Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Study protocol

Rationale and design of a randomized controlled trial examining the effect of classroom-based physical activity on math achievement

verfasst von: Mona Have, Jacob Have Nielsen, Anne Kær Gejl, Martin Thomsen Ernst, Kjeld Fredens, Jan Toftegaard Støckel, Niels Wedderkopp, Sidsel Louise Domazet, Claire Gudex, Anders Grøntved, Peter Lund Kristensen

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Integration of physical activity (PA) into the classroom may be an effective way of promoting the learning and academic achievement of children at elementary school. This paper describes the research design and methodology of an intervention study examining the effect of classroom-based PA on mathematical achievement, creativity, executive function, body mass index and aerobic fitness.

Methods

The study was designed as a school-based cluster-randomized controlled trial targeting schoolchildren in 1st grade, and was carried out between August 2012 and June 2013. Eligible schools in two municipalities in the Region of Southern Denmark were invited to participate in the study. After stratification by municipality, twelve schools were randomized to either an intervention group or a control group, comprising a total of 505 children with mean age 7.2 ± 0.3 years. The intervention was a 9-month classroom-based PA program that involved integration of PA into the math lessons delivered by the schools’ math teachers. The primary study outcome was change in math achievement, measured by a 45-minute standardized math test. Secondary outcomes were change in executive function (using a modified Eriksen flanker task and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) questionnaire filled out by the parents), creativity (using the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, TTCT), aerobic fitness (by the Andersen intermittent shuttle-run test) and body mass index. PA during math lessons and total PA (including time spent outside school) were assessed using accelerometry. Math teachers used Short Message Service (SMS)-tracking to report on compliance with the PA intervention and on their motivation for implementing PA in math lessons. Parents used SMS-tracking to register their children’s PA behavior in leisure time.

Discussion

The results of this randomized controlled trial are expected to provide schools and policy-makers with significant new insights into the potential of classroom-based PA to improve cognition and academic achievement in children.

Trial registration

Clinicaltrials.gov: NCT02488460 (06/29/2015)
Hinweise

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MH designed and conducted the study, designed, planned, coordinated and supervised the data collection, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AKG supervised the data collection, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JHN helped design the study, planned and coordinated the data collection and critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JTS helped design the study, reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. MTE revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AG supervised the statistical considerations, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. PLK supervised the statistical considerations, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. KF helped design the study, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. SLD revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. NW revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. CG revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors have approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Abkürzungen
BRI
behavior regulating index
BRIEF
behavior rating inventory of executive function
cpm
counts per minute
GEC
global executive composite
IMK
Ib Michael Kristiansen Almene Foundation
kg
kilograms
m
meters
MCI
metacognition index
MG
matematik grundlæggende
MI
movement integration
msec
milliseconds
MVPA
moderate-to-vigorous physical activity
PA
physical activity
PE
physical education
RT
reaction time
SD
standard deviation
SMS
short message service
TTCT
torrance tests of creative thinking

Background

Physical activity (PA) in schools has historically received little attention and, in many countries, economic concerns and a greater focus on standardized testing have reduced the time spent on physical activity in favor of academic classes [14]. A regular school day often includes very little PA and children are required to sit quietly in the same position for long periods of time [57]. However, this may not be the most effective way of enhancing children’s academic achievement. Breaking up the sitting periods with physical activity might be a better approach to promote learning [8, 9].
PA has several cognitive and physiological benefits [10, 11]. A meta-analysis showed a significant positive relationship between PA in general and cognition in children [12]. This is consistent with the “executive function hypothesis”, in which PA is believed to enhance executive functions that are crucial to the learning process, such as organization and integration of information, and modulation of behavior through selective attention (e.g. [1316]). Studies report clear acute cognitive benefits of PA, typically increased information processing speed or response accuracy [1719]. Early experiments on the effects of regular PA showed either no effect of PA on cognitive outcomes [20] or limited improvements [21, 22]. However, recent studies report positive effects of regular PA on the structures and functions of the brain [14, 2325]. Possible neurophysiological mechanisms of PA include changes in brain blood flow and arousal level, improved conduction of information due to increased concentration of neurotransmitters and receptors, as well as increased concentrations of growth factors important for the development of new brain cells [26]. In addition, regular PA may increase brain volume (e.g. the volume of the anterior hippocampus), leading to improved spatial and relational memory [14, 27, 28]. Lastly, a recent study demonstrated a positive relation between mathematic achievement and decreased gray matter thickness in the superior frontal cortex, superior temporal areas, and lateral occipital cortex, in more fit children [29]. This suggests that aerobic fitness is essential for the normal thinning of the cortical gray matter during brain maturation, thus providing us with a predictor of math performance that could in turn help educators to identify interventions that can enhance learning.
Despite the increasing evidence for an association between PA or fitness and cognitive performance [3035], few high-quality longitudinal studies have examined the effect of PA on academic performance (rather than cognitive control or other educational outcomes) [8, 14]. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, only three studies have assessed academic outcomes following the integration of PA into the classroom. A study conducted among US elementary school children reported that children with 90 min/week of moderate to vigorous physical activity in academic lessons over three years showed greater academic improvement than controls [36]. However, these results were considered secondary and were based on a small subsample (n = 203) of the study. Another small-scale randomized experiment from the US conducted among 3rd grade children showed that integrating 30 min of PA three days per week for three months gave mixed results for academic improvement compared with controls [37]. Finally, a recent study showed that physically active academic lessons significantly improved mathematics and spelling performance of elementary school children [38].
Even the studies investigating the link between PA and cognition have typically examined cognition and PA separately, possibly due to a commonly held view that the mind and body function separately. However, recent theories of embodied cognition have emerged in several disciplines to challenge this traditional dualistic approach [3946]. These theories consider the body to be a key factor in shaping our cognition, where ‘non-cognitive’ processes of acting and perceiving providing bodily experience are fundamental to and inseparable from the development of important cognitive functions. This emphasizes the importance of sensory–motor interactions with our environment during learning, resulting in more endurable and richer consolidation of knowledge [47]. This psychological and neuroscientific research has implications for education as it emphasizes the importance of suitable sensory and motor interactions during learning for the development of human cognition [47].
We designed a randomized controlled trial with the primary aim of investigating the effect on mathematical achievement of incorporating physical activity in math teaching for 7-year-old schoolchildren. Secondary aims of the study were to investigate effects on executive function, and the longitudinal associations between PA and creativity, math, executive function, body mass index and aerobic fitness. The hypothesis was that PA integrated into classroom teaching would be superior to traditional, physically inactive classroom teaching in facilitating learning, and would be associated with a greater improvement in academic skills.

