0.165***
 Neuroticism
2.06 (0.58)
0.161***
 Conscientiousness
3.27 (0.50)
−0.224***
Household wealth (£), median (IQR)
241,102 (131,215–418,219)
−0.292***
Smoking status, no. (%)
 
0.103***
 Never
1956 (36.8)
 
 Ex-smoker
2822 (53.1)
 
 Current smoker
536 (10.1)
 
Physical activity, no. (%)
 
−0.415***
 Sedentary
279 (5.25)
 
 Low
1294 (24.4)
 
 Medium
2749 (51.7)
 
 High
992 (18.7)
 
 Frailty index score, median (IQR)
0.068 (0.022–0.136)
0.830***
***p < 0.001, Spearman correlation significance level

Personality

Levels of the five major personality traits—extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness to experience—were assessed at wave 5 using a version of the Midlife Development Inventory previously used in the US Health and Retirement Survey [46]. These dimensions were measured using self-ratings of 26 adjectives. Respondents were asked the degree to which each adjective described them, rating each one on a four-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 to 4). The adjectives making up each dimension were as follows: extraversion: outgoing, friendly, active, talkative and lively; agreeableness: warm, helpful, soft-hearted, sympathetic and caring; conscientiousness: organised, responsible, thorough, hardworking and careless; neuroticism: moody, worrying, nervous and calm; and openness to experience: creative, imaginative, intelligent, curious, sophisticated and adventurous. Each score was calculated by obtaining the average of the ratings defining that dimension. Cronbach alpha values in these data were 0.76 (extraversion), 0.80 (agreeableness), 0.68 (neuroticism), 0.67 (conscientiousness) and 0.79 (openness to experience), indicating at least adequate internal consistency.

Covariates

We chose age, socioeconomic position, smoking and physical activity, all measured at baseline, as covariates. Socioeconomic position was indexed by total household wealth, including savings and investments, value of any property or business assets and net of debt, excluding pension assets. Household wealth has been identified as the most accurate indicator of long-term socioeconomic circumstances in ELSA [47]. Participants provided information on whether they were current smokers, were ex-smokers or had never smoked. Participants were asked about the level of physical activity involved in their job (if they were working) and responded to three questions on mild, moderate or vigorous physical activity carried out in daily life. The answers to these questions were used to derive a categorical summary variable on physical activity (sedentary, low, moderate or high) that approximates as closely as possible to the classification used in the Allied Dunbar Survey of Fitness [48].

Analytical Sample

In total, 7122 cohort members aged 60 and over took part in the baseline survey at wave 5. The current analysis is based on 5314 (75 %) of them who had complete data on personality, frailty index score and all the covariates at baseline and frailty index score at the wave 6 follow-up.

Statistical Analysis

We used rank order correlations to examine baseline characteristics in relation to the frailty index scores at follow-up. Frailty index scores have a gamma distribution [45], so we used general linear models assuming a gamma distribution to examine the relation between a standard deviation increase in each personality trait at baseline and frailty index score at follow up. Preliminary analyses showed that associations between personality traits and frailty index score did not differ by sex, so we analysed men and women together. We adjusted for age, sex and baseline frailty index score, then in addition for the other personality traits and next for household wealth, smoking status and physical activity Analyses were carried out using STATA version 13 (StataCorp 2013, College Station, TX). We used the STATA command ‘mfpigen’ to investigate whether the effect of personality traits on frailty index scores varied significantly according to the covariates. Mfpigen investigates interactions between pairs of variables, while simultaneously applying multivariable functional polynomials to the remaining variables to select a ‘confounder model’, which is used to adjust the interaction model for possible confounding by other covariates [49, 50]. In view of the large sample size and the likelihood that even small effects would be statistically significant, we used p < 0.01 to indicate statistical significance.
All data were weighted to correct for sampling probabilities, non-response and differential sample loss between waves in order to make them more closely reflect the population from whom the ELSA sample was drawn. Detailed descriptions of these weights and their calculation can be found in the technical reports on the study available at www.​ifs.​org.​uk/​elsa.

