Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neural Transmission 5/2016

Open Access 31.03.2016 | Neurology and Preclinical Neurological Studies - Original Article

Secondary task performance during challenging walking tasks and freezing episodes in Parkinson’s disease

verfasst von: Valeria Dibilio, Claudia Stummer, Linda Drenthen, Bastiaan R. Bloem, Jorik Nonnekes, Vivian Weerdesteyn

Erschienen in: Journal of Neural Transmission | Ausgabe 5/2016

Abstract

Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients likely use attentional strategies to compensate for their gait deficits, which increases the cognitive challenge of walking. The interplay between cognitive functions and gait can be investigated by evaluating the subject’s attendance to a secondary task during walking. We hypothesized that the ability to attend to a secondary task decreases during challenging walking conditions in PD, particularly during freezing of gait (FOG)-episodes. Twenty-nine PD patients and 14 age-matched controls performed a simple reaction task that involved squeezing a ball as fast as possible in response to an auditory stimulus. Participants performed this reaction task during four conditions: (1) walking at preferred speed; (2) walking with short steps at preferred speed; (3) walking with short steps, as rapidly as possible; (4) making rapid full turns. We used surface electromyography to determine reaction times, and a pressure sensor located within the ball to determine movement onset. Reaction times of PD patients were slower (on average by 42 ms) compared to controls, regardless of the walking task. In both groups, reaction times were significantly longer during the turning condition compared to all other conditions. FOG-episodes were most often seen during the turning condition. In PD patients, reaction times were significantly longer during FOG-episodes compared to trials without FOG. Our results suggest that turning requires more attentional resources than other walking tasks. The observation of delayed reaction times during FOG-episodes compared to trials without FOG suggests that freezers use additional resources to overcome their FOG-episodes.

Introduction

Gait deficits are common and debilitating signs of Parkinson’s disease (PD). Gait impairments in PD include reduced speed and stride length, and increased stride-to-stride variability (Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012; Fasano et al. 2013). In the more advanced stages, festination and freezing of gait (FOG) can emerge (Morris et al. 2001; Kelly et al. 2012). FOG is a gait disorder characterized by sudden, relatively brief episodes of inability to step, or by extremely short steps (Nutt et al. 2011; Nonnekes et al. 2015). FOG is frequently evoked by challenging walking tasks such as walking with short steps or by turning as rapidly as possible (Chee et al. 2009; Snijders et al. 2012; Spildooren et al. 2010). It has been hypothesized that PD patients increase their attention during walking to compensate for their gait deficits (Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012; Yogev et al. 2005; Rochester et al. 2014). When cognitive compensation becomes insufficient, particularly when challenging walking tasks further increase attentional and executive demands, FOG might emerge (Giladi et al. 2006; Vandenbossche et al. 2013).
The interplay between cognitive functions and gait can be investigated by evaluating a secondary task during gait, as it creates competition for attention and allocation of cognitive resources (Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012; Woollacott et al. 2002). Indeed, during turning (as an example of a complex gait task), performance on a cognitive secondary task is poorer as compared to normal walking, and most evidently so in PD patients with freezing of gait (Yogev et al. 2005). However, it is unknown whether the greater decrements in secondary task performance in PD patients are also seen during other walking tasks that frequently evoke FOG, such as walking with small rapid steps (Chee et al. 2009; Snijders et al. 2012). If so, these results would further support the hypothesis that FOG may occur as a manifestation of insufficient attentional compensation during challenging gait tasks. Furthermore, it is unknown whether the poorer secondary task performance during turning—as observed previously in the freezers—was indeed related to the greater attentional resources needed for executing this rather difficult motor task, or alternatively, resulted from the utilization of additional attentional resources to overcome FOG-episodes evoked by the turning task.
Here, we hypothesized that the ability to attend to a secondary task disproportionally decreases during challenging walking conditions in PD, and particularly during FOG-episodes. To test this idea, we evaluated manual reaction times (in response to an auditory stimulus) in PD patients and healthy controls during walking, walking with short steps, walking with short steps as rapidly as possible and full rapid turns in both directions, both during FOG-episodes (if present) and during normal task execution.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty-nine patients with PD participated. All patients were diagnosed according to the UK Brain Bank criteria (Hughes et al. 1992). Exclusion criteria were any other neurological or orthopedic disorder affecting gait, severe cognitive impairment and medication negatively affecting gait or balance. All PD patients were measured in an OFF-state, when they experienced an end-of-dose effect prior to intake of their next medication dose. In addition, 14 healthy controls of similar age were included. The study was approved by the local medical ethics committee and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and with local ethical guidelines. All subjects gave their written informed consent prior to the experiment.