Methods

Study design

The study was designed as a cluster-randomized controlled trial with two arms. In two municipalities in the southern part of Denmark, twelve elementary schools were randomized to either active math intervention (n = 7) or control (n = 5) (Fig. 1). Randomization was performed by random selection of sealed envelopes containing the intervention allocation stratified by municipality, in the presence of school leaders, municipality representatives and study researchers.

Recruitment of participants

We approached three municipalities in the Region of Southern Denmark with the aim of recruiting schools and 1st grade children to the study. One municipality (15 schools) declined to participate. We hypothesized that children in this age group would particularly benefit from a classroom-based PA intervention, as they were likely to be struggling to sit in the same position in lessons for the majority of the day. Schools in these municipalities were eligible if they did not have a structured program that incorporated PA in the classroom.
Out of the 48 eligible schools that were invited to participate in the study, 12 schools accepted (7 in Svendborg municipality and 5 in Kolding municipality). The 1st grade children in these schools were invited to participate in a letter describing the study, measurement methods, randomization procedure and consent form. The children, parents and teachers were then invited to a meeting at the school, where a full description of the scientific background and the study was given. After this, 90.7 % of parents gave consent for their child to participate in the study, corresponding to 505 out of 557 students in 26 different classes. The excluded children received the same intervention as their classmates but did not participate in testing. A flowchart illustrating the recruitment and randomization process is shown in Fig. 1.
In Svendborg municipality, the participating schools were all part of an existing intervention study that had been initiated in 2008 (the CHAMPS-study DK). This intervention consisted of four extra lessons of physical education (PE) each week, in addition to the two compulsory lessons, resulting in a total of 4.5 h extra per week, divided over at least three weekly sessions of at least 60 min (a more detailed description of the CHAMPS-study DK can be found in [48]). To ensure that the extra PE lessons in Svendborg municipality did not bias the results of the present study, randomization to the intervention was stratified by municipality.
All schools were co-educational schools, with gender integrated classes in all subjects, which is typical for the Danish educational system.
Written parental consent was requested from parents in the included schools. Exclusion criteria were physical disability or no written parental consent. The trial protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Region of Southern Denmark (S-20140105) and registered at clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02488460).

The classroom-based PA intervention

The active math intervention consisted of math teaching that implemented PA in the classroom as a facilitating instrument. The intervention period lasted one school year, during which the students received on average 6 math lessons of 45 min per week with physically active teaching. PA in this math intervention was defined as any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that resulted in increased energy expenditure [49]. This is similar to the definition known as movement integration (MI) [50].
Each 45-minute lesson consisted of at least 15 min of PA spread over the lesson. Math lessons usually took place in the classroom, but were also held in other school areas such as corridors and surrounding outdoor areas. Sedentary activities were limited to periods of maximum 20 min, given that most students are unable to sustain attention on one thing for more than about 20 min at a time [51]. The rationale behind the active math was that if an abstract phenomenon is connected to a concrete bodily experience other than just seeing or hearing it, the comprehension and memory of the given phenomenon could be optimized (e.g. [52, 53]). In addition, we expected that breaking up the cognitive load imposed by a learning task would limit overload of working memory in the children and thus help maintain focus [54]. Studies have detected limited working memory capacity in children, and spreading the load across different working memory subsystems might prevent overload in one specific subsystem [5557]. The active math lessons followed the principles of “learning by doing” and the development of practical life skills that are crucial to children’s education [58]. This is related to experiential learning theory, i.e. learning through action and experience as opposed to through repetition [59, 60]. The active math approach was adopted for all math lessons for the entire school year. Appendix 1 provides examples from the active math intervention.
Children in the control schools received regular classroom instruction, also with an average of 6 math lessons of 45 min per week. The math teachers in the control schools were asked not to make any changes to their usual teaching methods before the study endpoint measurements. This was accepted with the agreement that at the end of the study, these teachers would receive the same training in physically active math teaching as the teachers in the intervention group.

Teacher training for the intervention

Teachers in the intervention schools attended a 4-day mandatory course on how to integrate active math into the Danish curriculum for mathematics in public schools. The aim of the course was to provide the teachers with the skills to implement physically active math lessons, i.e. to use suggested methods and develop new ways of integrating physical activity into lessons, to organize the activities and to report on intensity of PA (for an overview of the course plan, see Appendix 2).
The research group developed the training course and the associated training material that included specific instructions and inspiration for how teachers could integrate PA as an element of academic instruction in math lessons. Examples from this material were used to help illustrate the practical dimensions of the active math lessons. It should be noted that the focus of the course was on developing the teacher’s own abilities to design active math lessons and thus their own pedagogical skills and knowledge rather than handing out a complete manual with activities for the math lessons. The course included an introduction to the study context and aims, with emphasis on creating high motivation among the teachers for the intervention by explaining the numerous potential positive cognitive effects of learning through action and describing previous findings related to PA in general. The teacher’s motivation for implementing PA in the classroom and their involvement are highly relevant matters for math achievement [61], and could influence the outcome of the study. The teachers thus played an active role in creating and developing the activities for the math lessons. Furthermore, several theories of how to develop activities in the classroom were presented e.g. the ‘Activity-wheel’ [62], with the purpose of creating activities that would help the children to reach the desired learning objectives.
On the last course day, active teaching techniques were discussed and related to motivational, organizational and management techniques. Several teachers attending the course highlighted the challenges they expected to face when implementing PA in math. This initiated an internet-based intra-school communication forum in each of the two municipalities, where the teachers shared activities, experiences and advice for the active math intervention. The teachers received normal salary for attending the course, as compensation for their time.