Results

Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the 5314 men and women in the study and the rank order correlations between those characteristics and the frailty index scores at follow-up. Greater frailty at follow-up, as indicated by a higher frailty index score, was associated with older age, being female, lower household wealth, greater exposure to smoking, lower physical activity, lower extraversion, lower conscientiousness, lower openness, higher neuroticism and a higher frailty index at baseline. There was no association between baseline levels of agreeableness and frailty index score at follow-up. People who were excluded from our analytical sample due to loss to follow-up or missing data were, on average, older (mean 73.8 vs 70.0 years), frailer (median frailty index score 0.114 vs 0.068), poorer (median household wealth £188,900 vs £241,142), less physically active (22.6 % sedentary vs 5.25) and more likely to be a current smoker (14.2 vs 10.1 %). Those excluded had slightly lower scores for conscientiousness (mean 3.18 vs 3.27), extraversion (mean 3.07 vs 3.14) and openness (2.79 vs 2.86). There were no differences between our analytical sample and those excluded from it as regard sex distribution or scores for neuroticism and agreeableness.
The personality traits agreeableness, openness, extraversion and conscientiousness were all moderately positively correlated with each other (rho 0.41 to 0.59); correlations between these four traits and neuroticism were weaker and inverse (rho −0.04 to −0.20).
Table 2 shows mean (SD) personality trait scores and median (IQR) frailty index scores by age group in men and women separately. In both sexes, mean scores for the personality traits were lower with increasing age; the only exception was agreeableness in men where mean scores were slightly higher in older age groups. Mean differences in personality between the youngest and oldest age groups were small in size in both sexes: Cohen’s d = 0.2–0.3. In both sexes, median frailty index scores at baseline and at follow-up were higher and more dispersed with increasing age.
Table 2
Personality trait scores and frailty index scores according to age group in men and women
 
Men
Women
Age group
Age group
60–69 (n = 1255)
70–79 (n = 827)
≥80 (n = 266)
60–69 (n = 1558)
70–79 (n = 1025)
≥80 (n = 383)
Personality traits, mean (SD)
 Extraversion
3.12 (0.56)
3.11 (0.56)
2.95 (0.57)
3.25 (0.51)
3.16 (0.56)
3.02 (0.60)
 Agreeableness
3.39 (0.51)
3.40 (0.50)
3.40 (0.51)
3.62 (0.41)
3.59 (0.45)
3.51 (0.51)
 Openness
2.96 (0.52)
2.87 (0.54)
2.76 (0.54)
2.89 (0.55)
2.80 (0.57)
2.69 (0.60)
 Neuroticism
2.06 (0.59)
1.95 (0.57)
1.94 (0.53)
2.16 (0.58)
2.06 (0.57)
1.97 (0.53)
 Conscientiousness
3.28 (0.50)
3.19 (0.50)
3.12 (0.50)
3.38 (0.45)
3.25 (0.49)
3.13 (0.55)
Frailty index score at baseline, median (IQR)
0.045 (0.023–0.091)
0.068 (0.023–0.136)
0.113 (0.068–0.227)
0.068 (0.023–0.114)
0.091 (0.045–0.182)
0.159 (0.091–0.273)
Frailty index score at follow-up, median (IQR)
0.045 (0.023–0.113)
0.068 (0.023–0.136)
0.136 (0.068–0.250)
0.068 (0.023–0.136)
0.114 (0.045–0.205
0.182 (0.091–0.295)
In both men and women, all personality traits, with the exception of agreeableness in men, and both frailty index scores differed significantly by age group (p < 0.001)
Table 3 shows the results of generalised linear models estimating the relation of personality traits at baseline (per standard deviation increase in score) with change in frailty index scores by follow-up. In model 1, where we examined each personality trait separately and adjusted for age, sex and baseline frailty index score, people who were more extravert, more conscientious and lower in neuroticism at baseline had a lower frailty index score at follow-up. There were no associations between baseline agreeableness or openness and frailty index score at follow-up. In model 2, where we further adjusted for all personality traits simultaneously, greater extraversion, greater conscientiousness and lower neuroticism continued to be significantly associated with lower frailty index scores at follow-up; adjusting for other personality traits strengthened the size of the effects between extraversion and