Clinical assessment

PD patients were assessed clinically with the motor subsection (part III) of the MDS-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS, score/132) (Goetz et al. 2008). Patients also completed the New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire (N-FOGQ, score/33) (Nieuwboer et al. 2009). Global executive function was tested using the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB, score/18) (Dubois et al. 2000).

Experimental set-up and protocol

Participants performed a manual simple reaction time task under four conditions of increasing complexity; (1) while walking at preferred speed; (2) while walking with short steps (approximately 25 % of step length) at preferred speed; (3) while walking with short steps as rapidly as possible; (4) while making rapid axial 360° turns in both directions. The gait tasks were performed on a 4-m walkway. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced across subjects.
The simple reaction time task involved squeezing a rubber ball (6 cm in diameter) as fast as possible in response to an auditory stimulus (50 ms of white noise at 70 dB sound pressure level). The participants were instructed continue walking or turning when they heard the stimulus. PD patients held the ball in their most affected hand and controls in their dominant hand. The stimulus was generated by a custom made noise generator, and delivered through binaural earphones (Sennheiser, type HD518). The experimenter (VD) administered the stimulus via a button press on a keyboard that was concealed to the participants. The protocol included twelve repetitions of each gait task, each involving one auditory stimulus delivered at unpredictable moments. The experimenter, who was experienced in recognizing FOG-episodes, aimed to administer stimuli during both non-freezing and freezing episodes (if present). Prior to each task, subjects were allowed a few practice trials.

Data collection

Electromyographic (EMG) data were collected from the flexor digitorum muscle and extensor carpi radialis (ZeroWire, Aurion, Italy). Self-adhesive Ag–AgCl electrodes (Tyco Arbo ECG) were placed approximately 2 cm apart and longitudinally on the belly of each muscle, according to Seniam guidelines (Hermens et al. 1999). Furthermore, to assess movement onset, a wireless pressure sensor (ZeroWire, Aurion, Italy) was placed inside the ball. Both EMG and sensor signals were sampled at 2000 Hz. Each trial was videotaped for 4 s following administration of the auditory stimulus by two cameras in the frontal and sagittal plane of the walkway to verify the presence or absence of FOG.

Data analysis

Reaction time parameters

Two reaction time parameters were assessed: EMG onset latencies (from flexor digitorum and extensor carpi radialis) and pressure-sensor onset latency. First, EMG data were full-wave rectified and low-pass filtered at 30 Hz (zero-lag, second order Butterworth filter). Muscle onset latencies were determined using a semi-automatic computer algorithm that selected the first instant at which the EMG activity exceeded a threshold of 2 SD above the background activity, as calculated over a 500 ms period just prior to the auditory stimulus. Onsets were first selected by the computer algorithm, then visually approved and (when necessary) corrected. Onset latencies were determined for each trial separately. Movement onset as recorded from the pressure sensor inside the ball was determined in the same manner.

FOG-episodes

Two independent and experienced raters (VD and CS) scored the videos for the presence of FOG.

Statistical analysis

We first tested for differences in reaction times between PD patients and controls, discarding reaction times during FOG-episodes. We used a repeated measures ANOVA, with task (normal walking–walking with short steps–walking with short steps rapidly-turning) as within-subject factors and group (PD patients-controls) as between-subjects factor. In case of a significant task effect, we used post hoc paired t tests to identify differences in reaction times between tasks. Finally, for patients who showed freezing during the measurement, we compared outcome measures during FOG-episodes and during trials without FOG using a paired samples t tests.