Baseline and follow-up assessments

The study was carried out over nine months from August 2012 to May/June 2013. Unfortunately during this period, all Danish schools were closed for a month due to a teacher lockout as a result of a union-employer dispute. Thus in the final period of the intervention, participants did not attend school for 25 days during April/May 2013. This period ended 4 weeks prior to the end of the intervention.
The test battery included measurements of math skills, cognitive function, creativity, aerobic fitness, anthropometrics and level of PA. Prior to testing, the research assistants (i.e. master’s and PhD students) were thoroughly trained in conduction of the test battery to ensure valid and uniform data collection. The training included several test trials with feedback and was performed at the University of Southern Denmark by a researcher experienced in the techniques. The research assistants were blinded to the randomization result for measurement of the outcomes and for data entry. All tests were performed at the participating schools either in the classrooms or a gym, except for the assessment of executive function (Flanker task), which was performed in smaller rooms with a maximum of two students present at the same time. Each outcome measure was assessed at baseline and follow-up using the methods described below. The tests applied at baseline and follow-up were identical.

Mathematical skills test

Math skills were evaluated using a 45-minute standardized math test (MG) that was specifically designed for this age group by the developer of the Danish national tests (Hoegrefe Forlag) [63]. It consisted of 24 tasks assessing calculus and math in terms of the understanding of quantity and numbers, relations, addition, subtraction and geometry, with the final score ranging between 0 and 24. MG is a common test material in Danish primary schools and has been used for more than 25 years [63]. It is based on multiple-choice questions, and the different response options were selected in such a way that incorrect options related to typical mistakes made by students in 1st grade. The test provides a score reflecting the participant’s math skills and was conducted individually with paper and pencil in a classroom, with no aids permitted. Following a general presentation of the test, the participants were presented with each task separately and given 1 min to answer. The number of correct answers was used for further analysis.

Flanker test

Executive functions were identified as working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility, which were evaluated using a computer-based modified Erickson Flanker task [64]. Modified versions of the Flanker task have been used in previous studies indicating that the test is sensitive enough to detect changes in cognitive function associated with exercise [65]. The tests were conducted with the child seated at a 15” laptop with an external keyboard on which the corner keys with right and left arrows were clearly marked.
The task was to focus on a target stimulus ignoring other distracting stimuli. In this version, the stimuli were pictograms of fish, and the child was instructed to “feed” the fish by pressing the arrow in the direction the target stimulus (fish) was facing (Fig. 2). The test consisted of three conditions: (a) standard flanker, (b) reverse flanker, and (c) mixed trials. In the standard flanker condition, fish were blue and subjects were instructed to press the arrow that pointed in the direction the middle fish was facing (target stimulus), ignoring the distractor fish on either side. This task required remembering the rule for the task, paying attention to the task, and inhibiting distraction from the flanker fish on either side of the target stimulus. In the reverse flanker condition, the fish were pink. In contrast to the previous task, the child was now instructed to press the arrow that pointed in the direction the fish on either side (target stimulus) of the central fish were facing. Not only did this task require remembering the new rule for the task and selective attention, it also required cognitive flexibility to change from the strategy used for the standard flanker task. In the third condition of mixed trials, standard flanker (blue fish) and reverse flanker (pink fish) tasks were randomly intermingled, requiring flexible application of the rules for each task. This made a heavy demand on all three core executive functions and was expected to detect small individual and group differences.
The child was thoroughly instructed in the test by one of the research assistants, and made two familiarization rounds with blue and pink fish to ensure understanding of the task. Familiarization rounds consisted of four ‘habituation tasks’ where the child received verbal feedback from the test administrator, and then 17 further tasks. The child was instructed to respond as quickly and as accurately as possibly by pressing the left or right arrow on the keyboard. Each task was shown on the screen until an answer was given.
The test itself consisted of 45 tasks, with the first one not included in the analysis to exclude possible delay in the start-up. To test the child’s ability to focus and exclude interfering stimuli, the flankers were divided into four categories: 1) ‘congruent’ stimuli in which the flanking stimuli were identical to the target stimulus, 2) ‘incongruent’ stimuli in which the flanking stimuli were the opposite of the target stimulus, 3) ‘No distractors’ with no flanking stimuli and 4) ‘Neutrals’ in which the flanking stimuli were neither identical nor opposite of the primary stimuli but were vertically placed. The 44 tasks were divided equally between flanker conditions and contained 12 congruent, 16 incongruent, 8 no-distracter and 8 neutral tasks. These were also evenly divided between right and left as the correct answer. The stimulus presentation time was 1500 ms, the feedback interval was 1000 ms, and the interstimulus interval was 500 ms. The response time of the different tasks were recorded and the following variables were calculated: i) % accurate congruent answers, ii) % accurate incongruent answers, iii) the reaction time for correct congruent answers, and iv) the reaction time for correct incongruent answers. These parameters have previously been related to high levels of physical activity and fitness [66].

BRIEF questionnaire

The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) is an assessment of behaviors at home for children and adolescents aged 5–18 years [67]. One parent or guardian for each child was asked to answer the questionnaire. After 3–4 weeks, parents were reminded to return the questionnaire via a message on the school’s intranet. Parents rated their child’s behavior on a three-point Likert scale (never, sometimes, and often). Eight scales were obtained (Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, Monitor, Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control), along with a metacognition index (MCI), a behavior regulation index (BRI), and a global executive composite (GEC). The BRIEF Parent Form was normed on 1419 control children and 852 children from referred clinical groups; the Cronbach coefficient measure of internal consistency ranged from 0.80 to 0.98 for the parent and teacher form in clinical and normative samples [67].

Creativity

Creativity was assessed because of a potential executive involvement in creative thought. We used the 30-minute standardized Figural Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT): Thinking Creatively with Pictures, which requires the subject to reflect upon their life experiences. It is the most widely used test of creativity [68] and the most referenced creativity test [69]. The child was given three different activities and asked to draw and title one drawing to each task (pictures). Due to the mean child age of 7.2 ± 0.3 years, the Figural TTCT was chosen over the Verbal TTCT in liaison with the developer (Scholastic Testing Service) to avoid test results being influenced by writing skills. The Figural TTCT assesses the following mental characteristics: i) Fluency, a measure of the total number of interpretable, meaningful, and relevant ideas generated in response to the stimulus ii) Flexibility, related to the number of different categories of relevant responses iii) Originality, a measure of the statistical rarity of the responses, iv) Elaboration, related to the number of added ideas. The test was conducted according to the developer’s manual. It was completed individually with paper and pencil in a classroom, but was administered as a group test. After a general presentation of the test, the children were presented to each of the three tasks separately. Ten minutes were given to complete each activity.
In Activity I, the child constructed a picture using a pear or jellybean shape provided on the page as a stimulus. The stimulus was required to be an integral part of the picture construction. Activity II required the child to use 10 incomplete figures to make an object or picture. Activity III, was composed of three pages of lines or circles which the child should use as a part of the picture [70]. No aids were permitted.