conscientiousness and frailty index score at follow-up. In this model, unexpectedly, a significant association emerged between greater agreeableness at baseline and higher frailty index score at follow-up. In the final model, we further adjusted for household wealth, physical activity and smoking status at baseline. This additional adjustment resulted in only slight changes to the estimates. Greater extraversion, greater conscientiousness and lower neuroticism continued to be significantly associated with lower frailty index scores at follow-up. Greater agreeableness continued to be associated with a higher frailty index at follow-up in this multivariate adjusted model. We examined whether associations between personality traits and frailty index scores at follow-up varied according to the covariates. There was only one significant interaction, between conscientiousness and smoking status (p < 0.001): the association between conscientiousness and frailty index score was present and similar in non-smokers and current smokers but was absent in ex-smokers.
Table 3
Coefficients (95 % CI) for the effects of a standard deviation increase in personality trait scores at baseline on change in frailty index scores by follow-up
Personality trait scores, per SD
Coefficient (95 % CI)
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Agreeableness
0.011 (−0.010, 0.032)
0.072 (0.045, 0.100)***
0.04658 (0.031, 0.085)***
Openness
−0.023 (−0.044, −0.002)
0.013 (−0.014, 0.041)
0.019 (−0.008, 0.046)
Extraversion
−0.049 (−0.071, −0.027)***
−0.072 (−0.10, 2–0.033)***
−0.061 (−0.091, −0.031)***
Neuroticism
0.045 (0.023, 0.066)***
0.032 (0.015, 0.041)**
0.035 (0.018, 0.052)***
Conscientiousness
−0.052 (−0.074, −0.03)***
−0.072 (−0.102, −0.041)***
−0.045 (−0.071, −0.020)***
Model 1 adjusts for age, sex and frailty index score at baseline. Model 2 further adjusts for all personality trait scores at baseline. Model 3 further adjusts for household wealth, physical activity and smoking status at baseline
***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01: general linear model significance levels
In the analyses described above, our adjustment for baseline frailty index score reduces the possibility of reverse causation whereby poorer health at the time that the personality inventory was completed might have influenced how participants responded to some of the items in the inventory. Examination of the rank order correlations between the individual items making up the personality inventory and frailty index score at baseline showed that the extraversion items ‘active’ and ‘lively’ and the conscientiousness item ‘hardworking’ were much more strongly correlated with the contemporaneous frailty index score than the other items, with figures for rho of −0.46, −0.29 and −0.26, respectively (see Supplementary Table 2). As a further check on whether reverse causation might explain our findings, we carried out a sensitivity analysis. Firstly, we repeated our analyses looking at the relations between personality trait scores and later frailty index score in a subset of 3089 participants whose score on the baseline frailty index—potential range 0 to 1—was <0.08, in other words, those who were in generally good health with no or very few problems. In this subset of largely healthy individuals, the fully adjusted (model 3) associations between baseline personality traits and frailty index score at follow-up remained significant and were very similar in size to those observed in the sample as a whole. Secondly, we repeated our analyses of the relations between extraversion and conscientiousness in the whole sample using a modified score for each trait, which had been calculated without using the extraversion items active and lively or the conscientiousness item hardworking (the Cronbach alpha for these two modified scores showed acceptable internal consistency at 0.68 and 0.62, respectively). In a fully adjusted model using these modified scores, greater conscientiousness continued to be a significant predictor of a lower frailty index score at follow-up, but the association between extraversion and frailty index score at follow-up was no longer significant (p = 0.51).