Results

Clinical assessment

Clinical characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1. PD patients and controls did not differ with respect to age [t(41) = 1.025, p = 0.311] or gender (χ 2 = 0.1, p = 0.75).
Table 1
Participants characteristics
 
PD patients (n = 29)
Controls (n = 14)
Age (years)
65.3 (48–83)
67.7 (58–74)
Gender
22 M
10 M
Disease duration (years)
9 (2–21)
 
Hoehn and Yahr stage
 2
21
 
 2.5
4
 
 3
4
 
MDS-UPDRS III
37.5 (18–59)
 
UPDRS-PIGD items
3.8 (0–8)
 
UPDRS-bradykinesia items
17.4 (6–27)
 
N-FOGQ
9.1 (0–23)
 
FAB
16.6 (13–18)
 
Data represent mean (range) and frequency. For both MDS-UPDRS and N-FOGQ, higher scores indicate worse functioning. For FAB, lower scores indicate worse functioning
MDS- UPDRS III MDS-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III, PIGD-items postural instability/gait difficulty items (item 9–13; score/20), bradykinesia items (item 4–8 and 14; score/44), N-FOGQ New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire (score/33), FAB Frontal Assessment Battery (score/18)

Reaction times in PD patients versus control subjects

All participants were able to complete the measurement without stopping during the gait tasks. When squeezing the ball, the flexor digitorum and extensor carpi radialis muscle were near-simultaneously activated. Onset latencies of the flexor digitorum muscle were delayed by on average 42 ms in PD patients (291 ± 68 ms) compared to controls (249 ± 55 ms; group; F 1,41 = 5.154, p = 0.029, see Fig. 1a), but this delay did not differ between tasks (task × group; F 3,41 = 0.762, p = 0.517). In addition, the analysis yielded a significant main effect of task (F 3,41 = 12.068, p < 0.001). Post-hoc paired samples t tests showed that in both PD patients and controls, reaction times were on average 37 ms longer during the turning condition compared to the other tasks [t(42) < −4.085, p < 0.001].
For the turning condition, we also verified whether reaction times differed between turns towards the most and the least affected side in PD patients with marked motor asymmetry. Motor asymmetry was defined as ≥30 % difference between left and right sided items of the MDS-UPDRS motor sub-section. N = 18 PD (62.1 %) patients had marked motor asymmetry. For these patients, average RTs were 290 ± 77 ms for turns towards the most affected side versus 313 ± 85 ms for turns towards the least affected side, which difference was not significant (paired t test, p = 0.233).
The results for onset latencies in extensor carpi radialis and for movement onsets using the sensor located within the ball (Fig. 1b, c) yielded an equivalent pattern of statistical significance and are therefore not described in detail here.

Reaction times during FOG-episodes

During the measurement, FOG was observed in nine patients and we managed to record reaction times to the auditory stimulus both when freezing and non-freezing in all these patients. Eight of them froze during the turning condition (n = 37 FOG-episodes). Two patients froze when walking with short steps (n = 10 FOG-episodes), and five when walking with short steps rapidly (n = 24 FOG-episodes). FOG was not observed during the normal walking task. There was 100 % agreement between the raters on the presence or absence of FOG when scoring the videos.
For patients with FOG during the turning condition, we compared reaction times in flexor digitorum during a FOG-episode to those in turning trials without FOG. Reaction times were on average 79 ms slower during a FOG-episode (407 ± 62 ms) compared to turning trials without FOG [328 ± 65 ms; t(6) = 3.101, p = 0.02] (see Fig. 2), whereas the non-freezing reaction times in these participants were not different from PD patients that did not freeze at all during turning [310 ± 77 ms; t(27) = −0.592, p = 0.50]. Again, these results were mirrored in extensor carpi radialis and movement onset latencies, which statistics are therefore not further reported. We observed the same pattern during walking with short steps rapidly. Reaction times were on average 57 ms longer during FOG-episodes (327 ± 64.3 ms) compared to trials without FOG (270 ± 73 ms), but this analysis included only five people and the difference did not reach significance [t(4) = 1.69, p = 0.17].