Aerobic fitness

Aerobic fitness was assessed using the Andersen test, an intermittent shuttle run test [71] that has previously been shown to be easy applicable and valid (r = 0.68) for measuring aerobic fitness in younger study populations. The children were instructed to run as far as possible in 10 min back and forth between two lines 20 m apart. This was performed in bouts of 15 s running followed by 15 s rest before the next bout. Participants were carefully instructed to take no more than two steps to stop after each bout of running and to sit down after the last bout facing the running direction to allow measurement of the distance of the last incomplete 2x20m round. Test personnel controlled the time and used a whistle to start and stop the participants. The test result was total distance in meters which has been validated against direct measures of maximal VO2-uptake [71, 72]. The Andersen test was found to be reproducible and valid (r2 = 0.53, n = 32, p < 0.001) for determining intermittent exercise capacity and estimating maximal oxygen uptake for children aged 6–9 years [72].

Anthropometric measures

Body height was measured without shoes to the nearest 0.5 cm using a Harpenden stadiometer (West Sussex, UK). Body mass was measured in light clothing to the nearest 0.1 kg using an electronic scale (Tanita BWB-800, Tokyo, Japan). Body mass index was calculated as body mass (kg) divided by height (m) squared.

Assessment of classroom physical activity and total physical activity

Intervention compliance was assessed using different methods targeting teachers and parents as well as the children. This included video analysis of lessons, SMS-tracking, and accelerometry.

Video analysis

Math lessons were video-filmed for one week halfway during the intervention. The video camera (Mini DV DVR AVI Hidden Spy Video Camera & Voice recorder, AEE, China) was hidden to avoid behavioral change due to the camera attention. The teacher was asked to start and stop the camera at the beginning and end of the lesson.

SMS-tracking

Short Message Service (SMS)-tracking was used to collect data from teachers about types and frequency of classroom PA elements. Each month, the math teachers received an SMS every day for one week asking about length, intensity of the PA (with specific examples of the three different intensities) and the teacher’s motivation for implementing PA in the math lessons. SMS-tracking was also used to assess the child’s leisure PA and school transport. This was accomplished by asking the parents to answer three questions. Parents received an SMS once a week during the entire intervention (see Appendix 3 for questions to teachers and parents).

Classroom PA and total PA by students using accelerometry

The level of PA during math lessons and total daily PA were assessed using accelerometry (ActiGraph, GT3X and GT3X+, ActiGraph LLC, Pensacola, FL, USA). PA data were collected for eight days at baseline and again just before follow-up measurements. The accelerometers were randomly distributed by the research staff to all participants, who were instructed to wear the monitor during waking hours for at least eight consecutive days. Assessment periods were balanced between control and intervention schools. To eliminate reactivity bias, the monitors started recording the day after distribution. Epoch length was set to 2 s but subsequently downloaded in 10-s epochs. A sequence of more than 60 min of consecutive zeros was considered non-wear time and eliminated from the analysis. To account for children who failed to remove the monitor at night, only activity recorded between 6 am and 10 pm was analyzed. A valid measurement of total PA was defined as a minimum of four days with at least 10 h of recorded activity each day. For PA levels during math lessons only one valid day of measurement was required for inclusion in the analysis. Propero software (University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark) was used to prepare the data for further analysis and to specify the time-points at which, according to the class schedule, math lessons took place. Total PA was expressed as mean counts per minute (cpm) and mean minutes in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) per day. Intensity cut-off points by Evenson et al. were used to define MVPA [73].

Statistics

Descriptive statistics according to outcome measures and relevant baseline characteristics will be calculated and compared across allocation groups.
Using conventional two-sided levels of significance (0.05) and statistical power (0.8) and given a sample size of 550 children from 12 different schools, the minimum detectable difference in the change in math score between the intervention and control group will be 0.9 points, assuming a standard deviation of the change in math score of 3.6 points. If a school-level cluster effect of 0.07 is taken into account, the minimum detectable difference in the change in math score will be 1.9 points.
The effect of the intervention will be tested using mixed effects regression models adjusted for relevant potential confounding factors that were not randomly distributed in children from the intervention and control schools (i.e. gender, age, baseline value of the variable, socioeconomic status). Because of the clustered nature of the data, schools will be included as random effects in the analyses. All data will be entered twice and thereafter checked manually for outliers. Analyses will be performed with statistical software STATA version 14.

Discussion

The literature lacks well-designed large-scaled randomized controlled trials on the effect of classroom-based physical activity on academic achievement and cognition. The present study will contribute to the field by generating detailed knowledge about the relationship between physical experiences that are combined with abstract math information and the acquisition of math skills. We expect the study results to provide schools and education policy-makers with an evidence base for implementing physical activity in daily school life, as a change from schoolchildren sitting still much of the day.
Apart from the large sample size and the comprehensive follow-up of children’s learning experiences and physical behavior over nine months, this study includes detailed measurement of the primary and secondary outcomes using well-validated methods. The math test applied in this study has been used for over 25 years to assess the math skills of Danish schoolchildren. The complex flanker task that assesses the three core executive functions will allow us to verify the few previous research findings concerning the association between a change in physical activity level and the ability to focus on a target stimulus and inhibit distractions [14, 74]. Furthermore, we expect the combination of SMS-tracking and accelerometry to be an excellent solution for measuring physical activity in large cohort studies. This makes it possible to compensate for some of the problems associated with accelerometry measurements, for example by using SMS-tracking to ask questions about specific activities, including water activities, and to allow more precise measurement of exercise intensity during cycling. When all this information is coupled to the class school timetable, then we can analyze the physical activity undertaken in specific time periods such as lunch breaks or during the math lessons. However, accelerometry does not provide any qualitative detail about the types or modes of activity being undertaken. For this reason accelerometry measurements were combined with video recordings of math lessons.
An important limitation of this study is the lack of qualitative data collection. This could have been used to further describe the various interventions and the complexity of the social contexts in which the active math was tested, (i.e. through case studies of a specific child/teacher or a specific context, observations of classroom activities, or qualitative interviews with children, teachers, and parents). Due to economic reasons, qualitative assessment was not performed since the main objective of the study was of quantitative character.
In conclusion, the results of this study will expand the current evidence on the relationship between physical activity and academic achievement in schoolchildren. We expect the results to stimulate the debate about whether the integration of physical activity into the classroom can enhance children’s cognitive skills and creativity, and will help educational practitioners to design learning environments that are optimal for cognitive development and academic achievement.