Discussion

In this prospective study of people aged 60 to over 90 years, higher levels of neuroticism and lower levels of extraversion and conscientiousness were associated with greater frailty at follow-up around 2 years later. These associations persisted after adjustment for several potential confounding or mediating variables. Results of sensitivity analyses suggested that the association between extraversion and frailty may be due to reverse causation whereby poorer health at baseline may have influenced responses to specific items used to assess extraversion. Unexpectedly, higher levels of agreeableness were also associated with greater frailty at follow-up, though these associations only emerged when we controlled for the variance agreeableness shared with other personality traits. We examined whether the associations between personality traits and frailty index score varied according to levels of the covariates, but there were no statistically significant interactions with the exception of one between conscientiousness and smoking status that was hard to interpret. In view of the large number of potential interactions examined, it is possible that this interaction was statistically significant by chance.
The finding that older people who were higher in neuroticism or lower in extraversion or conscientiousness scored higher on a frailty index at follow-up after adjustment for frailty level at baseline and other potential covariates provides support for our hypothesis that these traits may be risk factors for onset or worsening of frailty. To our knowledge, there have been no previous studies into the relation between personality and risk of frailty, either modelled using a frailty index, as here, or as the Fried phenotype. But, there is some evidence to link personality with key components of the Fried frailty phenotype. For example, evidence from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Ageing suggests that muscle strength is poorer in those who are higher in neuroticism or lower in extraversion [51]. Similarly, in a prospective study of older people in Chicago, higher neuroticism and lower extraversion were associated with more rapid decline in motor function [52], while in the Health, Aging, and Body Composition Study, lower conscientiousness was associated with slower walking speed and greater decline in walking speed over a 3-year follow-up period [53]. The mechanisms underlying these associations between personality and components of the frailty phenotype are not fully understood. While lifestyle factors and disease status partially explained the cross-sectional associations between neuroticism, extraversion or conscientiousness and muscle strength [51] or walking speed [53] and the longitudinal association between neuroticism and extraversion and decline in motor function [52], they appeared to play no part in the longitudinal relation between conscientiousness and decline in walking speed [53]. This is consistent with other studies that have found that the protective effect of higher conscientiousness on mortality from all causes or cardiovascular disease was only partially explained by health behaviours, obesity or other common risk factors such as diabetes and hypertension [28, 34, 35]. In the current study too, the associations that we found between conscientiousness, neuroticism and extraversion and risk of frailty as measured by a frailty index were only partially attenuated by adjustment for potential mediating factors—smoking and physical activity—and potential confounders. Further studies are needed using other personality inventories to try to replicate our findings and to explore whether it is specific lower-order facets of these personality traits that influence frailty risk.
It is possible that physiological mechanisms underlie links between personality and later frailty. There is evidence that personality is associated with individual differences in physiological processes that have been hypothesised to underlie the onset of frailty [54], namely inflammation [55, 56] and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA) [57]. Some longitudinal studies have found that risk of frailty is increased in older people with higher blood concentrations of inflammatory markers, but associations are not consistent [21, 5860]. Recent findings suggest that frailty is accompanied by blunted cortisol reactivity, but the direction of effect in this cross-sectional survey is unclear [61]. Further longitudinal studies are needed to explore the extent to which inflammation or HPA dysregulation explains links between neuroticism, extraversion or conscientiousness and risk of frailty.
The observation in the current study that being higher in agreeableness became a risk factor for greater frailty at follow-up once we controlled for levels of other personality traits was unexpected. We are not aware of any previous evidence that being more agreeable might increase the risk of adverse health outcomes. We had no prior hypothesis as to the relation between agreeableness and risk of frailty, but in view of findings from a meta-analysis that greater disagreeableness and, in particular, greater hostility—one of the facets of this trait—are linked with increased mortality [62], we were surprised by the direction and strength of the association that we found once the variance agreeableness shared with the other four personality traits had been removed. For it to be interpretable as a genuine effect, there would need to be a substantive and plausible definition of the variance that has been partialled out. It seems likely that this apparently suppressed effect of agreeableness is a statistical artefact.
The strengths of our study include the large sample size and the fact that it is representative of the community-dwelling English population aged 60 and over. Furthermore, it is the first investigation of the prospective relationship between personality and risk of frailty. The study also has some weaknesses. Firstly, scores for four of the five personality traits examined were moderately highly correlated (rho 0.41 to 0.59), which is not ideal in a personality inventory. We dealt with this by adjusting each trait for the other four traits in our general linear models. Secondly, data on BMI was not available at baseline, so we were unable to examine its potential mediating role in the associations between personality traits and later frailty. Thirdly, those who were excluded from our analytical sample due to loss to follow-up or missing data were older and frailer and had slightly lower scores for some personality traits than those who were included. However, all data were weighted to correct for non-response and for differential sample loss between waves. Finally, personality was measured when participants were aged 60 to over 90 years. Once people reach around age 30 years, personality traits tend to show considerable stability over time [63], but there is some evidence for change in personality in old age, perhaps in response to critical life events or major chronic illness [64]. It is possible that the presence of illness and disability at the time of personality assessment may have influenced how our participants responded when asked how much each adjective in the personality inventory applied to them. This may apply particularly to the adjectives active, lively and hardworking, part of the extraversion and conscientiousness scales, respectively. Participants’ self-ratings of these adjectives were much more strongly correlated with the contemporaneous frailty index score than were their ratings of other adjectives. In our analyses, we adjusted for frailty index score at the time of personality assessment, thereby taking account of all comorbidities, functional and sensory impairments and symptoms recorded at that time. Furthermore, a sensitivity analysis in the subset of participants whose frailty index score at baseline suggested that they had few or no problems produced estimates for the associations between personality traits and frailty at follow-up that were very similar to those obtained in the sample as a whole. However, when re-ran our analyses using modified extraversion and conscientiousness scores which had been calculated without using the extraversion items active, lively or the conscientiousness item hardworking, the association between extraversion and frailty at follow-up was no longer significant, although that between conscientiousness and later frailty persisted. This suggests that reverse causality may explain our observation linking lower extraversion to greater frailty at follow-up, though another interpretation could be that active and lively reflect the aspects of extraversion that are protective against frailty [65]. Our findings on the potential importance of certain personality traits as risk factors for the onset or progression of frailty need replicating, ideally in a cohort with a measure of personality taken much earlier in adult life.
In this prospective study of men and women aged 60 to over 90 years, higher levels of neuroticism and lower levels of conscientiousness and extraversion were associated with greater frailty around 2 years later. These associations were only slightly attenuated by adjustment for potential confounding or mediating factors, but there was some evidence that reverse causation may explain the link between extraversion and frailty risk. Future studies need to replicate our observations in other populations and explore the mechanisms whereby these personality traits might increase the risk of frailty.

