Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Study protocol

Rehabilitation of Executive function in Paediatric Traumatic brain injury (REPeaT): protocol for a randomized controlled trial for treating working memory and decision-making

verfasst von: Nikita Sood, Celia Godfrey, Vicki Anderson, Cathy Catroppa

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Working memory allows us to hold information in an active state for short periods of time, and is essential in facilitating goal directed cognitive functioning. Difficulties in working memory and decision-making are common post childhood Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI). Despite this, there is a paucity of research pertaining to implementation and effectiveness of interventions to reduce these common difficulties which impact significantly on one’s ability to function independently. One such intervention, Cogmed Working Memory Training Program, has shown success in improving working memory in other childhood clinical populations, but has received little evaluation in the TBI area. This study aims to evaluate whether Cogmed improves working memory and decision-making post childhood TBI and whether these benefits generalize to functional areas.

Methods

The study is a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of the Cogmed (RM version) intervention for children post-TBI. Children aged 7–15 years are initially screened for working memory impairments. Eligible participants are then randomized into either the treatment group (Cogmed) or the active-control group (Lexia Reading). Each group trains online for 50 min each day, 5 days per week, for 5 consecutive weeks. The online training is supported by online clinician meetings each week. Outcome neuropsychological and functional assessments are carried out immediately at the completion of the intervention and at 6 months follow-up.

Discussion

This study follows gold standard methodology in intervention research; uses a novel measure of decision-making; measures the effects of intervention on functional outcomes immediately and longer-term post intervention; uses online clinician support in order to allow more families easy access to the program; and promotes the use of technology to improve health services. If efficacious in improving working memory, decision-making, and functional outcomes, our team will then take a key role in implementing Cogmed into clinical care.

Trial registration

Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ACTRN12617000085​370. Trial Registration Date: 16/01/2017. Protocol Version/Date: HREC 35181G/18.08.2017. Study Status: Ongoing.
Abkürzungen
ADHD
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
AWMA-S
Automated working memory assessment-short
BRIEF-P
Behaviour rating inventory of executive function-parent
BRIEF-T
Behaviour rating inventory of executive function-teacher
CBCL
Child Behaviour Checklist
DM
Decision-making
EF
Executive function
FSIQ
Full scale intelligence quotient
HREC
Human resources ethics committee
MCRI
Murdoch Children’s Research Institute
PedsQL
Paediatric Quality of Life
RCH
Royal Children’s Hospital
RCT
Randomized controlled trial
SD
Standard deviation
SSIS-P
Social skills improvement system rating scales-parent
SSIS-T
Social skills improvement system rating scales- teacher
T0-T2
Data collection time points 0 to 2
TRF
Teacher Report Form
VPRS
Victorian paediatric rehabilitation services
WASI-II
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence- Second Edition
WISC-V
Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children- Fifth Edition
WM
Working memory
WRAT-IV
Wide Range Achievement Test: Fourth Edition

Background

Paediatric Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) is one of the leading causes of death and disability in children and adolescents worldwide. TBI is defined as “an alteration in brain function, or other evidence of brain pathology, caused by an external force to the head” [1]. Substantial literature on paediatric TBI has identified acute and long-term impairments in higher order cognitive processes often identified as impairments in executive function (EF) [24].
Definitions of EF indicate that it encompasses the highest level of human functioning such as working memory (WM), decision-making (DM), aspects of attention, cognitive control, and inhibition [5, 6]. Neural substrates linked to WM and DM include the frontal lobes, prefrontal subregions, posterior cortex, and subcortical structures [79] These brain regions mature from childhood into early adulthood with different trajectories across sex and are specifically vulnerable following TBI sustained in childhood [1012]. Findings from brain imaging studies also suggest that the integrity of these brain regions and the neural circulatory connecting them is crucial for WM [13, 14] and DM [6, 1517]. With regards to sex, previous research shows difference in performance on WM and DM tasks in girls and boys post childhood TBI [12, 18].
WM is a limited multi-component system that facilitates maintenance and manipulation of information in an active state for short period of times. It is involved in goal-directed cognitive functioning and in the acquisition of academic skills including mathematical computation, reading, and writing [1924]. Impairments in WM post TBI are common, often with implications in a multitude of cognitive processes and adaptability in daily life [25, 26].
DM is defined as the fundamental skill for identifying, processing, and selecting one course of action from multiple alternatives [27]. Thus, DM is a stepwise process as proposed by the process tracing models of DM [28]. Myriad disciplines have studied DM from different theoretical assumptions ranging from normative theories of subjective expected utility [29], to dual process theories of information processing [30, 31], and to somatic marker hypothesis [32], with less research on process tracing methods of information acquisition in DM [33]. One such method pertains to the use of information boards [3436]. More recently, an emerging field of decision neuroscience has led to an integrated study of DM by taking into account the neural substrates, cognitive processes, and the association between one’s decisions and social outcomes [4, 3739]. Findings from these studies suggest WM allows multiple units of information to be assimilated and compared in DM [4, 40]. Additionally, DM has also been associated with functional outcomes such as return to school. In TBI, recent studies have also indicated deficits in DM [4147]. While there are studies that contradict DM’s association with WM in children with or without brain injuries, findings in support of this association demonstrate a pleading case for more research in this area [4854].
It is clear that EF domains such as WM and DM provide a target for intervention, given they are frequently impaired post childhood TBI, and are associated with everyday function and social functioning. Previous studies estimate that one-third of children post TBI benefit from intervention approaches [55]. Recently, several computerized interventions or training programs have been successfully trialled for executive function rehabilitation in other clinical populations. Despite this, there is a paucity of research in the intervention area, with even less research pertaining to the effectiveness of interventions implemented following childhood TBI [56, 57]. Methodological limitations of existing research include reliance on case studies instead of RCT design; no preregistration of trials; lack of an active-control group, no blinding, small sample size, and limited use of clinically meaningful outcome measures [58].
The Cogmed Working Memory Training program (Cogmed), is a popular computerized cognitive intervention that is based on the multi-component theory of memory [59] and the theory of neuroplasticity [26]. Cogmed was first trialled in an RCT of 53 children with ADHD [60]. This trial aimed to investigate the impact of Cogmed in improving WM in this clinical population. Results immediately post-intervention and at 3 months follow-up suggested significant benefits in working memory, ADHD symptoms, and several other executive tasks thereby implying the benefits from the intervention were also transferrable. Since then Cogmed has been trialled in typical adults [61], pre-term children [62], children with ADHD, and school-aged children with low WM [63, 64]. However, evidence for benefits from Cogmed in children with TBI is scarce, where to date only seven published studies have investigated the efficiency of Cogmed post brain injury. Of these, six studies involved adults with acquired brain injuries [6570]. Additionally, limitations of these studies included small sample size, lack of an active-control group, and shorter length of follow-up. A single recent study involving children post-TBI [57] investigated the impact of Cogmed on various components of WM. Findings from this study provided preliminary evidence for the efficacy of Cogmed in improving WM, attention, and academic skills. However, findings are limited by a small sample size, lack of consideration of injury-related factors, and short length of follow-up post intervention. While research investigating the impact of Cogmed in brain injuries supports improvements in similar tasks as those in Cogmed training, other results are inconclusive. [71]. Hence, further research investigating the efficacy of Cogmed in improving performance in similar tasks as well as generalizability to other functional outcomes is required.
In summary, ours is the first study to use a novel task of DM to investigate the relationship between WM and DM, and to study the impact of Cogmed on WM, DM, and functional outcomes in paediatric TBI.