Discussion

In the present study we evaluated manual reaction times in PD patients and healthy controls during walking, walking with short steps, walking with short steps as rapidly as possible and full rapid turns. We observed an overall delay in reaction times in PD patients compared to controls, regardless of the walking condition. In both groups, reaction times were significantly longer during turning compared to the other conditions; in the PD group, the delay in reaction times during turning was independent of turning direction (i.e. towards most or least affected side). In the conditions that yielded substantial numbers of FOG-episodes in our group of PD patients (walking with short steps rapidly and turning), reaction times were delayed during FOG-episodes compared to trials without FOG, albeit only significantly for the turning condition.
Previous studies indicated that walking is not merely an automatic task, but also relies on executive functions and attention (Sparrow et al. 2002; Bloem et al. 2006). Our observation that reaction times were longer during turning compared to the other tasks shows that this challenging gait task indeed required more attentional resources compared to the other walking tasks. For the other two challenging gait tasks (i.e. walking with short steps and walking with short steps rapidly), however, no reaction time delays were observed. This finding is somewhat unexpected, in light of the postulated role of attentional compensation for PD-related gait deficits. Aggravation of these gait deficits and the occurrence of FOG was suggested to be due to the greater attentional and executive demands involved in challenging walking tasks (Giladi et al. 2006; Vandenbossche et al. 2013). We indeed observed substantial numbers of FOG-episodes (n = 34) during the two tasks involving walking with short steps, which confirms their reported ability to provoke freezing. We did not find evidence, however, for these tasks to impose major additional cognitive demands compared to normal walking.
People with PD who perform a secondary task during walking often show a performance decrement in one or both of the tasks (Fuller et al. 2013). Most studies focused on motor performance, showing greater decrements in spatiotemporal gait parameters under dual task conditions in PD patients compared to healthy controls (Bloem et al. 2006; Hausdorff et al. 2003). In contrast, secondary task performance was reported in relatively few studies (Spildooren et al. 2010; Rochester et al. 2014; O’Shea et al. 2002; Kelly et al. 2014). Our finding of an overall delay in reaction times in PD patients compared to controls is in agreement with these previous studies, which demonstrated significantly worse performance on a secondary cognitive task in PD patients compared to controls during walking (Kelly et al. 2014), and even more pronounced delays in PD patients with FOG during a turning task (Spildooren et al. 2010). These results indicate that difficulties in performing a secondary task in PD patients may be due to the utilization of attentional resources to compensate for their gait impairments (Willems et al. 2007; Browner et al. 2010; Peterson et al. 2012).
The present results, however, do not support our hypothesis of a disproportionate increase of attentional strategies in PD patients during more challenging gait tasks, as the delay in reaction times during turning (not including reaction times during freezing episodes) was similar between patients and healthy controls. This finding contrasts with the results of a study that evaluated a cognitive dual task during turning, and that found no decrements in secondary cognitive task performance during full turns in both PD patients without FOG and in controls (Spildooren et al. 2010). The presently observed delayed reaction times might be related to the instruction to perform the turns as rapidly as possible, which constitutes a more challenging task compared to turning at a comfortable pace (Snijders et al. 2012). Indeed, the nature of the specific instructions given to participants has a relevant influence on the performance of both the primary task at hand, as well as the secondary task(s) (Bloem et al. 2006).