Approval and registration

Ethics: Ethics committee of the Region of Southern Denmark (S-20140105)
Trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02488460).

Acknowledgements

We thank the schoolchildren and their families and schoolteachers for their participation, and the education administrative personnel and research staff for their support and efforts during the study. We also thank secretaries Charlotte Dickmeiss and Birgitte Laursen and the Ib Michael Kristiansen (IMK) Almene Foundation for providing financial support for the study.

Funding

This research was funded by the IMK foundation, Copenhagen and the University of Southern Denmark, Odense.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MH designed and conducted the study, designed, planned, coordinated and supervised the data collection, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AKG supervised the data collection, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JHN helped design the study, planned and coordinated the data collection and critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JTS helped design the study, reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. MTE revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AG supervised the statistical considerations, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. PLK supervised the statistical considerations, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. KF helped design the study, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. SLD revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. NW revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. CG revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors have approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Anhänge

Appendix 1

Active math intervention - Example 1: Mathematics darts

Aim

The aim of this activity is to rehearse simple mental arithmetic, i.e. to become familiar with numbers, addition, subtraction and mixed operations.

Materials

Balls or small sandbags to throw. Plates (approximately 1 x 1 m) with painted numbers and arithmetic operators (+, −, x,/), or you could draw these on the blackboard or outside on the floor/ground. The plates are divided into different sections (plus, times, minus, divide, etc. and various numbers 0 to 9).

Process

Students paired two and two are equipped with the balls and throw them one at a time at the numbers and operators. The aim is to be the couple who “reach” a predetermined number, such as “300” as fast as possible with the fewest throws. If the first of a pair of students throws three balls and hits “4”, “9” and “time sign”, they can form the equation 4x9 = 36, and have earned 36 points. If the student only hits numbers and no operators, then it can be agreed that the numbers must be added, or that no points are achieved in that round. The second student then takes their turn, and they continue to alternate until someone reaches the agreed total score.

Variation and progression

This task is quite versatile and the students can work with elementary multiplication and simple division or they can work with more complicated math questions, decimals and fractions. They can also bring paper or chalk to write down the calculations and remember them.
  • The operator signs can be varied (e.g. plus, minus, multiply, divide, power, square root, parentheses)
  • The numbers on the plates can be varied (e.g. natural numbers, integers, fractions, decimals)
  • The students can do the task individually, in pairs, or in groups with or without timed competition
  • The number of balls thrown in each round can be varied.

Active math intervention - Example 2: Skipping rope

In this activity, the children are divided into pairs and they skip calculations to each other. One child skips while the other counts. If the child skips on two legs, one skip counts for 10, and a one-legged skip counts for 1. Thus if a child jumps three times with both legs and two times with one leg, the answer will be 32. This activity can also be made with ‘minus’, by skipping backwards and ‘multiply’ by skipping once with arms crossed.

Appendix 2

Appendix 3

Short message service (SMS)-tracking for teachers

  • How many times have you started an activity where the students have gone from sitting still to being active (that is, where they get away from their chairs)?
  • How many minutes has the class been active at low, moderate and high intensity?
One Friday every month, the teachers answered the following questions on motivation:
  • How important do you think physical activity is to promote students’ acquisition of math on a scale of 1 to 6, where 6 is most important?
  • How motivated were you this week to implement physical activity in math lessons on a scale of 1 to 6, where 6 is extremely high motivation?

Short message service (SMS)-tracking for parents

1)
“How many times did [NAME OF CHILD] engage in sports during the last week”?
 
The parents were instructed to answer with the relevant number between 0 and 8. The answers 0 to 7 reflected the unique number of times engaging in sports, while 8 stood for ‘more than 7 times’.
2)
Which type of sports did [NAME OF CHILD] undertake?
 
The parents were instructed to answer with a number between 0 and 10. The answers 0 to 9 each represented a unique type of sport, while 10 stood for ‘something else’.
3)
How many times has [NAME OF CHILD] cycled to school in the last week?
 