Study aims

The overall aim of the proposed study is to evaluate the effectiveness of Cogmed post childhood TBI. RCT methodology with the inclusion of an active-control group and a larger sample size will be implemented to address previous limitations in this area. In addition to the primary executive function outcomes post intervention, this trial will investigate the generalized impact (far transfer) of Cogmed in improving everyday function including social functioning. The follow-up assessments will be completed at two time points, namely, immediately post-intervention, and at 6-months follow-up. This RCT will be a novel study to identify the relationship between WM, DM, functional outcomes and the impact of Cogmed in all these areas in children post TBI.
It is hypothesized that: (a) immediately post-intervention and at 6 months post-intervention compared to the active-control group the treatment group will report improvements in WM and DM; (b) improvements in WM will be associated with an increase in DM, academic achievement (mathematics), social skills, behavioural, and quality of life outcomes; and (c) age and sex will have an impact on WM and DM of children post- TBI (see Fig. 1).

Methods/design

Approval and registration

This study has been approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne (HREC 35181), and prospectively registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12617000085370).

Design

This study follows the gold standard methodology of intervention research. Conducted and reported according to CONSORT guidelines, it is a double-blinded, active-controlled, randomized trial. Data from neuropsychological assessments of participants, and parent and teacher questionnaires will be collected at four different time-points (see Fig. 2): T0 Study eligibility is determined by screening for working memory impairments in the child at least post 6 months after TBI; T1 Following inclusion of an eligible participant, pre-intervention baseline assessment conducted and data collected; T2 Immediately post-intervention assessment; T3 6-month follow-up post intervention assessment. At all four time points, data will be collected from both the treatment group and the active-control group.

Setting

This study will be conducted at the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute (MCRI) and The Royal Children’s Hospital (RCH), Australia. Participants will be recruited from The RCH Victorian Paediatric Rehabilitation Service (VPRS) state-wide registry, audit of presentations to The RCH, or from previous research projects conducted at The RCH. The study is expected to run for a 24 month period, with participant identification and recruitment, randomisation, and intervention implementation carried out over 18 months, followed by the 6 month follow-up post-intervention for all the study participants.

Participants

Children who have had a TBI will be contacted to participate in the study via their parent/guardian. To be eligible to participate in the RCT, children will be required to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) sustained a TBI where there was head trauma associated with a) altered consciousness, as defined by Glasgow Coma Score between 3 and 15 and/or post traumatic amnesia, or b) intra-cranial traumatic abnormalities on brain scan; (2) evidence of reduced working memory or executive dysfunction at screening (see Table 1); (3) attend primary or secondary school and be between the ages of 7–15 at the time of the intervention; and (4) a minimum of 6 months post-TBI.
Table 1
Summarises the study measures and data collection time-points
Outcome
Measure
Respondent
Criteria for inclusion
Time point
T0
T1
T2
T3
Screening outcome
 Intellectual functioning
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-Second Edition (WASI-II) [76]
Child
FSIQ> 70
X
   
 Working memory
AWMA-Screener [75]
Child
Score on any one of the sub-test < 1 SD below the mean
X
   
 Executive function
Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [77]
Parent
Score on any one of the sub-test < 1 SD below the mean
X
   
Primary outcome
 Working memory
AWMA-S [75]
Child
  
X
X
X
 Decision-making
The Decision-Making Task [34]
Child
  
X
X
X
Secondary outcome
 Working memory
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition (WISC-V): Digit Span [78]
Child
  