The challenging nature of the turning task was also exemplified by the more frequent occurrences of FOG compared to the other tasks, which is in line with previous reports (Snijders et al. 2012; Schaafsma et al. 2003). Interestingly, the within-subjects analysis demonstrated that reaction times were delayed during FOG-episodes compared to trials without FOG, whereas non-freezing reaction times in these participants were similar to those of the PD patients who did not freeze during turning. It suggests that the freezers did not allocate greater attentional resources to the turning task itself, but rather used additional resources to overcome the FOG-episodes evoked by turning. This finding supports the suggested utilization of a neural circuitry engaged in attention to overcome FOG-episodes (Browner et al. 2010).
Our discrete secondary cognitive task was very sensitive in detecting between-group and between-task differences in reaction times, and it had the additional advantage of flexibility in administering stimuli during both freezing and non-freezing episodes. However, it may be argued that this task does not optimally represent the typical impairments in executive functioning that have been associated with PD-related gait impairments (Smulders et al. 2013) and FOG (Amboni et al. 2008; Smulders et al. 2012). Several studies on dual task interference during walking used an auditory Stroop task (Kelly et al. 2014; Smulders et al. 2012), which may better reflect executive functioning, and can also be administered as a discrete stimulus. This task, however, requires many more trials for reliable estimates of performance because it involves congruent as well as incongruent stimulus–response sets. Moreover, reaction times can be confounded by changes in response accuracy. Also, the auditory Stroop task has much longer stimulus–response intervals (a factor 3–4 times greater than those for our ball-squeezing task) (Smulders et al. 2012). These disadvantages arguably render the auditory Stroop task less suitable for application during freezing episodes, as these are typically too brief and infrequent to allow for full evaluation of secondary task performance. Hence, we feel that our simple-reaction ball-squeezing task was appropriate for the purpose of this study. Yet, it would be of interest to determine whether a choice reaction task (e.g. squeezing with left or right hand in response to distinct auditory stimuli) may be even more sensitive in revealing subtle differences between individual tasks, as selecting the appropriate response to a specific stimulus requires more cognitive processing than the singular stimulus–response mapping in a simple reaction task.
A limitation of our study is the lack of a ‘baseline’ condition that involved responding to the auditory stimulus in a stationary position (sitting or standing). Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the PD-related delays in reaction times were due to general slowness, rather than being related to interference of gait with the cognitive task (Rochester et al. 2014; Amboni et al. 2013). Recent studies, however, found no differences between PD patients and controls in simple reaction times while subjects sat or stood in a stationary position (Rochester et al. 2014; Nonnekes et al. 2014; Fernandez-Del-Olmo et al. 2013) suggesting that the presently observed delay in reaction times was indeed caused by interference between gait and secondary tasks. Hence, our results appear consistent with the notion that PD patients have difficulties performing a secondary cognitive task while walking (Giladi and Hausdorff 2006; Willems et al. 2007).
In conclusion, our results suggest that turning requires more attentional resources compared to other walking tasks, both in PD patients and in healthy controls. The observation of delayed reaction times during FOG-episodes compared to trials without FOG suggests utilization of additional cognitive resources to overcome FOG-episodes.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a Radboud University Medical Centre Research Grant to J. H. Nonnekes, a Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research Veni Research Grant to V. Weerdesteyn (916.10.106) and by a European Community‘s Seventh Framework Programme FP7/2012 to C. Stummer under Grant Agreement No. 316639. We thank Roland Loeffen and Geert van Bon for technical assistance.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