The returned answers were automatically recorded and inserted into a database. To improve compliance rates, the responders were contacted by telephone if the answer did not fit with the instructions. Furthermore, a reminder was automatically sent if a participant had not responded within 72 h and, if necessary, again within 120 h of receiving the message.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Center on Education Policy. Choices, changes, and challenges: curriculum and instruction in the NCLB era. In: From the Capital to the Classroom. Washington: Center on Education Policy; 2007. Center on Education Policy. Choices, changes, and challenges: curriculum and instruction in the NCLB era. In: From the Capital to the Classroom. Washington: Center on Education Policy; 2007.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat National Association for Sport and Physical Education. Comprehensive school physical activity programs, position statement. Reston: 2008. National Association for Sport and Physical Education. Comprehensive school physical activity programs, position statement. Reston: 2008.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Trost SG, van der Mars H. Why We Should Not Cut PE. Educational Leadership. 2009;67:60–5. Trost SG, van der Mars H. Why We Should Not Cut PE. Educational Leadership. 2009;67:60–5.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Abbott RA, Straker LM, Mathiassen SE. Patterning of Children’s Sedentary Time At and Away from School. Obesity. 2013;21:E131–3.CrossRefPubMed Abbott RA, Straker LM, Mathiassen SE. Patterning of Children’s Sedentary Time At and Away from School. Obesity. 2013;21:E131–3.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Mantjes JA, Jones AP, Corder K, Jones NR, Harrison F, Griffin SJ, van Sluijs EMF. School related factors and 1 year change in physical activity amongst 9–11 year old English schoolchildren. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:153.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mantjes JA, Jones AP, Corder K, Jones NR, Harrison F, Griffin SJ, van Sluijs EMF. School related factors and 1 year change in physical activity amongst 9–11 year old English schoolchildren. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:153.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Esliger DW, Hall J. Accelerometry in children. In: Craig R, Mindell J, Hirani V, editors. Health Survey for England 2008: Physical activity and fitness, vol. 1. London: The Health and Social Care Information Centre; 2009. p. 159–80. Esliger DW, Hall J. Accelerometry in children. In: Craig R, Mindell J, Hirani V, editors. Health Survey for England 2008: Physical activity and fitness, vol. 1. London: The Health and Social Care Information Centre; 2009. p. 159–80.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA, Smith BK, Washburn RA, Sullivan DK, DuBose K, Mayo MS, Schmelzle KH, Ryan JJ, et al. Physical Activity Across the Curriculum (PAAC): A randomized controlled trial to promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity in elementary school children. Prev Med. 2009;49:336–41.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA, Smith BK, Washburn RA, Sullivan DK, DuBose K, Mayo MS, Schmelzle KH, Ryan JJ, et al. Physical Activity Across the Curriculum (PAAC): A randomized controlled trial to promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity in elementary school children. Prev Med. 2009;49:336–41.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Mahar MT, Murphy SK, Rowe DA, Golden J, Shields AT, Raedeke TD. Effects of a classroom-based program on physical activity and on-task behavior. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:2086–94.CrossRefPubMed Mahar MT, Murphy SK, Rowe DA, Golden J, Shields AT, Raedeke TD. Effects of a classroom-based program on physical activity and on-task behavior. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:2086–94.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children’s cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015;4:47–55.CrossRef Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children’s cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015;4:47–55.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman CH. An Introduction to the Relation of Physical Activity to Cognitive and Brain Health, and Scholastic Achievement. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2014;79:1–6.CrossRefPubMed Hillman CH. An Introduction to the Relation of Physical Activity to Cognitive and Brain Health, and Scholastic Achievement. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2014;79:1–6.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2003;15:243–56.CrossRef Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2003;15:243–56.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review. 2010;30:331–51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review. 2010;30:331–51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, Moore RD, Wu CT, Kamijo K. Effects of the FITKids Randomized Controlled Trial on Executive Control and Brain Function. Pediatrics. 2014;134:E1063–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, Moore RD, Wu CT, Kamijo K. Effects of the FITKids Randomized Controlled Trial on Executive Control and Brain Function. Pediatrics. 2014;134:E1063–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS. Physical activity interventions and children’s mental function: An introduction and overview. Prev Med. 2011;52:S3–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS. Physical activity interventions and children’s mental function: An introduction and overview. Prev Med. 2011;52:S3–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Castelli DM, Hall EE, Kramer AF. The Effect of Acute Treadmill Walking on Cognitive Control and Academic Achievement in Preadolescent Children. Neuroscience. 2009;159:1044–54.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Castelli DM, Hall EE, Kramer AF. The Effect of Acute Treadmill Walking on Cognitive Control and Academic Achievement in Preadolescent Children. Neuroscience. 2009;159:1044–54.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL. The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:728–34.CrossRefPubMed Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL. The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:728–34.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Winter B, Breitenstein C, Mooren FC, Voelker K, Fobker M, Lechtermann A, Krueger K, Fromme A, Korsukewitz C, Floel A, Knecht S. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:597–609.CrossRefPubMed Winter B, Breitenstein C, Mooren FC, Voelker K, Fobker M, Lechtermann A, Krueger K, Fromme A, Korsukewitz C, Floel A, Knecht S. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:597–609.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Ismail AH. The effects of a well-organized physical education programme on intellectual performance. Physics Education Research. 1967;1:31–8. Ismail AH. The effects of a well-organized physical education programme on intellectual performance. Physics Education Research. 1967;1:31–8.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuckman BW, Hinkle JS. An Experimental-study of the Physcial and Psychological Effects of Aerobic Exercise on Schoolchildren. Health Psychol. 1986;5:197–207.CrossRefPubMed Tuckman BW, Hinkle JS. An Experimental-study of the Physcial and Psychological Effects of Aerobic Exercise on Schoolchildren. Health Psychol. 1986;5:197–207.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hinkle JS, Tuckman BW, Sampson JP. The Psychology, Physiology, and Creativity of Middle School Aerobic Exercisers. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling. 1993;28:133–45. Hinkle JS, Tuckman BW, Sampson JP. The Psychology, Physiology, and Creativity of Middle School Aerobic Exercisers. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling. 1993;28:133–45.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, Yanasak NE, Allison JD, Naglieri JA. Exercise Improves Executive Function and Achievement and Alters Brain Activation in Overweight Children: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Health Psychol. 2011;30:91–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, Yanasak NE, Allison JD, Naglieri JA. Exercise Improves Executive Function and Achievement and Alters Brain Activation in Overweight Children: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Health Psychol. 2011;30:91–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher A, Boyle JME, Paton JY, Tomporowski P, Watson C, McColl JH, Reilly JJ. Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11:97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fisher A, Boyle JME, Paton JY, Tomporowski P, Watson C, McColl JH, Reilly JJ. Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11:97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Stroth S, Kubesch S, Dieterle K, Ruchsow M, Heim R, Kiefer M. Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents. Brain Res. 2009;1269:114–24.CrossRefPubMed Stroth S, Kubesch S, Dieterle K, Ruchsow M, Heim R, Kiefer M. Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents. Brain Res. 2009;1269:114–24.CrossRefPubMed
26.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:3017–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:3017–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaddock L, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. A Review of the Relation of Aerobic Fitness and Physical Activity to Brain Structure and Function in Children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:975–85.CrossRefPubMed Chaddock L, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. A Review of the Relation of Aerobic Fitness and Physical Activity to Brain Structure and Function in Children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:975–85.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Kienzler C, King M, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. The Role of Aerobic Fitness in Cortical Thickness and Mathematics Achievement in Preadolescent Children. Plos One. 2015;10:11. Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Kienzler C, King M, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. The Role of Aerobic Fitness in Cortical Thickness and Mathematics Achievement in Preadolescent Children. Plos One. 2015;10:11.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:1515–9.CrossRefPubMed Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:1515–9.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Castelli DM, Centeio EE, Hwang J, Barcelona JM, Glowacki EM, Calvert HG, Nicksic HM. The History of Physical Activity and Academic Performance Research: Informing the Future. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2014;79:119–48.CrossRefPubMed Castelli DM, Centeio EE, Hwang J, Barcelona JM, Glowacki EM, Calvert HG, Nicksic HM. The History of Physical Activity and Academic Performance Research: Informing the Future. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2014;79:119–48.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007;29:239–52.CrossRefPubMed Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007;29:239–52.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Sardinha LB, Marques A, Martins S, Palmeira A, Minderico C. Fitness, fatness, and academic performance in seventh-grade elementary school students. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14:176.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sardinha LB, Marques A, Martins S, Palmeira A, Minderico C. Fitness, fatness, and academic performance in seventh-grade elementary school students. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14:176.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittberg RA, Northrup KL, Cottrell LA. Children’s Aerobic Fitness and Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Examination of Students During Their Fifth and Seventh Grade Years. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:2303–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wittberg RA, Northrup KL, Cottrell LA. Children’s Aerobic Fitness and Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Examination of Students During Their Fifth and Seventh Grade Years. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:2303–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Dwyer T, Sallis JF, Blizzard L, Lazarus R, Dean K. Relation of academic performance to physical activity and fitness in children. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2001;13:225–37.CrossRef Dwyer T, Sallis JF, Blizzard L, Lazarus R, Dean K. Relation of academic performance to physical activity and fitness in children. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2001;13:225–37.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Donnelly JE, Lambourne K. Classroom-based physical activity, cognition, and academic achievement. Prev Med. 2011;52:S36–42.CrossRefPubMed Donnelly JE, Lambourne K. Classroom-based physical activity, cognition, and academic achievement. Prev Med. 2011;52:S36–42.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining the Impact of Integrating Physical Activity on Fluid Intelligence and Academic Performance in an Elementary School Setting: A Preliminary Investigation. J Phys Act Health. 2010;7:343–51.CrossRefPubMed Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining the Impact of Integrating Physical Activity on Fluid Intelligence and Academic Performance in an Elementary School Setting: A Preliminary Investigation. J Phys Act Health. 2010;7:343–51.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Doolaard S, Bosker RJ, Visscher C. Physically Active Math and Language Lessons Improve Academic Achievement: A Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137:1–9.CrossRef Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Doolaard S, Bosker RJ, Visscher C. Physically Active Math and Language Lessons Improve Academic Achievement: A Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137:1–9.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark A. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. Clark A. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Maouene J, Ionescu T. Embodiment and Development. Spec Issue Cogn, Brain, Behav interdiscip J. 2011;15:403–8. Maouene J, Ionescu T. Embodiment and Development. Spec Issue Cogn, Brain, Behav interdiscip J. 2011;15:403–8.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Hutto DD, Kirchhoff MD, Abrahamson D. The Enactive Roots of STEM: Rethinking Educational Design in Mathematics. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:371–89.CrossRef Hutto DD, Kirchhoff MD, Abrahamson D. The Enactive Roots of STEM: Rethinking Educational Design in Mathematics. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:371–89.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Gallagher S, Lindgren R. Enactive Metaphors: Learning Through Full-Body Engagement. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:391–404.CrossRef Gallagher S, Lindgren R. Enactive Metaphors: Learning Through Full-Body Engagement. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:391–404.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Crollen V, Dormal G, Seron X, Lepore F, Collignon O. Embodied numbers: The role of vision in the development of number-space interactions. Cortex. 2013;49:276–83.CrossRefPubMed Crollen V, Dormal G, Seron X, Lepore F, Collignon O. Embodied numbers: The role of vision in the development of number-space interactions. Cortex. 2013;49:276–83.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Pouw WTJL, van Gog T, Paas F. An Embedded and Embodied Cognition Review of Instructional Manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review. 2014;26:51–72.CrossRef Pouw WTJL, van Gog T, Paas F. An Embedded and Embodied Cognition Review of Instructional Manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review. 2014;26:51–72.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Glenberg AM. Few Believe the World Is Flat: How Embodiment Is Changing the Scientific Understanding of Cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale. 2015;69:165–71.CrossRefPubMed Glenberg AM. Few Believe the World Is Flat: How Embodiment Is Changing the Scientific Understanding of Cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale. 2015;69:165–71.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiefer M, Trumpp NM. Embodiment theory and education: The foundations of cognition in perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience and Education. 2012;1:15–20.CrossRef Kiefer M, Trumpp NM. Embodiment theory and education: The foundations of cognition in perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience and Education. 2012;1:15–20.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Wedderkopp N, Jespersen E, Franz C, Klakk H, Heidemann M, Christiansen C, Moller NC, Leboeuf-Yde C. Study protocol. The Childhood Health, Activity, and Motor Performance School Study Denmark (The CHAMPS-study DK). BMC Pediatr. 2012;12:128.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wedderkopp N, Jespersen E, Franz C, Klakk H, Heidemann M, Christiansen C, Moller NC, Leboeuf-Yde C. Study protocol. The Childhood Health, Activity, and Motor Performance School Study Denmark (The CHAMPS-study DK). BMC Pediatr. 2012;12:128.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspersen CJ, Powell KE, Christenson GM. Physical-Activity, Exercise, and Physical-Fitness - Definitions and Distinctions for Health-related Research. Public Health Rep. 1985;100:126–31.PubMedPubMedCentral Caspersen CJ, Powell KE, Christenson GM. Physical-Activity, Exercise, and Physical-Fitness - Definitions and Distinctions for Health-related Research. Public Health Rep. 1985;100:126–31.PubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Webster CA, Russ L, Vazou S, Goh TL, Erwin H. Integrating movement in academic classrooms: understanding, applying and advancing the knowledge base. Obes Rev. 2015;16:691–701.CrossRefPubMed Webster CA, Russ L, Vazou S, Goh TL, Erwin H. Integrating movement in academic classrooms: understanding, applying and advancing the knowledge base. Obes Rev. 2015;16:691–701.