X
X
X
 Decision-making
The Jumping to Conclusions: the Beads Task [79]
Child
  
X
X
X
 Academic achievement (Mathematics)
The Wide Range Achievement Test: Fourth Edition (WRAT-IV): The Math Computation [80]
Child
  
X
X
X
 Adaptive behaviour
Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF): Global Executive Composite [77]
Parent (BRIEF-P)
Teacher (BRIEF-T)
  
X
X
X
Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) & Teacher Report Form (TRF) [81]
Parent (CBCL)
Teacher (TRF)
  
X
X
X
 Social skills
Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales (SSIS) [82]
Parent(SSIS-P)
Teacher(SSIS-T)
  
X
X
X
 Quality of life
Paediatric Quality of Life (PedsQL) [83]
Parent
  
X
X
X
T0: Screening; T1: Pre-intervention baseline assessment; T2: Immediately post-intervention assessment; T3: 6 months follow-up post intervention assessment
Ineligibility for participation in the study will be determined by the following exclusion criteria: (1) non-fluency in English; (2) IQ below 70 at screening (3) other previously documented neurological or learning difficulties diagnosis; (4) severe sensory or physical impairment that affects their capacity to attend mainstream school and complete the training program; (5) families and/or primary caregivers who are unable to support/assist their child through to the completion of the intervention program. This will be determined through discussions with primary caregivers/parents during the recruitment phase.

Recruitment and allocation

Recruitment will begin with the identification of children who meet the inclusion criteria for a diagnosis of TBI and their current age. Parents/guardians of children will be approached through the tracing letter. After 2 weeks, the research team member will follow-up with a phone-call to provide them with more information about the study. At this time, if the parent/guardian declines their consent for participating in the study, they will be excluded.
When a parent/guardian indicates interest in their child’s participation a letter of invitation to the study will be sent, along with the detailed study information statement and consent form. After the informed written consent is obtained, the first appointment for child’s screening assessment will be arranged at the family’s convenient time. This appointment (T0) will include cognitive screening for working memory impairments and IQ, and if eligible, (T1) the pre-intervention neuropsychological baseline assessment. Eligible participants then will be randomly allocated either to the treatment group or the active-control group.
Parent/guardians will also be provided with the teacher questionnaires along with a reply-paid envelope to forward to their child’s teacher for completion. Teacher consent to participate will be implied in the completion of questionnaires.

Randomization

Following the first study appointment, eligible participants will be randomized in a ratio of 1:1 to the treatment group or the active-control group. The randomization list will be generated using online software (sealed envelope™) by an independent statistician. Block randomization will be used to generate treatment allocations. At the time of randomization, participants will be allocated the next available sequential study number in an opaque sealed envelope. Allocation of participants to the relevant study group is managed by the chief investigator and will remain blind to the participant, their family and all other members of the trial team. The participants will be also blinded as to which program they have been assigned. However once the program is commenced treatment allocation may become clear to participating families.

Intervention delivery

Participants in the treatment group will be administered Cogmed ™ (RM version) program. Developed for children aged 7 years and above, it is a commercially available [72] online adaptive working memory training program. For the purpose of this study, participants will train on the standard protocol consisting of a total 25 training blocks to be completed over a span of 5 weeks. Participants will be required to complete one training block in 50 min every day for 5 days a week for 5 weeks. Each training block includes a series of interactive and adaptive exercises that not only targets but also dynamically adapt to the visuo-spatial and verbal WM ability of the participants at the time of the training. During the training, if the participant makes consecutive errors on four trials, the program will automatically enforce a mandatory break for 15 s. At the completion of each training block, the participant gets rewarded with a reward game, RoboRacing.
Cogmed training will be completed online by the participants from their homes. It will be supervised by a trained Cogmed Coach who is the study co-ordinator. Once the participants are randomized to the treatment group, the Cogmed Coach will conduct a Start-up session with the participants and their Training Aides. In this session, the Cogmed Coach will (a) provide an information sheet to set up the program on the home computer or iPad, (b) explain the training structure, (c) plan the training times (d) discuss participants’ motivation and expectations, (e) discuss reward system, (f) schedule online weekly meetings, and (g) fill out the Cogmed Questionnaire. Online meetings (through Skype Business/Google Hangouts/GoTo Meetings) will be held once per week for 5 weeks of training to provide support and answer any queries.
The training performance and compliance will be monitored through a secure server by the Coach.
The program requires internet access and if necessary families will be provided with an internet-connected iPad for the duration of the program. The coach can monitor the training and compliance by logging into a secure server. Program compliance can be assessed as the number of sessions completed by each child, and time spent per session will be recorded.

Implementation of active control: Lexia Reading Core5

Participants in the active-control group will be administered commercially available Lexia Reading Core5 [73, 74]. An adaptive computerized reading program, Lexia battery caters for all age groups of children but has no memory training component. Consisting of a total 18 level and 89 activities, it delivers structured, in-depth, and individualized training in six areas of reading – Phonological Awareness, Structural Analysis, Vocabulary, Phonics, Fluency, and Comprehension. Each participant will first complete 20 min of auto-placement on their initial log-in to Lexia Reading Core5. Auto-placement is a tool that assesses participant’s current level of functioning. Based on this performance, participants will be assigned an appropriate program level for their training. Once participants successfully complete a prescribed set of units, they will progress to higher levels of the program. In addition to the online training, Lexia also provides supplementing individualized paper-based practice materials for participants struggling on a particular activity. On successful completion of a level, the participant is rewarded with a certificate. Participants will train on Lexia Reading Core5 for 50 min each day for 5 days a week for 5 weeks at their homes.
The Lexia program provides an active- control for the experience of sitting in front of a computer and engaging with fun learning tasks randomized to the participants in the active-control group. As with Cogmed, the coach will provide assistance with the set-up and weekly online meetings.

Technology

Since this RCT uses online training programs and online clinician support, internet enabled iPads will be provided to the participants with no access to a computer. Technical issues faced by the participant will be addressed by the project co-ordinator in the online weekly meetings.

Facilitator training and program fidelity

The Chief Investigator and the study co-ordinator were trained in administration and use of Cogmed and Lexia. Program fidelity will be monitored through the Coach’s weekly online meetings with families.

Study measures

The participants will be screened for eligibility in a neuropsychological assessment (approximately 1–2 h). If the child is found eligible for the intervention study, questionnaires covering background and demographic information as well as behavioural function, will be completed by parents/guardians (approx. 1.5–2 h) and the teacher. The study measures are all widely-used, standardised tests and questionnaires appropriate for the target age group, and have established reliability and validity. Details pertaining to the participant’s injury (e.g., date of injury, age at injury, diagnosis, Glasgow Coma Score, neurological signs) are extracted from the hospital database. The parents/guardian provides the demographic information.
WM and DM of the participants are the two primary outcomes for this study. Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA) [75] is a validated computerized measure of WM skills, is used for assessing WM. AWMA- Short (AWMA-S) comprises of four sub-tests, namely, Listening Recall (verbal WM), Spatial Recall (visuo-spatial WM), Digit Recall (verbal short-term memory), and Dot Matrix (visuo-spatial short-term memory). In this battery, WM tasks involve both storage and processing of information whereas short-term memory tasks involve storage with minimal processing.
DM is measured using an experimental digital task that has been conceptualised on the basis of a previous research study [34]. Initially, the child is presented with everyday scenarios for example, purchasing a book. They are presented with different aspects related to that scenario, for example, cost of the book. Then they are asked to identify from choices presented on an information board what they feel is most important in making that decision. In order to reach the final decision in a situation, information acquisition is a key process in DM. Therefore, by understanding how children process information in this task, this study purports to further the knowledge of underlying cognitive processes involved in DM.
Secondary outcomes include functional measures of academic achievement, social, behavioural, and quality of life outcomes. Parent and teacher report measures will be administered to assess social and behavioural status, and quality of life outcomes.

Participant flow and estimated sample size

This study will recruit 74 eligible participants and randomize 37 participants each in the treatment group and the active-control group. This estimated sample size is in accordance with our power calculation so that a clinically significant difference can be found between the treatment group and the active-control group. Sample size calculations were based on the ability to detect a statistically significant difference of 0.8 SD between the treatment group and an active control group based on Automated Working Memory Assessment, with a significance level of 0.05 and power of 0.80 (Fig. 3).

Data storage

Data will be collected in both pen-paper and digital format and securely stored at MCRI. Only this study’s chief investigators will have access to the collected data, both throughout and after the conclusion of the study.

Data analysis

This RCT will follow the intention-to-treat paradigm for all the study participants. In order to compare pre-, immediately post-, and 6 months follow-up post intervention performance on the outcome measures all questionnaire and assessment data will be analysed using the SPSS General Linear Model procedure. Primary analysis will be done using simple t-statistics or chi-square statistics to evaluate differences between groups. Secondary analysis will involve explore the effect of moderators on working memory and decision-making. .

Trial management

This RCT will be supervised by chief investigators CC and CG who will meet fortnightly to discuss the study status. They will also manage unexpected occurrences relating to the protocol or any unforseen adverse events. The RCH Ethics Committee and the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry will be notified if necessary. No harm, potential or actual, is anticipated as a consequence of participation in this study, but in case of severe distress to any study participant, appropriate clinical referrals will be made.

Strengths and limitations

This study has notable strengths. The trial will follow the gold-standard methodology in intervention research, i.e., double-blind, randomized-controlled, pre-registered with clinical trials registry, inclusion of active-control group, large sample size, and long duration of follow-up. There is limited research on DM, the relationship between WM and DM, and the efficacy of Cogmed in a paediatric TBI population. This study will address these gaps in the literature. DM deficits are typically assessed using tasks that are limited in their ability to replicate everyday functioning. A novel measure of DM has been developed in order to measure naturalistic decision-making processes in children. To our knowledge, this is the first trial investigating the impact of Cogmed on WM, DM, and functional outcomes in TBI population. The use of online training programs and online clinician support will ensure fewer burdens on families, higher participation, and adherence to training.
Predicted challenges of this study include recruitment of such a large sample size with respect to a screening component, ensuring participant adherence to training regardless of randomisation, and participant follow-up. Another limitation pertains to the blinding of participating families. While the research personnel responsible for screening participants and administering outcome assessments are completely blinded, it is not possible to completely blind the parents to the training programs.

Discussion

Impairments in WM and DM commonly occur following childhood TBI. Currently there is minimal literature and a poor evidence base regarding intervention in this area. It is essential that effective treatment options are made available. The expected outcome of this proposed study is that the Cogmed program will be shown to be effective in improving WM and DM, and will also lead to generalised improvements in functional skills in children post-TBI. Our team will play a key role in translating Cogmed into standard clinical care, schools, and community settings. However, if findings are not significant, it will still be significant in knowing that Cogmed may not be an effective intervention for this population, suggesting it may need to be adapted for implementation in this clinical group.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Dr. Joshua Klayman, Lexia Reading and IT Education Company Australia, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Royal Children’s Hospital, University of Melbourne, the Victorian Government Operational Infrastructure Scheme, and all the research participants along with their families and teachers for their valuable time and contributions to this research project.

Funding

This trial received a grant from the Collier Charitable Fund. However the granting body did not have any role in this trial’s design and conduct at any stage.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study will be made available in the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute repository.
Ethics approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Royal Children’s Hospital (RCH), Melbourne (HREC 35181). Ethics approved study protocol will be used for all study related procedures. For all children and adolescents, study participation will involve mandatory written informed parental consent. In addition to this, all children aged 12 years or older will provide their own signed written consent. Ethics approved Parent Information Statement and Consent Form, and Participant Information Statement and Consent Form will be used for this purpose.
N/A. This trial will only publish unidentified data.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests. The authors will be responsible for study related manuscripts.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Menon DK, Schwab K, Wright DW, Demographics MAI. Clinical assessment working group of the international and interagency initiative toward common data elements for research on traumatic brain injury and psychological health. Position statement: definition of traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:1637–40.CrossRef Menon DK, Schwab K, Wright DW, Demographics MAI. Clinical assessment working group of the international and interagency initiative toward common data elements for research on traumatic brain injury and psychological health. Position statement: definition of traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:1637–40.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Catroppa C, Anderson VA. Prospective study of the recovery of attention from acute to 2 years following pediatric traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:84–98.CrossRef Catroppa C, Anderson VA. Prospective study of the recovery of attention from acute to 2 years following pediatric traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:84–98.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Muscara F, Catroppa C, Anderson V. Social problem-solving skills as a mediator between executive function and long-term social outcome following paediatric traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychol. 2008;2:445–61.CrossRef Muscara F, Catroppa C, Anderson V. Social problem-solving skills as a mediator between executive function and long-term social outcome following paediatric traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychol. 2008;2:445–61.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Sonuga Barke EJS, Cortese S, Fairchild G, Stringaris A. Annual research review: transdiagnostic neuroscience of child and adolescent mental disorders – differentiating decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:321–49.CrossRef Sonuga Barke EJS, Cortese S, Fairchild G, Stringaris A. Annual research review: transdiagnostic neuroscience of child and adolescent mental disorders – differentiating decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:321–49.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat McNally KA, Shear PK, Tlustos S, Amin RS, Beebe DW. Iowa gambling task performance in overweight children and adolescents at risk for obstructive sleep apnea. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:481–9.CrossRef McNally KA, Shear PK, Tlustos S, Amin RS, Beebe DW. Iowa gambling task performance in overweight children and adolescents at risk for obstructive sleep apnea. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:481–9.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Tlustos SJ, Chiu CYP, Walz NC, Holland SK, Bernard L, Wade SL. Neural correlates of interference control in adolescents with traumatic brain injury: functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the counting Stroop task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:181–9.CrossRef Tlustos SJ, Chiu CYP, Walz NC, Holland SK, Bernard L, Wade SL. Neural correlates of interference control in adolescents with traumatic brain injury: functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the counting Stroop task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:181–9.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42.CrossRef Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Jurado MB, Rosselli M. The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol R. 2007;17:213–33.CrossRef Jurado MB, Rosselli M. The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol R. 2007;17:213–33.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Levin HS, Hanten G. Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatr Neurol. 2005;33:79–93.CrossRef Levin HS, Hanten G. Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatr Neurol. 2005;33:79–93.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Lebel C, Beaulieu C. Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10937–47.CrossRef Lebel C, Beaulieu C. Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10937–47.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilde EA, Hunter JV, Newsome MR, Scheibel RS, Bigler ED, Johnson JL, et al. Frontal and temporal morphometric findings on MRI in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. 2005;22:333–44.CrossRef Wilde EA, Hunter JV, Newsome MR, Scheibel RS, Bigler ED, Johnson JL, et al. Frontal and temporal morphometric findings on MRI in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. 2005;22:333–44.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidt AT, Hanten GR, Li X, et al. Decision making after pediatric traumatic brain injury: trajectory of recovery and relationship to age and gender. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2012;30(3):225–30.CrossRef Schmidt AT, Hanten GR, Li X, et al. Decision making after pediatric traumatic brain injury: trajectory of recovery and relationship to age and gender. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2012;30(3):225–30.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Collette F, Van Der Linden M. Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehavl R. 2002;26:105–25.CrossRef Collette F, Van Der Linden M. Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehavl R. 2002;26:105–25.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat D'Esposito M. From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2007;362:761–72.CrossRef D'Esposito M. From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2007;362:761–72.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson SW, Nathan PE. Decision-making deficits linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:376–89.CrossRef Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson SW, Nathan PE. Decision-making deficits linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:376–89.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Brand M, Recknor CE, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A. Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:86–99.CrossRef Brand M, Recknor CE, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A. Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:86–99.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:401–33.CrossRef Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:401–33.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Donders J, Hoffman NM. Gender differences in learning and memory after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:491–9.CrossRef Donders J, Hoffman NM. Gender differences in learning and memory after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:491–9.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Ayr LK, Yeates KO, Enrile BG. Arithmetic skills and their cognitive correlates in children with acquired and congenital brain disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:249–62.CrossRef Ayr LK, Yeates KO, Enrile BG. Arithmetic skills and their cognitive correlates in children with acquired and congenital brain disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:249–62.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat de Jonge P, de Jong PF. Working memory, intelligence and reading ability in children. Pers Individ Differ. 1996;21:1007–20.CrossRef de Jonge P, de Jong PF. Working memory, intelligence and reading ability in children. Pers Individ Differ. 1996;21:1007–20.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanten G, Levin HS, Song JX. Working memory and metacognition in sentence comprehension by severely head-injured children: a preliminary study. Dev Neuropsychol. 1999;16:393–414.CrossRef Hanten G, Levin HS, Song JX. Working memory and metacognition in sentence comprehension by severely head-injured children: a preliminary study. Dev Neuropsychol. 1999;16:393–414.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Moran C, Gillon G. Inference comprehension of adolescents with traumatic brain injury: a working memory hypothesis. Brain Inj. 2005;19:743–51.CrossRef Moran C, Gillon G. Inference comprehension of adolescents with traumatic brain injury: a working memory hypothesis. Brain Inj. 2005;19:743–51.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Soto D, Hodsoll J, Rotshtein P, Humphreys GW. Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:342–8.CrossRef Soto D, Hodsoll J, Rotshtein P, Humphreys GW. Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:342–8.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Swanson HL. What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:986–1000.CrossRef Swanson HL. What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:986–1000.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Catroppa C, Anderson V. Intervention approaches for executive dysfunction following brain injury in childhood. In: Anderson V, Jacobs R, Anderson PJ, editors. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a lifespan perspective. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 439–69. Catroppa C, Anderson V. Intervention approaches for executive dysfunction following brain injury in childhood. In: Anderson V, Jacobs R, Anderson PJ, editors. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a lifespan perspective. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 439–69.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:317–24.CrossRef Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:317–24.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanfey AG. Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science. 2007;318:598–602.CrossRef Sanfey AG. Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science. 2007;318:598–602.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuhberger A, Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Ranyard R. Introduction: windows for understanding the mind. In: Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Kuhberger A, Ranyard R, editors. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 1–17. Kuhberger A, Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Ranyard R. Introduction: windows for understanding the mind. In: Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Kuhberger A, Ranyard R, editors. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 1–17.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Ward E. The theory of decision making. Psychol Bull. 1954;51:380–417.CrossRef Ward E. The theory of decision making. Psychol Bull. 1954;51:380–417.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Body R. Decision making and somatic markers in conversation after traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology. 2007;21:3–4 394-408.CrossRef Body R. Decision making and somatic markers in conversation after traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology. 2007;21:3–4 394-408.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Fletcher JM, Marks ADG, Hine DW. Working memory capacity and cognitive styles in decision-making. Pers Individ Differ. 2011;50:1136–41.CrossRef Fletcher JM, Marks ADG, Hine DW. Working memory capacity and cognitive styles in decision-making. Pers Individ Differ. 2011;50:1136–41.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Reimann M, Bechara A. The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research. J Econ Psychol. 2010;31:767–76.CrossRef Reimann M, Bechara A. The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research. J Econ Psychol. 2010;31:767–76.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Willemsen MC, Johnson EJ. Visiting the decision factory: observing cognition with mouselabWeb and other information acquisition methods. In: Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Kuhberger A, Ranyard R, editors. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 21–42. Willemsen MC, Johnson EJ. Visiting the decision factory: observing cognition with mouselabWeb and other information acquisition methods. In: Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Kuhberger A, Ranyard R, editors. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research: a critical review and user’s guide. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2011. p. 21–42.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Klayman J. Children’s decision strategies and their adaptation to task characteristics. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1985;35:179–201.CrossRef Klayman J. Children’s decision strategies and their adaptation to task characteristics. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1985;35:179–201.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Lindow S, Betsch T. Children’s adaptive decision making and the costs of information search. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2019;60:24–34.CrossRef Lindow S, Betsch T. Children’s adaptive decision making and the costs of information search. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2019;60:24–34.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Payne J. Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: an information search and protocol analysis. Organ Behav Hum Perfor. 1976;16:366–87.CrossRef Payne J. Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: an information search and protocol analysis. Organ Behav Hum Perfor. 1976;16:366–87.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson V, Beauchamp MH, Yeates KO, et al. Social competence at 6 months following childhood traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19:539–50.CrossRef Anderson V, Beauchamp MH, Yeates KO, et al. Social competence at 6 months following childhood traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19:539–50.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Garon N, Moore C. Complex decision-making in early childhood. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:158–70.CrossRef Garon N, Moore C. Complex decision-making in early childhood. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:158–70.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith DV, Huettel SA. Decision neuroscience: neuroeconomics. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2010;1(6):854–71.CrossRef Smith DV, Huettel SA. Decision neuroscience: neuroeconomics. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2010;1(6):854–71.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Amso D, Haas S, McShane L, Badre D. Working memory updating and the development of rule-guided behaviour. Cognition. 2014;133(1):201–10.CrossRef Amso D, Haas S, McShane L, Badre D. Working memory updating and the development of rule-guided behaviour. Cognition. 2014;133(1):201–10.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonatti E, Zamarian L, Wagner M, Benke T, Hollosi P, Strubreither W, et al. Making decisions and advising decisions in traumatic brain injury. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21:164–75.CrossRef Bonatti E, Zamarian L, Wagner M, Benke T, Hollosi P, Strubreither W, et al. Making decisions and advising decisions in traumatic brain injury. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21:164–75.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat García-Molina A, Roig-Rovira T, Enseñat-Cantallops A, Sánchez-Carrión R, Pico-Azanza N, Pena-Casanova J. Examination of decision-making processes in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neurologia. 2007;22:206–12.PubMed García-Molina A, Roig-Rovira T, Enseñat-Cantallops A, Sánchez-Carrión R, Pico-Azanza N, Pena-Casanova J. Examination of decision-making processes in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neurologia. 2007;22:206–12.PubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanten G, Scheibel SR, Li X, Oomer I, Stallings-Roberson G, Hunter JV, Levin SH. Decision making after traumatic brain injury in children: a preliminary study. Neurocase. 2006;12:247–51.CrossRef Hanten G, Scheibel SR, Li X, Oomer I, Stallings-Roberson G, Hunter JV, Levin SH. Decision making after traumatic brain injury in children: a preliminary study. Neurocase. 2006;12:247–51.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Cotrena C, Branco LD, Zimmermann N, Cardoso CO, Grassi-Oliveira R, Fonseca RP. Impaired decisionmaking after traumatic brain injury: The Iowa Gambling Task, Brain Inj. 2014; 28(8):1070–1075.CrossRef Cotrena C, Branco LD, Zimmermann N, Cardoso CO, Grassi-Oliveira R, Fonseca RP. Impaired decisionmaking after traumatic brain injury: The Iowa Gambling Task, Brain Inj. 2014; 28(8):1070–1075.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat MacPherson SE, Phillips HL, Della Sala S, Cantagallo A. Iowa gambling task impairment is not specific to ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;23:510–22.CrossRef MacPherson SE, Phillips HL, Della Sala S, Cantagallo A. Iowa gambling task impairment is not specific to ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;23:510–22.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Sigurdardottir S, Jerstad T, Andelic N, Roe C, Schanke A. Olfactory dysfunction, gambling task performance and intracranial lesions after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:504.CrossRef Sigurdardottir S, Jerstad T, Andelic N, Roe C, Schanke A. Olfactory dysfunction, gambling task performance and intracranial lesions after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:504.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiederkehr S, Barat M, Dehail P, de Sèze M, Lozes-Boudillon S, Giroire JM. Decision making and executive function in severe traumatic brain injured patients: validation of a decision-making task and correlated features. Rev Neurol. 2005;161:201–10.CrossRef Wiederkehr S, Barat M, Dehail P, de Sèze M, Lozes-Boudillon S, Giroire JM. Decision making and executive function in severe traumatic brain injured patients: validation of a decision-making task and correlated features. Rev Neurol. 2005;161:201–10.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Audusseasu J, Juhel J. Working memory in children predicts performance on a gambling task. J Genet Psychol. 2015;176:38–54.CrossRef Audusseasu J, Juhel J. Working memory in children predicts performance on a gambling task. J Genet Psychol. 2015;176:38–54.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Bechara A, Anderson SW. Dissociation of working memory from decision-making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998;18:428–37.CrossRef Bechara A, Anderson SW. Dissociation of working memory from decision-making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998;18:428–37.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP. Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci. 1999;19:5473–81.CrossRef Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP. Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci. 1999;19:5473–81.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Del Missier FT, Mäntylä T, Bruine de Bruin W. Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. J Behav Decis Mak. 2012;25:331–51.CrossRef Del Missier FT, Mäntylä T, Bruine de Bruin W. Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. J Behav Decis Mak. 2012;25:331–51.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Dretsch MN, Tipples J. Working memory involved in predicting future outcomes based on past experiences. Brain Cogn. 2008;66:83–90.CrossRef Dretsch MN, Tipples J. Working memory involved in predicting future outcomes based on past experiences. Brain Cogn. 2008;66:83–90.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, Aitken M, Robbins T. Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain. 2002;125:624–39.CrossRef Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, Aitken M, Robbins T. Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain. 2002;125:624–39.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Pecchinenda A, Dretsch M, Chapman P. Working memory involvement in emotion-based processes underlying choosing advantageously. Exp Psychol. 2006;53:191–7.CrossRef Pecchinenda A, Dretsch M, Chapman P. Working memory involvement in emotion-based processes underlying choosing advantageously. Exp Psychol. 2006;53:191–7.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Michaud LJ, Rivara FP, Grady MS, Reay DT. Predictors of survival and severe disability after severe brain injury in children. Neurosurgery. 1992;31:254–64.CrossRef Michaud LJ, Rivara FP, Grady MS, Reay DT. Predictors of survival and severe disability after severe brain injury in children. Neurosurgery. 1992;31:254–64.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Laatsch L, Harrington D, Hotz G, Marcantuono J, Mozzoni MP, Walsh V, Pike HK. An evidence-based review of cognitive and behavioral rehabilitation treatment studies in children with acquired brain injury. J H Trauma Rehabil. 2007;22:248–56.CrossRef Laatsch L, Harrington D, Hotz G, Marcantuono J, Mozzoni MP, Walsh V, Pike HK. An evidence-based review of cognitive and behavioral rehabilitation treatment studies in children with acquired brain injury. J H Trauma Rehabil. 2007;22:248–56.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips NL, Mandalis A, Benson S, Parry L, Epps A, Morrow A, Lah S. Computerized working memory training for children with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Neurotrauma. 2016;33:2097–104.CrossRef Phillips NL, Mandalis A, Benson S, Parry L, Epps A, Morrow A, Lah S. Computerized working memory training for children with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Neurotrauma. 2016;33:2097–104.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Simons DJ, Boot WR, Charness N, Gathercole SE, Chabris CF, Hambrick DZ, Stine-Morrow EAL. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2016;17:103–86.CrossRef Simons DJ, Boot WR, Charness N, Gathercole SE, Chabris CF, Hambrick DZ, Stine-Morrow EAL. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2016;17:103–86.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working memory. In: Bower GA, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press; 1974. p. 47–90. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working memory. In: Bower GA, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press; 1974. p. 47–90.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD - a randomized controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–86.CrossRef Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD - a randomized controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–86.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Bäckman L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:1–7.CrossRef Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Bäckman L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:1–7.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Pascoe L, Roberts G, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, Thompson DK, Seal ML, Josev EK, Nosarti C, Gathercole S, Anderson P. Preventing academic difficulties in preterm children: a randomised controlled trial of an adaptive working memory training intervention – IMPRINT study. BMC Pediatr. 2013;13:144.CrossRef Pascoe L, Roberts G, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, Thompson DK, Seal ML, Josev EK, Nosarti C, Gathercole S, Anderson P. Preventing academic difficulties in preterm children: a randomised controlled trial of an adaptive working memory training intervention – IMPRINT study. BMC Pediatr. 2013;13:144.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci. 2009;12:9–15.CrossRef Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci. 2009;12:9–15.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts G, Quach J, Spencer-Smith M, Anderosn PJ, Gathercole S, Gold L, et al. Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(5):e154568.CrossRef Roberts G, Quach J, Spencer-Smith M, Anderosn PJ, Gathercole S, Gold L, et al. Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(5):e154568.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Akerlund E, Esbjornsson E, Sunnerhagen KS, Bjorkdahl A. Can computerized working memory training improve impaired working memory, cognition and psychological health? Brain Inj. 2013;27:1649–57.CrossRef Akerlund E, Esbjornsson E, Sunnerhagen KS, Bjorkdahl A. Can computerized working memory training improve impaired working memory, cognition and psychological health? Brain Inj. 2013;27:1649–57.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Bjorkdahl A, Akerlund E, Svensson S, Esbjornsson E. A randomized study of computerized working memory training effects on functioning in everyday life for patients with brain injury. Brain Inj. 2013;27:1658–65.CrossRef Bjorkdahl A, Akerlund E, Svensson S, Esbjornsson E. A randomized study of computerized working memory training effects on functioning in everyday life for patients with brain injury. Brain Inj. 2013;27:1658–65.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Hellgren L, Samuelsson K, Lundqvist A, Borsbo B. Computerized training of working memory for patients with acquired brain injury. Open J Ther Rehabil. 2015;3:46–55.CrossRef Hellgren L, Samuelsson K, Lundqvist A, Borsbo B. Computerized training of working memory for patients with acquired brain injury. Open J Ther Rehabil. 2015;3:46–55.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Johansson B, Tornmalm M. Working memory training for patients with acquired brain injury: effects in daily life. Scand J Occup Ther. 2012;19:176–83.CrossRef Johansson B, Tornmalm M. Working memory training for patients with acquired brain injury: effects in daily life. Scand J Occup Ther. 2012;19:176–83.CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Lundqvist A, Grundstrom K, Samuelsson K, Ronnberg J. Computerized training of working memory in a group of patients suffering from acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 2010;24:1173–83.CrossRef Lundqvist A, Grundstrom K, Samuelsson K, Ronnberg J. Computerized training of working memory in a group of patients suffering from acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 2010;24:1173–83.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, Clevberger P, Ostensson ML, Bartfai A, Klingberg T. Computerized working memory after stroke: a pilot study. Brain Inj. 2007;21:21–9.CrossRef Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, Clevberger P, Ostensson ML, Bartfai A, Klingberg T. Computerized working memory after stroke: a pilot study. Brain Inj. 2007;21:21–9.CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Rossignoli-Palomeque T, Perez-Hernandez E, Gonzalez-Marques J. Brain training in children and adolescents: is it scientifically valid? Front Psychol. 2018;9:565.CrossRef Rossignoli-Palomeque T, Perez-Hernandez E, Gonzalez-Marques J. Brain training in children and adolescents: is it scientifically valid? Front Psychol. 2018;9:565.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Alloway TP. Automated working memory assessment manual. London: Harcourt; 2007. Alloway TP. Automated working memory assessment manual. London: Harcourt; 2007.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. 2nd ed. Bloomington: Pearson; 2011. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. 2nd ed. Bloomington: Pearson; 2011.
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fifth edition (WISC-V). Bloomington: Pearson; 2014. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fifth edition (WISC-V). Bloomington: Pearson; 2014.
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Batty RA, Francis A, Thomas N, Hopwood M, Ponsford J, Rossell SL. Who “jumps to conclusions”? A comprehensive assessment of probabilistic reasoning in psychosis following traumatic brain injury (PFTBI), and comparison with TBI, schizophrenia, and nonclinical controls. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2016;21:32–44.CrossRef Batty RA, Francis A, Thomas N, Hopwood M, Ponsford J, Rossell SL. Who “jumps to conclusions”? A comprehensive assessment of probabilistic reasoning in psychosis following traumatic brain injury (PFTBI), and comparison with TBI, schizophrenia, and nonclinical controls. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2016;21:32–44.CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ. Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT-4). Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 2006. Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ. Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT-4). Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 2006.
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: an integrated system of mult-informant assessment. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families; 2001. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: an integrated system of mult-informant assessment. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families; 2001.
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills improvement system: rating scale. Bloomington: Pearson Assessments; 2008. Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills improvement system: rating scale. Bloomington: Pearson Assessments; 2008.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Varni JW, et al. The pediatric quality of life inventory-32 (PCQL-32). Cancer. 1998;82:1184–96.CrossRef Varni JW, et al. The pediatric quality of life inventory-32 (PCQL-32). Cancer. 1998;82:1184–96.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Rehabilitation of Executive function in Paediatric Traumatic brain injury (REPeaT): protocol for a randomized controlled trial for treating working memory and decision-making
verfasst von
Nikita Sood
Celia Godfrey
Vicki Anderson
Cathy Catroppa
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1338-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

BMC Pediatrics 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.