© Springer Medizin

Bis 11. April 2024 bestellen und im ersten Jahr 50 % sparen!

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

© Springer Medizin

Bis 11. April 2024 bestellen und im ersten Jahr 50 % sparen!

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

© Springer Medizin

Bis 11. April 2024 bestellen und im ersten Jahr 50 % sparen!

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Amboni M, Cozzolino A, Longo K, Picillo M, Barone P (2008) Freezing of gait and executive functions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 23(3):395–400CrossRefPubMed Amboni M, Cozzolino A, Longo K, Picillo M, Barone P (2008) Freezing of gait and executive functions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 23(3):395–400CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bloem BR, Grimbergen YA, van Dijk JG, Munneke M (2006) The “posture second” strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 248(1–2):196–204CrossRefPubMed Bloem BR, Grimbergen YA, van Dijk JG, Munneke M (2006) The “posture second” strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 248(1–2):196–204CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Browner N, Giladi N (2010) What can we learn from freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease? Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 10(5):345–351CrossRefPubMed Browner N, Giladi N (2010) What can we learn from freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease? Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 10(5):345–351CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Chee R, Murphy A, Danoudis M, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Iansek R (2009) Gait freezing in Parkinson’s disease and the stride length sequence effect interaction. Brain 132(Pt 8):2151–2160CrossRefPubMed Chee R, Murphy A, Danoudis M, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Iansek R (2009) Gait freezing in Parkinson’s disease and the stride length sequence effect interaction. Brain 132(Pt 8):2151–2160CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B (2000) The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55(11):1621–1626CrossRefPubMed Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B (2000) The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55(11):1621–1626CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fasano A, Bloem BR (2013) Gait disorders. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 19(5 Movement Disorders):1344–1382 Fasano A, Bloem BR (2013) Gait disorders. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 19(5 Movement Disorders):1344–1382
Zurück zum Zitat Fernandez-Del-Olmo M, Bello O, Lopez-Alonso V, Marquez G, Sanchez JA, Morenilla L, Valls-Solè J (2013) The effects of startle and non-startle auditory stimuli on wrist flexion movement in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 548:56–60CrossRefPubMed Fernandez-Del-Olmo M, Bello O, Lopez-Alonso V, Marquez G, Sanchez JA, Morenilla L, Valls-Solè J (2013) The effects of startle and non-startle auditory stimuli on wrist flexion movement in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 548:56–60CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fuller RL, Van Winkle EP, Anderson KE, Gruber-Baldini AL, Hill T, Zampieri C, Weiner WJ, Shulman LM (2013) Dual task performance in Parkinson’s disease: a sensitive predictor of impairment and disability. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19(3):325–328CrossRefPubMed Fuller RL, Van Winkle EP, Anderson KE, Gruber-Baldini AL, Hill T, Zampieri C, Weiner WJ, Shulman LM (2013) Dual task performance in Parkinson’s disease: a sensitive predictor of impairment and disability. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19(3):325–328CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Giladi N, Hausdorff JM (2006) The role of mental function in the pathogenesis of freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 248(1–2):173–176CrossRefPubMed Giladi N, Hausdorff JM (2006) The role of mental function in the pathogenesis of freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 248(1–2):173–176CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, Stebbins GT, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, Poewe W, Sampaio C, Stern MB, Dodel R, Dubois B, Holloway R, Jankovic J, Kulisevsky J, Lang AE, Lees A, Leurgans S, LeWitt PA, Nyenhuis D, Olanow CW, Rascol O, Schrag A, Teresi JA, van Hilten JJ, LaPelle N (2008) Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord Off J Mov Dis Soc 23(15):2129–2170CrossRef Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, Stebbins GT, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, Poewe W, Sampaio C, Stern MB, Dodel R, Dubois B, Holloway R, Jankovic J, Kulisevsky J, Lang AE, Lees A, Leurgans S, LeWitt PA, Nyenhuis D, Olanow CW, Rascol O, Schrag A, Teresi JA, van Hilten JJ, LaPelle N (2008) Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord Off J Mov Dis Soc 23(15):2129–2170CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N (2003) Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 16(1):53–58CrossRefPubMed Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N (2003) Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 16(1):53–58CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hermens HJFB, Merletti R et al (1999) SENIAM 8: European recommendations for surface electromyography. Roessingh research and development bv, Enschede Hermens HJFB, Merletti R et al (1999) SENIAM 8: European recommendations for surface electromyography. Roessingh research and development bv, Enschede
Zurück zum Zitat Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55(3):181–184CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55(3):181–184CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly VE, Shumway-Cook A (2014) The ability of people with Parkinson’s disease to modify dual-task performance in response to instructions during simple and complex walking tasks. Exp Brain Res 232(1):263–271CrossRefPubMed Kelly VE, Shumway-Cook A (2014) The ability of people with Parkinson’s disease to modify dual-task performance in response to instructions during simple and complex walking tasks. Exp Brain Res 232(1):263–271CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly VE, Eusterbrock AJ, Shumway-Cook A (2012) A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with Parkinson’s disease: motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsons Dis 2012:918719PubMedPubMedCentral Kelly VE, Eusterbrock AJ, Shumway-Cook A (2012) A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with Parkinson’s disease: motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsons Dis 2012:918719PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Morris ME, Huxham FE, McGinley J, Iansek R (2001) Gait disorders and gait rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol 87:347–361PubMed Morris ME, Huxham FE, McGinley J, Iansek R (2001) Gait disorders and gait rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol 87:347–361PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nieuwboer A, Rochester L, Herman T, Vandenberghe W, Emil GE, Thomaes T, Giladi N (2009) Reliability of the new freezing of gait questionnaire: agreement between patients with Parkinson’s disease and their carers. Gait Posture 30(4):459–463CrossRefPubMed Nieuwboer A, Rochester L, Herman T, Vandenberghe W, Emil GE, Thomaes T, Giladi N (2009) Reliability of the new freezing of gait questionnaire: agreement between patients with Parkinson’s disease and their carers. Gait Posture 30(4):459–463CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nonnekes J, Geurts AC, Oude Nijhuis LB, vanGeel K, Snijders AH, Bloem BR, Weerdesteyn V (2014) Reduced StartReact effect and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: two of a kind? J Neurol 261(5):943–950CrossRefPubMed Nonnekes J, Geurts AC, Oude Nijhuis LB, vanGeel K, Snijders AH, Bloem BR, Weerdesteyn V (2014) Reduced StartReact effect and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: two of a kind? J Neurol 261(5):943–950CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nonnekes J, Snijders AH, Nutt JG, Deuschl G, Giladi N, Bloem BR (2015) Freezing of gait: a practical approach to management. Lancet Neurol 14(7):768–778CrossRefPubMed Nonnekes J, Snijders AH, Nutt JG, Deuschl G, Giladi N, Bloem BR (2015) Freezing of gait: a practical approach to management. Lancet Neurol 14(7):768–778CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nutt JG, Bloem BR, Giladi N, Hallett M, Horak FB, Nieuwboer A (2011) Freezing of gait: moving forward on a mysterious clinical phenomenon. Lancet Neurol 10(8):734–744CrossRefPubMed Nutt JG, Bloem BR, Giladi N, Hallett M, Horak FB, Nieuwboer A (2011) Freezing of gait: moving forward on a mysterious clinical phenomenon. Lancet Neurol 10(8):734–744CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat O’Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R (2002) Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther 82(9):888–897PubMed O’Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R (2002) Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther 82(9):888–897PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson DS, Plotnik M, Hausdorff JM, Earhart GM (2012) Evidence for a relationship between bilateral coordination during complex gait tasks and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(9):1022–1026CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Peterson DS, Plotnik M, Hausdorff JM, Earhart GM (2012) Evidence for a relationship between bilateral coordination during complex gait tasks and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(9):1022–1026CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Rochester L, Galna B, Lord S, Burn D (2014) The nature of dual-task interference during gait in incident Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 265:83–94CrossRefPubMed Rochester L, Galna B, Lord S, Burn D (2014) The nature of dual-task interference during gait in incident Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 265:83–94CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schaafsma JD, Balash Y, Gurevich T, Bartels AL, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N (2003) Characterization of freezing of gait subtypes and the response of each to levodopa in Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurol 10(4):391–398CrossRefPubMed Schaafsma JD, Balash Y, Gurevich T, Bartels AL, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N (2003) Characterization of freezing of gait subtypes and the response of each to levodopa in Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurol 10(4):391–398CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Smulders K, Esselink RA, Weiss A, Kessels RP, Geurts AC, Bloem BR (2012) Assessment of dual tasking has no clinical value for fall prediction in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol 259(9):1840–1847CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Smulders K, Esselink RA, Weiss A, Kessels RP, Geurts AC, Bloem BR (2012) Assessment of dual tasking has no clinical value for fall prediction in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol 259(9):1840–1847CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Smulders K, van Nimwegen M, Munneke M, Bloem BR, Kessels RP, Esselink RA (2013) Involvement of specific executive functions in mobility in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19(1):126–128CrossRefPubMed Smulders K, van Nimwegen M, Munneke M, Bloem BR, Kessels RP, Esselink RA (2013) Involvement of specific executive functions in mobility in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19(1):126–128CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Snijders AH, Haaxma CA, Hagen YJ, Munneke M, Bloem BR (2012) Freezer or non-freezer: clinical assessment of freezing of gait. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(2):149–154CrossRefPubMed Snijders AH, Haaxma CA, Hagen YJ, Munneke M, Bloem BR (2012) Freezer or non-freezer: clinical assessment of freezing of gait. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18(2):149–154CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sparrow WA, Bradshaw EJ, Lamoureux E, Tirosh O (2002) Ageing effects on the attention demands of walking. Hum Mov Sci 21(5–6):961–972CrossRefPubMed Sparrow WA, Bradshaw EJ, Lamoureux E, Tirosh O (2002) Ageing effects on the attention demands of walking. Hum Mov Sci 21(5–6):961–972CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E, Nieuwboer A (2010) Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord 25(15):2563–2570CrossRefPubMed Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E, Nieuwboer A (2010) Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord 25(15):2563–2570CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, Coomans D, Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Nieuwboer A, Kerckhofs E (2013) Impaired implicit sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease patients with freezing of gait. Neuropsychology 27(1):28–36CrossRefPubMed Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, Coomans D, Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Nieuwboer A, Kerckhofs E (2013) Impaired implicit sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease patients with freezing of gait. Neuropsychology 27(1):28–36CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Willems AM, Nieuwboer A, Chavret F, Desloovere K, Dom R, Rochester L, Kwakkel G, van Wegen E, Jones D (2007) Turning in Parkinson’s disease patients and controls: the effect of auditory cues. Mov Disord 22(13):1871–1878CrossRefPubMed Willems AM, Nieuwboer A, Chavret F, Desloovere K, Dom R, Rochester L, Kwakkel G, van Wegen E, Jones D (2007) Turning in Parkinson’s disease patients and controls: the effect of auditory cues. Mov Disord 22(13):1871–1878CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A (2002) Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16(1):1–14CrossRefPubMed Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A (2002) Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16(1):1–14CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM (2005) Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci 22(5):1248–1256CrossRefPubMed Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM (2005) Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci 22(5):1248–1256CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Yogev-Seligmann G, Rotem-Galili Y, Dickstein R, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM (2012) Effects of explicit prioritization on dual task walking in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 35(4):641–646CrossRefPubMed Yogev-Seligmann G, Rotem-Galili Y, Dickstein R, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM (2012) Effects of explicit prioritization on dual task walking in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 35(4):641–646CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Secondary task performance during challenging walking tasks and freezing episodes in Parkinson’s disease
verfasst von
Valeria Dibilio
Claudia Stummer
Linda Drenthen
Bastiaan R. Bloem
Jorik Nonnekes
Vivian Weerdesteyn
Publikationsdatum
31.03.2016
Verlag
Springer Vienna
Erschienen in
Journal of Neural Transmission / Ausgabe 5/2016
Print ISSN: 0300-9564
Elektronische ISSN: 1435-1463
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-016-1516-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 5/2016

Journal of Neural Transmission 5/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Neurology and Preclinical Neurological Studies - Original Article

Reconstituting botulinum toxin drugs: shaking, stirring or what?

Neurology and Preclinical Neurological Studies - Short communication

Nigral involvement in atypical parkinsonisms: evidence from a pilot study with ultra-high field MRI

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Neu im Fachgebiet Neurologie

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.