Acknowledgments

The data were made available through the UK Data Archive. ELSA was developed by a team of researchers based at the National Centre for Social Research, University College London and the Institute for Fiscal Studies. The data were collected by the National Centre for Social Research. The funding is provided by the National Institute of Aging in the USA and a consortium of UK government departments coordinated by the Office for National Statistics. The developers and funders of ELSA and the Archive do not bear any responsibility for the analyses or interpretations presented here.
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC_MC_UU_12011/2 and MRC_MC_UP_A620_1015). CRG, RM and IJD are members of the Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1); funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) and Medical Research Council (MRC) is gratefully acknowledged.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards

Authors Catharine R Gale, Rene Mottus, Ian J Deary, Cyrus Cooper and Avan Aihie Sayer declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval for the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing was obtained from the Multicentre Research and Ethics Committee. Participants gave written informed consent. Procedures complied with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 as revised in 2000.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K: Frailty in elderly people. Lancet. 2013; 381:752–762.CrossRefPubMed Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K: Frailty in elderly people. Lancet. 2013; 381:752–762.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Gale CR, Cooper C, Aihie Sayer A: Prevalence of frailty and disability: Findings from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age Ageing. 2015; 44:162–165.CrossRefPubMed Gale CR, Cooper C, Aihie Sayer A: Prevalence of frailty and disability: Findings from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age Ageing. 2015; 44:162–165.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Walston J, Hadley EC, Ferrucci L, et al.: Research agenda for frailty in older adults: Toward a better understanding of physiology and etiology: Summary from the American Geriatrics Society/National Institute on Aging Research Conference on Frailty in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006; 54:991–1001.CrossRefPubMed Walston J, Hadley EC, Ferrucci L, et al.: Research agenda for frailty in older adults: Toward a better understanding of physiology and etiology: Summary from the American Geriatrics Society/National Institute on Aging Research Conference on Frailty in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006; 54:991–1001.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, et al.: Frailty in older adults: Evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001; 56:M146-M156.CrossRefPubMed Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, et al.: Frailty in older adults: Evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001; 56:M146-M156.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitnitski AB, Mogilner AJ, Rockwood K: Accumulation of deficits as a proxy measure of aging. Scientific World Journal. 2001; 1:323–336.CrossRefPubMed Mitnitski AB, Mogilner AJ, Rockwood K: Accumulation of deficits as a proxy measure of aging. Scientific World Journal. 2001; 1:323–336.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Rockwood K, Mitnitski A: Frailty in relation to the accumulation of deficits. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007; 62:722–727.CrossRefPubMed Rockwood K, Mitnitski A: Frailty in relation to the accumulation of deficits. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007; 62:722–727.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kulminski AM, Ukraintseva SV, Kulminskaya IV, et al.: Cumulative deficits better characterize susceptibility to death in elderly people than phenotypic frailty: Lessons from the Cardiovascular Health Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008; 56:898–903.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kulminski AM, Ukraintseva SV, Kulminskaya IV, et al.: Cumulative deficits better characterize susceptibility to death in elderly people than phenotypic frailty: Lessons from the Cardiovascular Health Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008; 56:898–903.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL: Personality development: Stability and change. Ann Rev Psychol. 2005; 56:453–484.CrossRef Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL: Personality development: Stability and change. Ann Rev Psychol. 2005; 56:453–484.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiebe DJ, Fortenberry KT: Mechanisms relating personality and health. In Vollrath ME (ed) Handbook of Personality and Health. Chichester: Wiley Ltd, 2006, 137–156.CrossRef Wiebe DJ, Fortenberry KT: Mechanisms relating personality and health. In Vollrath ME (ed) Handbook of Personality and Health. Chichester: Wiley Ltd, 2006, 137–156.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC: Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011; 101:862–882.CrossRefPubMed Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC: Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011; 101:862–882.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Leszko M, Elleman LG, Bastarache ED, Graham EK, Mroczek DK: Future directions in the study of personality in adulthood and older age. Gerontology. 2016; 62:210–215.CrossRefPubMed Leszko M, Elleman LG, Bastarache ED, Graham EK, Mroczek DK: Future directions in the study of personality in adulthood and older age. Gerontology. 2016; 62:210–215.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa PT, McCrae RR: NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992. Costa PT, McCrae RR: NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Vollrath ME (ed): Handbook of Personality and Health. Chichester: Wiley, 2006. Vollrath ME (ed): Handbook of Personality and Health. Chichester: Wiley, 2006.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard RE, Searle SD, Mitnitski A, Rockwood K: Effect of smoking on the accumulation of deficits, frailty and survival in older adults: A secondary analysis from the Canadian study of health and aging. J Nutr Health Aging. 2009; 13:468–472.CrossRefPubMed Hubbard RE, Searle SD, Mitnitski A, Rockwood K: Effect of smoking on the accumulation of deficits, frailty and survival in older adults: A secondary analysis from the Canadian study of health and aging. J Nutr Health Aging. 