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Dukette D, Cornish D. The Essential 20: Twenty Components of an Excellent Health Care Team. Pittsburgh: RoseDog Books; 2009. Dukette D, Cornish D. The Essential 20: Twenty Components of an Excellent Health Care Team. Pittsburgh: RoseDog Books; 2009.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Toumpaniari K, Loyens S, Mavilidi MF, Paas F. Preschool Children’s Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning by Embodying Words Through Physical Activity and Gesturing. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:445–56.CrossRef Toumpaniari K, Loyens S, Mavilidi MF, Paas F. Preschool Children’s Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning by Embodying Words Through Physical Activity and Gesturing. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:445–56.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandler P, Tricot A. Mind Your Body: the Essential Role of Body Movements in Children’s Learning. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:365–70.CrossRef Chandler P, Tricot A. Mind Your Body: the Essential Role of Body Movements in Children’s Learning. Educational Psychology Review. 2015;27:365–70.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley A. Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1–29.CrossRefPubMed Baddeley A. Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1–29.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Paas F, Sweller J. An Evolutionary Upgrade of Cognitive Load Theory: Using the Human Motor System and Collaboration to Support the Learning of Complex Cognitive Tasks. Educational Psychology Review. 2012;24:27–45.CrossRef Paas F, Sweller J. An Evolutionary Upgrade of Cognitive Load Theory: Using the Human Motor System and Collaboration to Support the Learning of Complex Cognitive Tasks. Educational Psychology Review. 2012;24:27–45.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Dewey J. Democracy and Education. An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: Free Press; 1966. Dewey J. Democracy and Education. An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: Free Press; 1966.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Kolb D. Experiential learning: experience as the source of learning and development. Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey; 1984. Kolb D. Experiential learning: experience as the source of learning and development. Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey; 1984.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Kolb A, A KD. Experiential Learning Theory Bibliography 1971–2001. Boston: McBer and Co; 2001. Kolb A, A KD. Experiential Learning Theory Bibliography 1971–2001. Boston: McBer and Co; 2001.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Saritas T, Akdemir O. Identifying Factors Affecting the Mathematics Achievement of Students for Better Instructional Design. International Journal of Instructional Technology and Distance Learning. 2009;6:26. Saritas T, Akdemir O. Identifying Factors Affecting the Mathematics Achievement of Students for Better Instructional Design. International Journal of Instructional Technology and Distance Learning. 2009;6:26.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Hovgaard M. Aktivitetshjulet: en model til beskrivelse, analyse og udvikling af aktiviteter. Focus - Tidsskrift for Idræt. 2009;33:22–7. Hovgaard M. Aktivitetshjulet: en model til beskrivelse, analyse og udvikling af aktiviteter. Focus - Tidsskrift for Idræt. 2009;33:22–7.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Hansen KF. MG 1: matematik grundlæggende : vejledning. Copenhagen: Hogrefe Psykologisk Forlag; 2011. Hansen KF. MG 1: matematik grundlæggende : vejledning. Copenhagen: Hogrefe Psykologisk Forlag; 2011.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Schonert-Reichl KA, Oberle E, Lawlor MS, Abbott D, Thomson K, Oberlander TF, Diamond A. Enhancing Cognitive and Social-Emotional Development Through a Simple-to-Administer Mindfulness-Based School Program for Elementary School Children: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Dev Psychol. 2015;51:52–66.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schonert-Reichl KA, Oberle E, Lawlor MS, Abbott D, Thomson K, Oberlander TF, Diamond A. Enhancing Cognitive and Social-Emotional Development Through a Simple-to-Administer Mindfulness-Based School Program for Elementary School Children: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Dev Psychol. 2015;51:52–66.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman CH, Snook EM, Jerome GJ. Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003;48:307–14.CrossRefPubMed Hillman CH, Snook EM, Jerome GJ. Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003;48:307–14.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C. The Influence of Exercise on Cognitive Abilities. Comprehensive Physiology. 2013;3:403–28.PubMedPubMedCentral Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C. The Influence of Exercise on Cognitive Abilities. Comprehensive Physiology. 2013;3:403–28.PubMedPubMedCentral
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6:235–8.CrossRef Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6:235–8.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis GA. Identifying creative students and measuring creativity. In: Colangelo N, Davis GA, editors. Handbook of gifted education. 2nd ed. Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon; 1997. p. 269–81. Davis GA. Identifying creative students and measuring creativity. In: Colangelo N, Davis GA, editors. Handbook of gifted education. 2nd ed. Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon; 1997. p. 269–81.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Lissitz RW, Willhoft JL. A methodological study of the Torrance Tests of Creativity. Journal of Educational Measurement. 1985;22:1–111.CrossRef Lissitz RW, Willhoft JL. A methodological study of the Torrance Tests of Creativity. Journal of Educational Measurement. 1985;22:1–111.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Torrance EP. The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms—technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville: Scholastic Testing Service Inc.; 1998. Torrance EP. The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms—technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville: Scholastic Testing Service Inc.; 1998.
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersen LB, Andersen TE, Andersen E, Anderssen SA. An intermittent running test to estimate maximal oxygen uptake: the Andersen test. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness. 2008;48:434–7.PubMed Andersen LB, Andersen TE, Andersen E, Anderssen SA. An intermittent running test to estimate maximal oxygen uptake: the Andersen test. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness. 2008;48:434–7.PubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahler T, Bendiksen M, Krustrup P, Wedderkopp N. Aerobic Wtness testing in 6- to 9-year-old children: reliability and validity of a modified Yo-Yo IR1 test and the Andersen test. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2012;112:871–6.CrossRefPubMed Ahler T, Bendiksen M, Krustrup P, Wedderkopp N. Aerobic Wtness testing in 6- to 9-year-old children: reliability and validity of a modified Yo-Yo IR1 test and the Andersen test. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2012;112:871–6.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Evenson KR, Catellier DJ, Gill K, Ondrak KS, McMurray RG. Calibration of two objective measures of physical activity for children. J Sports Sci. 2008;26:1557–65.CrossRefPubMed Evenson KR, Catellier DJ, Gill K, Ondrak KS, McMurray RG. Calibration of two objective measures of physical activity for children. J Sports Sci. 2008;26:1557–65.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Scudder MR, Lambourne K, Drollette ES, Herrmann SD, Washburn RA, Donnelly JE, Hillman CH. Aerobic Capacity and Cognitive Control in Elementary School-Age Children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46:1025–35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Scudder MR, Lambourne K, Drollette ES, Herrmann SD, Washburn RA, Donnelly JE, Hillman CH. Aerobic Capacity and Cognitive Control in Elementary School-Age Children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46:1025–35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Rationale and design of a randomized controlled trial examining the effect of classroom-based physical activity on math achievement
verfasst von
Mona Have
Jacob Have Nielsen
Anne Kær Gejl
Martin Thomsen Ernst
Kjeld Fredens
Jan Toftegaard Støckel
Niels Wedderkopp
Sidsel Louise Domazet
Claire Gudex
Anders Grøntved
Peter Lund Kristensen
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-2971-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

BMC Public Health 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe