Skip to main content
Erschienen in:

01.05.2019 | Research Article

Response suppression produces a switch-cost for spatially compatible saccades

verfasst von: Benjamin Tari, Mohammed A. Fadel, Matthew Heath

Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research | Ausgabe 5/2019

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Executive function supports the rapid alternation between tasks for online reconfiguration of attentional and motor goals. The oculomotor literature has found that a prosaccade (i.e., saccade to veridical target location) preceded by an antisaccade (i.e., saccade mirror symmetrical to a target) elicits an increase in reaction time (RT), whereas the converse switch does not. This switch-cost has been attributed to the antisaccade task’s requirement of inhibiting a prosaccade (i.e., response suppression) and transforming a target’s coordinate (i.e., vector inversion)—executive processes thought to contribute to a task-set inertia that proactively interferes with the planning of a subsequent prosaccade. It is, however, unclear whether response suppression and vector inversion contribute to a task-set inertia or whether the phenomenon relates to a unitary component (e.g., response suppression). Here, the same stimulus-driven (SD) prosaccades (i.e., respond at target onset) as used in previous work were used with minimally delayed (MD) prosaccades (i.e., respond at target offset) and arranged in an AABB paradigm (i.e., A = SD prosaccade, B = MD prosaccade). MD prosaccades provide the same response suppression as antisaccades without the need for vector inversion. RTs for SD task-switch trials were longer and more variable than their task-repeat counterparts, whereas values for MD task-switch and task-repeat trials did not reliably differ. Moreover, SD task-repeat and task-switch movement times and amplitudes did not vary and thus demonstrate that a switch-cost is unrelated to a speed accuracy trade-off. Accordingly, results suggest the executive demands of response suppression is sufficient to engender the persistent activation of a non-standard task-set that selectively delays the planning of a subsequent SD prosaccade.
Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Barton and Manoach’s group (e.g., Barton et al. 2006) have reported that an antisaccade delays the RT of any subsequent pro- or antisaccade. Notably, however, Barton and Manoach’s group employed a cued saccade paradigm wherein two targets were concurrently presented left and right of a central fixation and remained visible throughout a response. In contrast, our group as well as Chan and DeSouza (2013), have employed a classic antisaccade paradigm (Hallett 1978) wherein a single target is exogenously presented and therefore requires the sensorimotor transformation of a target’s coordinates to mirror-symmetrical space (i.e., vector inversion) (see Munoz and Everling 2004). This difference in methodology has been discussed in depth elsewhere (Weiler and Heath 2012a, b, 2014b) and we believe that it provides a parsimonious account for the difference in experimental findings.
 
2
For simplicity we henceforth use saccade – instead of prosaccade – to refer to a SR compatible response.
 
3
Correlations computed separately for SD and MD trials showed that foreperiod durations and RTs were not reliably related (ps > 0.40).
 
4
The text boxes in the main panels of Fig. 2 present skewness (g1) for each experimental condition and demonstrate a positive skew for each. We therefore employed median RT and RT IQR values in our ANOVA model. Notably, however, analysis of mean RT also revealed a task by task-transition interaction, F(1,18) = 7.76, p = 0. 012, ηp2 = 0.30. RTs for SD task-repeat and task-switch trials differed (t(18) = 3.48, p  = 0.003, d = 0.80), whereas MD task-switch and task-repeat did not (t(18) = − 0.31, p = 0 .075, d = − 0.07).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Allport DA, Styles EA, Hsieh S (1994) Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In: Umiltà C, Moscovitch M (eds) Attention and performance 15: conscious and nonconscious information processing. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 421–452 Allport DA, Styles EA, Hsieh S (1994) Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In: Umiltà C, Moscovitch M (eds) Attention and performance 15: conscious and nonconscious information processing. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 421–452
Zurück zum Zitat Barton JJ, Raoof M, Jameel O, Manoach DS (2006) Task-switching with antisaccades versus no-go trials: a comparison of inter-trial effects. Exp Brain Res 172:114–119CrossRefPubMed Barton JJ, Raoof M, Jameel O, Manoach DS (2006) Task-switching with antisaccades versus no-go trials: a comparison of inter-trial effects. Exp Brain Res 172:114–119CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Becker W (1989) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Metrics Rev Oculomot Res 3:13–67PubMed Becker W (1989) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Metrics Rev Oculomot Res 3:13–67PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vis Res 9:1247–1258CrossRefPubMed Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vis Res 9:1247–1258CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bulmer MG (1979) Principles of statistics. Dover Publications, New York Bulmer MG (1979) Principles of statistics. Dover Publications, New York
Zurück zum Zitat Chan JL, DeSouza JF (2013) The effects of attentional load on saccadic task switching. Exp Brain Res 227:301–309CrossRefPubMed Chan JL, DeSouza JF (2013) The effects of attentional load on saccadic task switching. Exp Brain Res 227:301–309CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cherkasova MV, Manoach DS, Intriligator JM, Barton JJ (2002) Antisaccades and task-switching: interactions in controlled processing. Exp Brain Res 144:528–537CrossRefPubMed Cherkasova MV, Manoach DS, Intriligator JM, Barton JJ (2002) Antisaccades and task-switching: interactions in controlled processing. Exp Brain Res 144:528–537CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cornelissen FW, Peters EM, Palmer J (2002) The eyelink toolbox: eye tracking with matlab and the psychophysics toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 34:613–617CrossRefPubMed Cornelissen FW, Peters EM, Palmer J (2002) The eyelink toolbox: eye tracking with matlab and the psychophysics toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 34:613–617CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dafoe JM, Armstrong IT, Munoz DP (2007) The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res 179:563–570CrossRefPubMed Dafoe JM, Armstrong IT, Munoz DP (2007) The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res 179:563–570CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, von Cramon DY (2005) Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Hum Brain Mapp 25:22–34CrossRefPubMed Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, von Cramon DY (2005) Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Hum Brain Mapp 25:22–34CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Edelman JA, Goldberg ME (2001) Dependence of saccade-related activity in the primate superior colliculus on visual target presence. J Neurophysiol 86:676–691CrossRefPubMed Edelman JA, Goldberg ME (2001) Dependence of saccade-related activity in the primate superior colliculus on visual target presence. J Neurophysiol 86:676–691CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Everling S, Dorris MC, Klein RM, Munoz DP (1999) Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccades. J Neurosci 19:2740–2754CrossRefPubMed Everling S, Dorris MC, Klein RM, Munoz DP (1999) Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccades. J Neurosci 19:2740–2754CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Everling S, Johnston K (2013) Control of the superior colliculus by the lateral prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 368:20130068CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Everling S, Johnston K (2013) Control of the superior colliculus by the lateral prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 368:20130068CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol Gen 47:381–391CrossRef Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol Gen 47:381–391CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gillen C, Heath M (2014) Target frequency influences antisaccade endpoint bias: evidence for perceptual averaging. Vis Res 105:151–158CrossRefPubMed Gillen C, Heath M (2014) Target frequency influences antisaccade endpoint bias: evidence for perceptual averaging. Vis Res 105:151–158CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hallett PE (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vis Res 8:1279–1296CrossRef Hallett PE (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vis Res 8:1279–1296CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Harris CM (1995) Does saccadic undershoot minimize saccadic flight-time? A Monte-Carlo study. Vis Res 35:691–701CrossRefPubMed Harris CM (1995) Does saccadic undershoot minimize saccadic flight-time? A Monte-Carlo study. Vis Res 35:691–701CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Gillen C, Samani A (2016) Alternating between pro-and antisaccades: switch-costs manifest via decoupling the spatial relations between stimulus and response. Exp Brain Res 234:853–865CrossRefPubMed Heath M, Gillen C, Samani A (2016) Alternating between pro-and antisaccades: switch-costs manifest via decoupling the spatial relations between stimulus and response. Exp Brain Res 234:853–865CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C (2007) What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception 36:1–16 Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C (2007) What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception 36:1–16
Zurück zum Zitat Lakens D, Scheel AM, Isager P (2018) Equivalence testing for psychological research: a tutorial. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci 1:259–269CrossRef Lakens D, Scheel AM, Isager P (2018) Equivalence testing for psychological research: a tutorial. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci 1:259–269CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Manoach DS, Thakkar KN, Cain MS, Polli FE, Edelman JA, Fischl B, Barton JJ (2007) Neural activity is modulated by trial history: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the effects of a previous antisaccade. J Neurosci 27:1791–1798CrossRefPubMed Manoach DS, Thakkar KN, Cain MS, Polli FE, Edelman JA, Fischl B, Barton JJ (2007) Neural activity is modulated by trial history: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the effects of a previous antisaccade. J Neurosci 27:1791–1798CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228CrossRefPubMed Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J (2007) Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 7:1–17CrossRefPubMed Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J (2007) Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 7:1–17CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Müri R, Vermersch AI (1995) Cortical control of saccades. Ann Neurol 37:557–567CrossRefPubMed Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Müri R, Vermersch AI (1995) Cortical control of saccades. Ann Neurol 37:557–567CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pouget P, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ, Boucher L, Paré M, Schall JD (2011) Neural basis of adaptive response time adjustment during saccade countermanding. J Neurosci 31:12604–12612CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pouget P, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ, Boucher L, Paré M, Schall JD (2011) Neural basis of adaptive response time adjustment during saccade countermanding. J Neurosci 31:12604–12612CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Stuphorn V, Schall JD (2006) Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nat Neurosci 9:925–931CrossRefPubMed Stuphorn V, Schall JD (2006) Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nat Neurosci 9:925–931CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Trappenberg TP, Dorris MC, Munoz DP, Klein RM (2001) A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. J Cogn Neurosci 13:256–271CrossRefPubMed Trappenberg TP, Dorris MC, Munoz DP, Klein RM (2001) A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. J Cogn Neurosci 13:256–271CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Webb B, Humphreys D, Heath M (2018) Oculomotor executive dysfunction during the early and later stages of sport-related concussion recovery. J Neurotrauma 35:1874–1881CrossRefPubMed Webb B, Humphreys D, Heath M (2018) Oculomotor executive dysfunction during the early and later stages of sport-related concussion recovery. J Neurotrauma 35:1874–1881CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler J, Heath M (2012a) The prior-antisaccade effect influences the planning and online control of prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 216:545–552CrossRefPubMed Weiler J, Heath M (2012a) The prior-antisaccade effect influences the planning and online control of prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 216:545–552CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler J, Heath M (2012b) Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades. Neurosci Lett 530:150–154CrossRefPubMed Weiler J, Heath M (2012b) Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades. Neurosci Lett 530:150–154CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler J, Heath M (2014a) Oculomotor task switching: alternating from a nonstandard to a standard response yields the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. J Neurophysiol 112:2176–2184CrossRefPubMed Weiler J, Heath M (2014a) Oculomotor task switching: alternating from a nonstandard to a standard response yields the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. J Neurophysiol 112:2176–2184CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler J, Heath M (2014b) Repetitive antisaccade execution does not increase the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. Acta Psychol (Amst) 146:67–72CrossRef Weiler J, Heath M (2014b) Repetitive antisaccade execution does not increase the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. Acta Psychol (Amst) 146:67–72CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler J, Hassall CD, Krigolson OE, Heath M (2015) The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: Electroencephalographic evidence of task-set inertia in oculomotor control. Behav Brain Res 278:323–329CrossRefPubMed Weiler J, Hassall CD, Krigolson OE, Heath M (2015) The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: Electroencephalographic evidence of task-set inertia in oculomotor control. Behav Brain Res 278:323–329CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wenban-Smith MG, Findlay JM (1991) Express saccades: is there a separate population in humans? Exp Brain Res 87:218–222CrossRefPubMed Wenban-Smith MG, Findlay JM (1991) Express saccades: is there a separate population in humans? Exp Brain Res 87:218–222CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2003) No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. J Mot Behav 35:127–133CrossRefPubMed Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2003) No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. J Mot Behav 35:127–133CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat White JM, Sparks DL, Stanford TR (1994) Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors. Vis Res 34:79–92CrossRefPubMed White JM, Sparks DL, Stanford TR (1994) Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors. Vis Res 34:79–92CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wolohan FDA, Knox PC (2014) Oculomotor inhibitory control in express saccade makers. Exp Brain Res 232:3949–3963CrossRefPubMed Wolohan FDA, Knox PC (2014) Oculomotor inhibitory control in express saccade makers. Exp Brain Res 232:3949–3963CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wurtz RH, Albano JE (1980) Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. Annu Rev Neurosci 3:189–226CrossRefPubMed Wurtz RH, Albano JE (1980) Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. Annu Rev Neurosci 3:189–226CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wylie G, Allport A (2000) Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychol Res 63:212–233CrossRefPubMed Wylie G, Allport A (2000) Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychol Res 63:212–233CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wylie GR, Sumowski JF, Murray M (2011) Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? Psychol Res 75:535–543CrossRefPubMed Wylie GR, Sumowski JF, Murray M (2011) Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? Psychol Res 75:535–543CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Zambarbieri D, Beltrami G, Versino M (1995) Saccade latency toward auditory targets depends on the relative position of the sound source with respect to the eyes. Vis Res 35:3305–3312CrossRefPubMed Zambarbieri D, Beltrami G, Versino M (1995) Saccade latency toward auditory targets depends on the relative position of the sound source with respect to the eyes. Vis Res 35:3305–3312CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Response suppression produces a switch-cost for spatially compatible saccades
verfasst von
Benjamin Tari
Mohammed A. Fadel
Matthew Heath
Publikationsdatum
01.05.2019
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Experimental Brain Research / Ausgabe 5/2019
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-019-05497-z

Kompaktes Leitlinien-Wissen Neurologie (Link öffnet in neuem Fenster)

Mit medbee Pocketcards schnell und sicher entscheiden.
Leitlinien-Wissen kostenlos und immer griffbereit auf ihrem Desktop, Handy oder Tablet.

Neu im Fachgebiet Neurologie

Erneut Hinweise für Neuroprotektion durch Gürtelroseimpfung

Ergebnisse eines kürzlich publizierten „natürlichen Experiments“ in Wales legten nahe, dass eine Herpes-Zoster-Impfung das Demenzrisiko senkt. Jetzt hat das Studienteam ähnliche Daten aus Australien publiziert, die in die gleiche Richtung zeigen. Offene Fragen bleiben allerdings so oder so.

Podcast

Leben statt zu Überleben: Post-Intensive-Care-Syndrom

Immer mehr Menschen überleben kritische Erkrankungen. Aber Beatmung, Sedierung und die Eindrücke der Intensivstation hinterlassen Spuren. Das Post-Intensive-Care-Syndrom kann die Folge sein. Es ist nicht nur eine Herausforderung für Kliniken, sondern auch Hausarztpraxen. Mit Allgemeinmediziner Prof. Dr. med. Konrad Schmidt sprechen wir in dieser Folge darüber, wie die Überlebenden wieder ins Leben finden können.

Zeitschrift für Allgemeinmedizin, DEGAM

Ehe schützt nicht vor Demenz

  • 25.04.2025
  • Demenz
  • Nachrichten

Eigentlich leben Verheiratete länger und gesünder. Eine aktuelle Untersuchung kommt jedoch zu dem überraschenden Schluss, dass sie eher an Demenz erkranken als nie Verheiratete, Geschiedene oder Verwitwete.

Lohnt sich die Karotis-Revaskularisation?

Die medikamentöse Therapie für Menschen mit Karotisstenosen hat sich in den vergangenen Dekaden verbessert. Braucht es also noch einen invasiven Eingriff zur Revaskularisation der Halsschlagader bei geringem bis moderatem Risiko für einen ipsilateralen Schlaganfall?

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.