2009; 13:468–472.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Savela SL, Koistinen P, Stenholm S, et al.: Leisure-time physical activity in midlife is related to old age frailty. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013; 68:1433–1438.CrossRefPubMed Savela SL, Koistinen P, Stenholm S, et al.: Leisure-time physical activity in midlife is related to old age frailty. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013; 68:1433–1438.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Afilalo J, Karunananthan S, Eisenberg MJ, Alexander KP, Bergman H: Role of frailty in patients with cardiovascular disease. Am J Cardiol. 2009; 103:1616–1621.CrossRefPubMed Afilalo J, Karunananthan S, Eisenberg MJ, Alexander KP, Bergman H: Role of frailty in patients with cardiovascular disease. Am J Cardiol. 2009; 103:1616–1621.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee JS, Auyeung TW, Leung J, Kwok T, Woo J: Transitions in frailty states among community-living older adults and their associated factors. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2014; 15:281–286.CrossRefPubMed Lee JS, Auyeung TW, Leung J, Kwok T, Woo J: Transitions in frailty states among community-living older adults and their associated factors. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2014; 15:281–286.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Gale CR, Baylis D, Cooper C, Sayer AA: Inflammatory markers and incident frailty in men and women: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age. 2013; 35:2493–2501.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gale CR, Baylis D, Cooper C, Sayer AA: Inflammatory markers and incident frailty in men and women: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age. 2013; 35:2493–2501.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard RE, Lang IA, Llewellyn DJ, Rockwood K: Frailty, body mass index, and abdominal obesity in older people. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010; 65:377–381.CrossRefPubMed Hubbard RE, Lang IA, Llewellyn DJ, Rockwood K: Frailty, body mass index, and abdominal obesity in older people. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010; 65:377–381.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcia-Esquinas E, Jose Garcia-Garcia F, Leon-Munoz LM, et al.: Obesity, fat distribution, and risk of frailty in two population-based cohorts of older adults in Spain. Obesity. 2015; 23:847–855.CrossRefPubMed Garcia-Esquinas E, Jose Garcia-Garcia F, Leon-Munoz LM, et al.: Obesity, fat distribution, and risk of frailty in two population-based cohorts of older adults in Spain. Obesity. 2015; 23:847–855.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Gale CR, Cooper C, Deary IJ, Aihie Sayer A: Psychological well-being and incident frailty in men and women: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychol Med. 2014; 44:697–706.CrossRefPubMed Gale CR, Cooper C, Deary IJ, Aihie Sayer A: Psychological well-being and incident frailty in men and women: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychol Med. 2014; 44:697–706.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Raji MA, Al Snih S, Ostir GV, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ: Cognitive status and future risk of frailty in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010; 65:1228–1234.CrossRefPubMed Raji MA, Al Snih S, Ostir GV, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ: Cognitive status and future risk of frailty in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010; 65:1228–1234.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Bogg T, Roberts BW: Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004; 130:887–919.CrossRefPubMed Bogg T, Roberts BW: Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004; 130:887–919.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Jokela M, Batty GD, Nyberg ST, et al.: Personality and all-cause mortality: Individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2013; 178:667–675.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Jokela M, Batty GD, Nyberg ST, et al.: Personality and all-cause mortality: Individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2013; 178:667–675.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Bogg T, Roberts BW: The case for conscientiousness: Evidence and implications for a personality trait marker of health and longevity. Ann Behav Med. 2013; 45:278–288.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bogg T, Roberts BW: The case for conscientiousness: Evidence and implications for a personality trait marker of health and longevity. Ann Behav Med. 2013; 45:278–288.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin LR, Friedman HS, Schwartz JE: Personality and mortality risk across the life span: The importance of conscientiousness as a biopsychosocial attribute. Health Psychol. 2007; 26:428–436.CrossRefPubMed Martin LR, Friedman HS, Schwartz JE: Personality and mortality risk across the life span: The importance of conscientiousness as a biopsychosocial attribute. Health Psychol. 2007; 26:428–436.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Kern ML, Friedman HS: Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychol. 2008; 27:505–512.CrossRefPubMed Kern ML, Friedman HS: Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychol. 2008; 27:505–512.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Deary IJ, Batty GD, Pattie A, Gale CR: More intelligent, more dependable children live longer a 55-year longitudinal study of a representative sample of the Scottish nation. Psychol Sci. 2008; 19:874–880.CrossRefPubMed Deary IJ, Batty GD, Pattie A, Gale CR: More intelligent, more dependable children live longer a 55-year longitudinal study of a representative sample of the Scottish nation. Psychol Sci. 2008; 19:874–880.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Gruenewald TL, Mroczek DK: Personality and the leading behavioral contributors of mortality. Health Psychol. 2015; 34:51–60.CrossRefPubMed Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Gruenewald TL, Mroczek DK: Personality and the leading behavioral contributors of mortality. Health Psychol. 2015; 34:51–60.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Jokela M, Pulkki-Raback L, Elovainio M, Kivimaki M: Personality traits as risk factors for stroke and coronary heart disease mortality: Pooled analysis of three cohort studies. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2014; 37:881–889.CrossRefPubMed Jokela M, Pulkki-Raback L, Elovainio M, Kivimaki M: Personality traits as risk factors for stroke and coronary heart disease mortality: Pooled analysis of three cohort studies. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2014; 37:881–889.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Terracciano A, Lockenhoff CE, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Costa PTJ: Personality predictors of longevity: Activity, emotional stability and conscientiousness. Psychosom. Med. 2008; 70:621–627.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Terracciano A, Lockenhoff CE, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Costa PTJ: Personality predictors of longevity: Activity, emotional stability and conscientiousness. Psychosom. Med. 2008; 70:621–627.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Hagger-Johnson G, Sabia S, Nabi H, et al.: Low conscientiousness and risk of all-cause, cardiovascular and cancer mortality over 17 years: Whitehall II cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2012; 73:98–103.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hagger-Johnson G, Sabia S, Nabi H, et al.: Low conscientiousness and risk of all-cause, cardiovascular and cancer mortality over 17 years: Whitehall II cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2012; 73:98–103.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Jokela M, Elovainio M, Nyberg ST, et al.: Personality and risk of diabetes in adults: Pooled analysis of 5 cohort studies. Health Psychol. 2014; 33:1618–1618.CrossRefPubMed Jokela M, Elovainio M, Nyberg ST, et al.: Personality and risk of diabetes in adults: Pooled analysis of 5 cohort studies. Health Psychol. 2014; 33:1618–1618.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, et al.: Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: A meta-analysis based on individual-participant data. Obes Rev. 2013; 14:315–323.CrossRefPubMed Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, et al.: Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: A meta-analysis based on individual-participant data. Obes Rev. 2013; 14:315–323.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Luchetti M, Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Sutin AR: Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: Data from a longitudinal sample and meta-analysis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015. Luchetti M, Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Sutin AR: Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: Data from a longitudinal sample and meta-analysis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Low LF, Harrison F, Lackersteen SM: Does personality affect risk for dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013; 21:713–728.CrossRefPubMed Low LF, Harrison F, Lackersteen SM: Does personality affect risk for dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013; 21:713–728.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ: Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosom. Med. 2007; 69:923–931.CrossRefPubMed Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ: Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosom. Med. 2007; 69:923–931.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, et al.: Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: A five-year population-based prospective cohort study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008; 16:399–405.CrossRefPubMed Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, et al.: Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: A five-year population-based prospective cohort study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008; 16:399–405.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Steptoe A, Breeze E, Banks J, Nazroo J: Cohort profile: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2013; 42:1640–1648.CrossRefPubMed Steptoe A, Breeze E, Banks J, Nazroo J: Cohort profile: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2013; 42:1640–1648.CrossRefPubMed
45.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachman ME, Weaver, S.L.: The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) Personality Scales: Scale Construction and Scoring. Waltham: MA: Brandeis University, 1997. Lachman ME, Weaver, S.L.: The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) Personality Scales: Scale Construction and Scoring. Waltham: MA: Brandeis University, 1997.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Banks J, Karlsen S, Oldfield Z: Socio-economic position. In M. Marmot, J. Banks, R. Blundell, C. Lessof and J. Nazroo (eds), Health, Wealth and Lifestyles of the Older Population in England. London: Institute of Fiscal Studies, 2003, 71–125. Banks J, Karlsen S, Oldfield Z: Socio-economic position. In M. Marmot, J. Banks, R. Blundell, C. Lessof and J. Nazroo (eds), Health, Wealth and Lifestyles of the Older Population in England. London: Institute of Fiscal Studies, 2003, 71–125.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Activity and Health Research. Allied Dunbar National Fitness Survey: Main findings. London: 1992. Activity and Health Research. Allied Dunbar National Fitness Survey: Main findings. London: 1992.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Royston P, Sauerbrei W: Multivariable Model-Building: A Pragmatic Approach to Regression Analysis Based on Fractional Polynomials for Modelling Continuous Variables. Chichester: Wiley, 2008.CrossRef Royston P, Sauerbrei W: Multivariable Model-Building: A Pragmatic Approach to Regression Analysis Based on Fractional Polynomials for Modelling Continuous Variables. Chichester: Wiley, 2008.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Royston P, Sauerbrei W: Handling Interactions in Stata, Especially with Continuous Predictors. German Stata Users' meeting. Berlin, 2012. Royston P, Sauerbrei W: Handling Interactions in Stata, Especially with Continuous Predictors. German Stata Users' meeting. Berlin, 2012.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Tolea MI, Terracciano A, Simonsick EM, et al.: Associations between personality traits, physical activity level, and muscle strength. J Res Pers. 2012; 46:264–270.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tolea MI, Terracciano A, Simonsick EM, et al.: Associations between personality traits, physical activity level, and muscle strength. J Res Pers. 2012; 46:264–270.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, et al.: Neuroticism, extraversion, and motor function in community-dwelling older persons. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013; 21:145–154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, et al.: Neuroticism, extraversion, and motor function in community-dwelling older persons. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013; 21:145–154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Tolea MI, Costa PT, Jr, Terracciano A, et al.: Associations of openness and conscientiousness with walking speed decline: Findings from the Health, Aging, and Body Composition Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012; 67:705–711.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tolea MI, Costa PT, Jr, Terracciano A, et al.: Associations of openness and conscientiousness with walking speed decline: Findings from the Health, Aging, and Body Composition Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012; 67:705–711.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Fried LP, Walston J: Frailty and failure to thrive. In W. R. Hazzard, J. Blass, W. H. Ettinger, J. Halter and J. Ouslander (eds), Principles of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1998, 1387–1402. Fried LP, Walston J: Frailty and failure to thrive. In W. R. Hazzard, J. Blass, W. H. Ettinger, J. Halter and J. Ouslander (eds), Principles of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1998, 1387–1402.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, et al.: High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychol Med. 2010; 40:1485–1493.CrossRefPubMed Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, et al.: High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychol Med. 2010; 40:1485–1493.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Mottus R, Luciano M, Starr JM, Pollard MC, Deary IJ: Personality traits and inflammation in men and women in their early 70s: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study of healthy aging. Psychosom Med. 2013; 75:11–19.CrossRefPubMed Mottus R, Luciano M, Starr JM, Pollard MC, Deary IJ: Personality traits and inflammation in men and women in their early 70s: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study of healthy aging. Psychosom Med. 2013; 75:11–19.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Mangold DL, Wand GS: Cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone responses to naloxone in subjects with high and low neuroticism. Biol Psychiatry. 2006; 60:850–855.CrossRefPubMed Mangold DL, Wand GS: Cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone responses to naloxone in subjects with high and low neuroticism. Biol Psychiatry. 2006; 60:850–855.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Baylis D, Bartlett DB, Syddall HE, et al.: Immune-endocrine biomarkers as predictors of frailty and mortality: A 10-year longitudinal study in community-dwelling older people. Age (Dordr.). 2012. Baylis D, Bartlett DB, Syddall HE, et al.: Immune-endocrine biomarkers as predictors of frailty and mortality: A 10-year longitudinal study in community-dwelling older people. Age (Dordr.). 2012.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Puts MT, Visser M, Twisk JW, Deeg DJ, Lips P: Endocrine and inflammatory markers as predictors of frailty. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf). 2005; 63:403–411.CrossRef Puts MT, Visser M, Twisk JW, Deeg DJ, Lips P: Endocrine and inflammatory markers as predictors of frailty. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf). 2005; 63:403–411.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Reiner AP, Aragaki AK, Gray SL, et al.: Inflammation and thrombosis biomarkers and incident frailty in postmenopausal women. Am. J. Med. 2009; 122:947–954.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Reiner AP, Aragaki AK, Gray SL, et al.: Inflammation and thrombosis biomarkers and incident frailty in postmenopausal women. Am. J. Med. 2009; 122:947–954.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Johar H, Emeny RT, Bidlingmaier M, et al.: Blunted diurnal cortisol pattern is associated with frailty: A cross-sectional study of 745 participants aged 65 to 90 years. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014; 99:E464–468.CrossRefPubMed Johar H, Emeny RT, Bidlingmaier M, et al.: Blunted diurnal cortisol pattern is associated with frailty: A cross-sectional study of 745 participants aged 65 to 90 years. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014; 99:E464–468.CrossRefPubMed
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR: The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socieconomic status and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007; 2:313–345.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR: The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socieconomic status and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007; 2:313–345.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa PT, McCrae RR: Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective (2nd Ed.). New York: Routledge, 2005. Costa PT, McCrae RR: Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective (2nd Ed.). New York: Routledge, 2005.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Small BJ, Hertzog C, Hultsch DF, Dixon RA, Victoria Longitudinal S: Stability and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2003; 58:P166–176.CrossRefPubMed Small BJ, Hertzog C, Hultsch DF, Dixon RA, Victoria Longitudinal S: Stability and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2003; 58:P166–176.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Vainik U, Mottus R, Allik J, Esko T, Realo A: Are trait-outcome associations caused by scales or particular items? Example analysis of personality facets and BMI. Eur J Pers. 2015. Vainik U, Mottus R, Allik J, Esko T, Realo A: Are trait-outcome associations caused by scales or particular items? Example analysis of personality facets and BMI. Eur J Pers. 2015.
Metadaten
Titel
Personality and Risk of Frailty: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing
verfasst von
Catharine R Gale, PhD
René Mõttus, PhD
Ian J Deary, PhD
Cyrus Cooper, DM
Avan Aihie Sayer, PhD
Publikationsdatum
22.09.2016
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Annals of Behavioral Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0883-6612
Elektronische ISSN: 1532-4796
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9833-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

Annals of Behavioral Medicine 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie