Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Research article

Temperament and character traits in female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury disorder with and without comorbid borderline personality disorder

verfasst von: Taru Tschan, Claudia Peter-Ruf, Marc Schmid, Tina In-Albon

Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

Temperament and character traits of adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury disorder (NSSI) might differentiate those- with and without comorbid borderline personality disorder (BPD).

Methods

Participants were 57 female adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD (NSSI − BPD), 14 adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD (NSSI + BPD), 32 clinical controls (CC), and 64 nonclinical controls (NC). Temperament and character traits were assessed with the Junior Temperament and Character Inventory, and impulsivity with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale and a Go/NoGo task.

Results

Adolescents with NSSI disorder scored significantly higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness than CC. The NSSI + BPD group scored even higher than the NSSI − BPD group on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence and cooperativeness (d ≥ 0.72). Adolescents with NSSI reported higher levels of impulsivity than the CC and NC group. However, this difference was not found in a Go/NoGo task.

Conclusions

The results provide further evidence for a distinct diagnostic entity of NSSI disorder.
Hinweise
Taru Tschan and Claudia Peter-Ruf contributed equally to this work
Abkürzungen
NSSI
nonsuicidal self-injury
BPD
Borderline personality disorder
NSSI − BPD
adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD
NSSI + BPD
adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD
CC
clinical controls
NC
nonclinical controls
DSM-5
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed
PTSD
posttraumatic stress disorder
Kinder-DIPS
Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents
SCID-II
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II disorders
BSL-95
Borderline Symptom List 95
JTCI
Junior Temperament and Character Inventory
BIS
Barratt Impulsiveness Scale
YSR
Youth Self Report
BDI-II
Beck Depression Inventory-II
ANOVA
analyses of variance
MANOVA
multivariate analyses of variance

Background

Due to the inclusion of nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5) [1] as a research diagnosis in section III, further studies are needed to enable a better understanding of this behavior. Independent of classification discussions, high prevalence and comorbidity rates [24], low quality of life [5], and increased risk of suicidality [6] highlight the importance of further research on NSSI. Special attention should be paid to adolescents, as NSSI often has its onset during this time [4, 7]. Previously, NSSI was generally assessed as one of the nine symptoms of Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), however only a minority of adolescents with NSSI suffer from BPD [5, 8]. Several differences in the phenomenology and functions of NSSI can be found between patients with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD) and patients with NSSI without BPD (NSSI − BPD). Patients with NSSI + BPD show more frequent and severe NSSI, greater diagnostic comorbidity, more severe depressive symptomatology, suicidal ideation, and emotion dysregulation than patients with NSSI − BPD [9, 10]. Regarding functions of NSSI, adolescents with NSSI + BPD endorsed higher self-punishment, anti-suicide, and anti-dissociation functions of NSSI than adolescents with NSSI − BPD [11].
Among different personality concepts, Cloninger´s [12, 13] biopsychosocial personality model seems to be able to describe healthy as well as pathological temperament and character traits, and to differentiate between patients with and without personality disorders [14, 15]. The extended model [13] includes four temperament dimensions (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, persistence) and three character dimensions (self-directedness, cooperativeness, self-transcendence), see Table 1. Low levels of self-directedness and cooperativeness are characteristics for personality disorders [16].
Table 1
Temperament and character dimensions
Dimension
High level
Low level
Temperament
Novelty seeking
Curious, impulsive, sensation seeking
Indifferent, thoughtful, modest
Harm avoidance
Worried, pessimistic, frightened, shy
Relaxed, optimistic, fearless, confident, talkative
Reward dependence
Sensitive, warm, dependent
Cold, secluded, independent
Persistence
Hard-working, ambitious, perfectionist
Inactive, lethargic, pragmatic
Character
Self-directedness
Mature, effective, responsible, determined, high self-acceptance
Immature, unreliable, indecisive, low self-acceptance
Cooperativeness
Social tolerant, empathic, helpful
Social intolerant, critical, cold, not helpful, destructive
Self-transcendence
Experienced, patient, creative, self-forgetting, connected to the universe, spiritual
Uncomprehending, proud, unimaginative, lack of humility
Patients with BPD often show a temperament profile consisting of both high harm avoidance and novelty seeking [14, 1618]. According to Cloninger, Praybeck, Svrakic, and Wetzel [19], a personality pattern consisting of high novelty seeking and high harm avoidance represents an approach-avoidance conflict that may cause affective instability, a core feature of BPD. Studies of adolescents with NSSI − BPD are needed to investigate the link between NSSI and the described personality pattern, especially high novelty seeking and harm avoidance. Indeed, higher levels of novelty seeking were found in adolescents with NSSI compared to adolescents without NSSI [20]. Furthermore, adolescents with depressive disorder and self-harm behavior reported more harm avoidance than those without self-harm [21].
Low self-directedness is related to self-injurious behavior in adolescents [20, 21], BPD in adolescents [18] and BPD in adults [14]. Higher levels of cooperativeness were found in female adolescents with self-harm behavior (self-injuring behavior including suicidal behavior) compared to those without self-harm behavior [22], whereas adults with BPD showed lower levels of cooperativeness than adult controls [14]. Ohmann et al. [22] offer the explanation that higher cooperativeness levels in adolescents with self-harm behavior may be related to pronounced helplessness. High self-transcendence is linked to NSSI in adolescents [20] and to BPD in adults [14]. Low reward dependence is linked to internalizing symptoms like depression and anxiety [23], but no association has been found between reward dependence and NSSI [20], nor between reward dependence and self-harm behavior [21, 22]. Kaess et al. [18] found lower reward dependence in adolescents with BPD than in clinical and healthy controls. Further, persistence is linked neither to BPD [14, 18] nor to NSSI [20] or self-harm behavior.
In summary, for BPD, most studies support the personality pattern suggested by Cloninger et al. [16, 19], consisting of high novelty seeking and harm avoidance as well as low levels of self-directedness and cooperativeness [14, 18]. Adolescents with NSSI show a similar personality pattern to adolescents with BPD, however most studies have not controlled for comorbid BPD [e.g. 20, 21]. Studies using the big five model found similar personality traits related to self-injurious behavior, namely high neuroticism (comparable to harm avoidance), low agreeableness (comparable to cooperativeness), and low conscientiousness (comparable to self-directedness and persistence) [24, 25]. One part of novelty seeking, impulsivity, might explain the difficulties self-injurers have with resisting the urge to injure themselves [26]. NSSI itself is often an impulsive act, as most of the individuals with NSSI think about the act for less than five minutes before committing it [27]. Indeed, on self-report measures individuals with NSSI indicated higher impulsivity than individuals without NSSI [26, 28, 29], and patients with repetitive self-harm reported even higher impulsivity than patients with onetime self-harm behavior [30]. However, previous research has found low convergence between self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity [for a meta-analysis see [31].
Response inhibition, one aspect of impulsivity, can be measured with a Go/NoGo task. Janis and Nock [29] compared self-reported impulsivity with experimentally assessed impulsivity in adolescents with NSSI. While participants with NSSI scored higher on self-reported impulsivity, they did not differ from the mixed clinical and nonclinical comparison groups without NSSI on behavioral measures. This result has been replicated in studies of adults with NSSI [26, 32]. The difference between self-reported and experimentally assessed impulsivity may be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs. While self-report questionnaires measure general response tendencies (traits), behavioral tasks may in fact measure spontaneous reactions that are influenced by current cognitive processes [32]. Therefore, it seems important not only to investigate impulsivity with self-report measures, but also with behavioral tasks.
In summary, previous research is consistent with the notion that certain temperament traits underlie features of BPD symptoms. However, it remains unclear, if the same pattern can be found in a sample of adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD. None of the presented studies assessed self-injuring behavior according to the DSM-5 criteria [e.g. 2022]; whereas Hefti et al. [20] investigated a school sample, Joyce et al. [21] investigated depressed adolescents with and without self-harm behavior, and Ohmann et al. [22] investigated adolescents presenting at in- and outpatient clinics. Thus, the samples were heterogeneous. To our knowledge, no study has investigated Cloninger’s temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI disorder with and without BPD. Cloninger’s personality traits might be especially suitable for the distinction between adolescents with and without BPD because of its dimensional structure. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate impulsivity (self-report and a behavioral measures), temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI disorder (according to DSM-5), and differences in personality dimensions according to Cloninger et al. [13] between adolescents with NSSI with and without comorbid BPD.
We hypothesized that there are dimensional differences in temperament and character traits between four groups of adolescents. Specifically, we addressed the following research questions.
1.
Do adolescents with NSSI disorder show a different personality pattern in comparison to the clinical control (CC) and the nonclinical control (NC) groups? Taking the results of previous studies into account, we hypothesized that adolescents with NSSI disorder would show higher values on novelty seeking, self-transcendence, and harm avoidance as well as lower values on self-directedness compared to the NC and the CC groups.
 
2.
Do adolescents with NSSI + BPD show a distinct personality pattern in comparison to adolescents with NSSI − BPD? To our knowledge, no other studies exist, and therefore this analysis was exploratory.
 
3.
Do adolescents with NSSI − BPD report more impulsivity than the NC and the CC groups? Is this difference evident in an emotional Go/NoGo task? Because of the heterogeneous results of previous studies, this analysis was also exploratory.
 

Methods

Procedure

All participants and their parents were informed about the study and gave their written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The local ethics committee approved the study. First, the clinical interviews were conducted and questionnaires distributed, and then the Go/NoGo task was administered.

Measures

Diagnostic assessments

To examine the participants’ current or past DSM-IV-TR diagnoses for Axis I disorders, we conducted two structured interviews with each adolescent. The Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents (Kinder-DIPS) [33] assesses the most frequent mental disorders in childhood and adolescence. Questions for substance use disorders were asked from the adult DIPS [34]. The Kinder-DIPS has good validity and reliability for Axis I disorders (child version, kappa = 0.48–0.88) [35]. NSSI was assessed according to the DSM-5 research criteria, with questions reformulated as criteria. Interrater reliability estimates for the diagnosis of NSSI were very good (kappa = 0.90). Before conducting the interviews, Master’s students in clinical child psychology underwent systematic training.
Participants were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II disorders (SCID-II) [36], to assess for personality disorders. The SCID-II has been found to be suitable for use among adolescents [37]. Interrater reliability for BPD in our sample was very good (kappa = 1.00).
The Borderline Symptom List 95 (BSL-95) [38] was used as an additional instrument to measure the degree of borderline symptomatology. The items are based on the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV. The self-report questionnaire shows good psychometric properties [39].
The Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI) [40] is a self-report measure assessing the seven temperament and character traits based on Cloninger’s [13] biopsychosocial model of personality. The scales have good levels of internal consistency, with Cronbach´s α ranging from 0.79 to 0.85 [40]. The internal consistencies within the present sample ranged from α = 0.76 to 0.82.
The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) [41], German version [42] is a valid and reliable self-report questionnaire to assess impulsivity with three subscales: Attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity. The internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.81.
The Youth Self Report (YSR) [43, 44] measures a broad range of psychopathology. The problem behavior section of the YSR consists of the following primary subscales: withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior. Two second-order scales reflecting internalizing and externalizing problems and a total problems score can be calculated. Internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.94 for the total score, α = 0.94 for the internalizing score, and α = 0.79 for the externalizing score.
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [45] consists of 21 items and assesses depressive symptoms. The internal consistency within the present sample was α = 0.95.

Non-emotional and emotional Go/NoGo task

Participants were instructed to press a button as fast as possible if a Go stimulus appears on the screen and to suppress reactions to NoGo stimuli. Participants had a practice run with six trials, followed by the non-emotional Go/NoGo task. Afterwards participants completed an emotional Go/NoGo task with four combinations of angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions with 12 trials for each combination. For all runs, targets occurred on 50% of the trials. The order of the four emotional runs and the trials within each run were randomized across participants.
Facial stimuli consisted of colored angry, happy, and neutral expressions from 18 individuals (9 females) taken from the NimStimFace Stimulus set [46]. Non-emotional stimuli (“+” and “×”) were presented for 200 ms and emotional stimuli for 500 ms, after a 500 ms fixation cross. The longer presentation time for emotional stimuli was due to the higher complexity of faces compared to crosses, similar to Hare et al. [47]. The inter-stimulus interval was 1.5 s, in which a reaction was still possible. Stimuli were presented with E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA), and omission (no reaction to Go) and commission (reaction to NoGo) errors as well as reaction times were recorded simultaneously. Omission errors indicate inattention [48], commission errors insufficient response inhibition [49], and reaction time to Go stimuli as a measure of response bias, with faster reactions indicating a response or attention bias toward the shown emotion [50].

Data analyses

Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were used to compare the groups (NC, CC, NSSI − BPD, NSSI + BPD) on dependent variables such as impulsivity and psychopathology. One-way between groups analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) calculated to further analyze significant group differences of MANOVAs. As we were interested in specific group differences, we set up orthogonal comparisons for psychopathology, personality, and self-reported impulsivity. The first comparison contrasted the NC group with the clinical groups (CC, NSSI, NSSI + BPD), the second contrasted the CC group with the two NSSI groups (NSSI − BPD and NSSI + BPD), and the third contrasted the two NSSI groups (NSSI − BPD and NSSI + BPD). Due to the small sample size, the analyses proceeded using bootstrapping with 2000 resamples. To correct for multiple testing, p values were adjusted according to the Bonferroni-Holm procedure. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 24.
For the Go/NoGo task, a similar analytic strategy was used. First, outliers (z-values > 3) were excluded, then the sensitivity index d’ (z(Reaction rate to Go) – z(Reaction rate to NoGo) was calculated, as a measure of discrimination, with lower values representing an inability to distinguish between stimuli and lower performance levels [52]. To examine group differences, the non-emotional Go/NoGo task was evaluated with a one-way ANOVA, and the emotional Go/NoGo tasks were analyzed separately for emotional Go (neutral NoGo) and for neutral Go (emotional NoGo) with MANOVAs. These analyses were calculated for the sensitivity index d’, errors of commission and omission, as well as for the reaction time on Go trials. If the Levene test indicated that the variance homogeneity of an outcome was violated, we transformed it for the analysis (log10 or sqrt) and if indicated, Greenhouse Geisser corrected values were used. Significance levels were set at α = 0.05.

Results

Participants

Participants were 167 female adolescents, aged 12–19 years (M = 15.94, SD = 1.47), recruited from different inpatient child and adolescent psychiatric units in Switzerland and Germany. Participants included 57 adolescents fulfilling the DSM-5 research criteria for NSSI disorder (NSSI) but not for BPD, 14 adolescents with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD), 32 adolescents with a DSM-IV [51] diagnosis other than current or past NSSI (clinical controls, CC), and 64 nonclinical adolescents who had no current or past experience of mental disorders (nonclinical controls, NC). Participants were similar with respect to age, Welch’s F (3, 47.19) = 0.41. Regarding nationalities, most of the participants were Swiss and German, except for two Italians, one Thai and one Pole. The three most frequent mental disorders in all groups were: major depression (37.50% in CC, 70.18% in NSSI, 78.6% in NSSI + BPD), social phobia (34.38% in CC, 36.84% in NSSI, 42.9% in NSSI + BPD), and specific phobia (28.13% in CC, 19.30% in NSSI, 35.70% in NSSI + BPD). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was a common comorbid disorder in NSSI (14.04%) and NSSI + BPD (50%), with an additional two participants from the CC group also presenting with PTSD (6.25%). Groups differed significantly regarding the diagnoses depression, χ 2 (2, N = 103) = 11.87, p < 0.01, and PTSD, p < 0.01, according to a two-sided Fisher’s exact test. There were no significant differences regarding any other DSM-IV disorders assessed with clinical interviews. Further comorbid diagnoses of the clinical groups were dysthymia, oppositional defiant disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, bulimia nervosa, anorexia nervosa, obsessive–compulsive disorder, agoraphobia, panic disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Groups differed significantly regarding the number of diagnoses, F (2, 100) = 30.37, p < 0.01, with patients in the NSSI + BPD group meeting significantly more diagnoses than the other groups (M = 5.43, SD = 1.83), and the NSSI − BPD group meeting significantly more diagnoses (M = 3.39, SD = 1.36) than the CC group (M = 2.03, SD = 1.00). In addition to the number of diagnoses, significant group differences emerged for psychopathology, for both internalizing and externalizing problems (according to the Youth Self Report). NSSI + BPD scored highest, followed by NSSI, CC and NC, see Table 2. Regarding borderline symptomatology, adolescents with NSSI − BPD differed significantly from adolescents with NSSI + BPD on the subscales self-destruction and hostility. Furthermore, NSSI − BPD scored above the cut off on the subscale for social isolation.
Table 2
Mean (standard deviations) of characteristics of non-clinical adolescents (NC), clinical controls without NSSI (CC), adolescents with NSSI disorder (NSSI), and adolescents with NSSI and BPD (NSSI + BPD), as well as ANOVA with orthogonal contrasts and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) between non-clinical and clinical groups (NC vs. rest), clinical controls and NSSI (CC vs. NSSI and NSSI + BPD), and NSSI disorder vs. Borderline personality disorder (NSSI vs. NSSI + BPD)
Characteristic
NC
CC
NSSI
NSSI + BPD
NC vs. rest
Cohen’s
CC vs.
Cohen’s
NSSI vs.
Cohen’s
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
 
d
NSSI total
d
NSSI + BPD
d
YSR
(n = 57)
(n = 28)
(n = 47)
(n = 11)
t (139)
 
t (139)
 
t (139)
 
Total
57.60 (18.70)
81.80 (21.60)
105.38 (29.97)
134.28 (22.40)
12.56**
2.22
7.04**
1.55
4.03**
1.02
YSR exta
9.79 (6.56)
12.38 (6.45)
17.47 (9.15)
30.76 (7.82)
6.77**
1.43
4.58**
1.51
3.50**
1.52
YSR int
9.83 (6.46)
23.68 (9.56)
32.49 (9.53)
41.18 (8.68)
14.66**
2.76
6.22**
1.44
3.10**
0.94
BDIb
7.02 (7.20)
21.89 (12.68)
33.40 (12.17)
43.20 (13.29)
13.17**
2.39
4.70**
1.31
1.82*
0.81
BSL-95
(n = 57)
(n = 25)
(n = 38)
(n = 9)
t (125)
 
t (125)
 
t (125)
 
Totala
47.67 (28.69)
117.31 (68.98)
182.84 (68.26)
240.55 (70.52)
11.31**
2.42
4.01**
1.38
1.46*
0.86
Dysphoriac
20.56 (8.98)
26.40 (10.23)
30.53 (6.72)
33.04 (6.93)
−2.27
1.13
−.546
0.67
−.110
0.38
     
t (94.85)
 
t (34.51)
 
t (16.74)
 
Self-perceptiona
3.63 (4.37)
15.13 (14.98)
28.10 (17.06)
41.85 (20.82)
11.65**
1.85
4.30**
1.14
2.12
0.79
     
t (96.16)
 
t (34.61)
 
t (26.40)
 
Affect regulationb
6.04 (6.33)
19.48 (11.99)
28.66 (11.29)
36.67 (7.12)
13.42**
2.59
4.54**
1.31
2.95*
0.77
Self-destructionb
1.46 (2.38)
9.20 (7.91)
25.66 (11.55)
34.37 (7.88)
16.28**
3.12
8.17**
2.31
2.21**
0.81
Social isolationb
4.09 (4.97)
12.58 (9.65)
21.87 (12.66)
29.33 (10.46)
10.38**
1.96
4.31**
1.21
1.81*
0.62
Hostilityb
2.34 (3.14)
4.64 (4.41)
8.82 (5.92)
14.89 (5.82)
8.36**
1.58
4.69**
1.35
2.74**
1.05
Intrusionsa
1.34 (2.11)
6.32 (6.37)
12.13 (7.50)
20.33 (12.08)
10.51**
1.77
4.58**
1.15
1.65
0.99
JTCI
(n = 51)
(n = 26)
(n = 46)
(n = 11)
t (130)
 
t (130)
 
t (130)
 
Novelty seeking (T)a
47.29 (8.20)
43.00 (8.62)
48.20 (11.61)
56.00 (8.31)
0.66
0.20
3.42**
0,96
2.39**
0.72
Harm avoidance (T)
49.33 (10.18)
59.38 (8.59)
61.35 (11.10)
69.64 (8.51)
7.32**
1.47
2.34**
0,66
2.44**
0.79
Reward dependence (T)
57.06 (8.37)
52.04 (9.20)
49.96 (10.77)
45.91 (12.03)
−4.18**
0.79
−1.64
0,39
−1.24
0.37
Persistence (T)
50.22 (10.21)
53.73 (9.93)
45.09 (11.74)
35.27 (9.70)
−2.71**
0.54
−4.92**
1.31
−2.74**
0.88
Self-directedness (C)
52.22 (10.41)
43.88 (10.45)
33.22 (11.70)
26.73 (9.81)
−8.51**
1.68
−4.97**
1.32
−1.78
0.58
Cooperativeness (C)
53.75 (8.89)
56.88 (9.21)
54.93 (11.77)
46.27 (9.70)
−0.54
0.11
−2.41*
0.62
−2.56**
0.78
Self-transcendence (C)
49.43 (9.58)
53.92 (10.68)
50.02 (9.12)
50.82 (11.81)
1.15
0.21
−1.38
0.34
0.24
0.08
BIS
(n = 28)
(n = 21)
(n = 29)
(n = 8)
t (82)
 
t (82)
 
t (82)
 
Impulsivity (BIS)
20.76 (3.15)
20.06 (3.47)
22.97 (3.94)
26.85 (2.78)
2.99**
0.77
4.70**
1.45
2.78**
1.07
Attentional
15.61 (4.01)
14.90 (3.16)
18.25 (4.10)
20.88 (1.89)
2.67**
0.72
4.24**
1.55
1.77*
0.72
Non-planning
25.52 (4.33)
24.59 (5.13)
27.47 (5.76)
34.63 (5.07)
2.72**
0.68
4.27**
1.24
3.51**
1.31
Motor
21.16 (3.96)
20.70 (3.97)
23.21 (6.90)
25.04 (4.04)
1.46
0.39
2.24*
0.70
0.89
0.29
YSR Youth self report (ext = externalizing, int = internalizing); BDI Beck Depression Inventory-II; JTCI Junior Temperament and Character Inventory; BIS Barratt Impulsiveness Scale
Bootstrapped and Bonferroni-Holm corrected p values * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01
a log transformation, b root transformation, c reciprocal transformation

Junior Temperament and Character Inventory

As reported in Table 2, significant group differences were shown on the temperament scales novelty seeking, F(3, 130) = 4.32, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.09, harm avoidance, F(3, 130) = 18.80, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.30, reward dependence, F(3, 130) = 6.47, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.13, and persistence F(3, 130) = 9.57, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.18, as well as on the character scales self-directedness, F(3, 130) = 32.71, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.43, and cooperativeness, F(3, 130) = 2.99, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.06. There was no significant group difference regarding self-transcendence, F(3, 130) = 1.28, p = 0.28, η2 = 0.03. Compared to clinical controls, adolescents with NSSI scored higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on persistence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness. The harm avoidance score was over the cut off while the other scores were within the normal range. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD showed even higher scores for novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower scores for persistence and cooperativeness than adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD scored above the cut off on harm avoidance and below the cut off on persistence and self-directedness.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

Regarding the MANOVA for the BIS subscales, the group main effect was significant, F(3, 82) = 9.21, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.25. There was no significant Group x Impulsivity interaction, F(6, 164) = 1.36, p = 0.23, η2 = 0.05, indicating that the group differences are the same for all three subscales of the BIS. As shown in Table 2, the subsequent one-way ANOVA yielded significant group differences regarding impulsivity for the total scale, F(3, 130) = 9.21, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.25, as well as for the subscales attentional, F (3, 130) = 7.47, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.21, and non-planning impulsivity, F(3, 130) = 8.32, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.23, but not for the subscale motor impulsivity, F(3, 130) = 2.13, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.07.

Go/NoGo-Task

Regarding the non-emotional task, there was no significant group effect for participants’ sensitivity index, F(3, 151) = 0.93, p = 0.43, commission errors, F(3, 151) = 0.43, p = 0.73, omission errors, F(3, 154) = 1.22, p = 0.31, or reaction time, F(3, 147) = 2.06, p = 0.11. The ANOVAs for the emotional task, when emotional faces were Go trials, revealed no significant main effects or interactions except for commission errors. There was a significant main effect for facial emotion, F(1, 148) = 29.83, p < 0.01, indicating a higher commission error rate for angry faces than for happy faces. Regarding omission errors, the main effect for facial emotion reached significance, F(1, 155) = 65.50, p < 0.01, indicating a higher omission error rate for angry faces than for happy faces. For reaction time (Go), the main effect for facial emotion was significant, F(1, 154) = 20.95, p < 0.01, indicating a faster reaction to happy compared to angry faces. The ANOVAs conducted for the emotional task, when neutral faces were Go trials revealed no significant effects for the sensitivity index, commission and omission error rates. For reaction time as an outcome, only one significant main effect was found: facial emotion, F(1, 146) = 11.94, p < 0.01, indicating a faster reaction to neutral faces, when happy faces served as NoGo compared to angry faces. The means and standard deviations are displayed in Table 3.
Table 3
Sensitivity index d’, commission and omission errors of the Go/NoGo, as well as reaction times for go trials of non-clinical adolescents (NC), clinical controls without NSSI (CC), adolescents with NSSI disorder (NSSI), and adolescents with NSSI and borderline personality disorder (NSSI + BPD)
Condition
NC
CC
NSSI
NSSI + BPD
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
d’
X
0.16 (1.16)
0.31 (1.07)
−0.01 (1.30)
−0.27 (1.29)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo)
0.12 (1.66)
−0.18 (1.59)
0.02 (1.38)
−0.72 (1.46)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo)
−0.04 (1.47)
0.42 (0.87)
0.08 (1.37)
−0.86 (1.50)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo)
0.05 (1.12)
0.19 (1.19)
−0.10 (1.33)
−0.40 (1.50)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo)
0.34 (1.44)
0.36 (0.82)
0.06 (1.46)
−0.62 (1.20)
Commission
X
1.95 (4.55)
2.00 (5.19)
2.02 (4.57)
3.57 (7.45)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo)
15.42 (14.80)
15.42 (11.22)
18.63 (16.92)
21.15 (16.44)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo)
8.67 (11.43)
6.67 (10.24)
8.82 (11.80)
13.39 (11.46)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo)
5.83 (9.34)
4.03 (9.89)
6.37 (9.37)
4.46 (9.31)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo)
5.42 (10.88)
3.23 (6.43)
5.19 (9.31)
6.25 (9.49)
Omission
X
14.34 (13.24)
12.26 (13.09)
17.21 (15.13)
18.57 (10.46)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo)
7.38 (12.37)
10.48 (12.95)
6.37 (6.76)
11.61 (10.36)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo)
0.82 (3.12)
0.00 (0.00)
0.47 (2.40)
1.79 (4.54)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo)
2.29 (6.71)
2.92 (5.38)
3.54 (9.61)
8.65 (9.39)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo)
4.30 (16.44)
6.05 (18.78)
6.60 (18.61)
12.50 (18.99)
RT Go
X
373.62 (42.10)
378.22 (41.96)
361.03 (40.66)
353.66 (29.87)
Angry Go (neutral NoGo)
514.52 (86.87)
529.93 (109.17)
509.37 (83.11)
421.31 (119.90)
Happy Go (neutral NoGo)
483.46 (72.24)
492.22 (81.30)
478.21 (78.84)
487.61 (96.52)
Neutral Go (angry NoGo)
503.67 (86.93)
522.27 (89.08)
516.01 (82.00)
517.93 (100.72)
Neutral Go (happy NoGo)
533.06 (87.16)
546.78 (106.83)
527.60 (95.38)
551.99 (89.60)
d’ sensitivity index; Commission Commission error; Omission Omission error; RT Go reaction time for the go condition
There were no significant group effects

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate temperament and character traits on the basis of Cloninger's [12, 13] personality model, with a special focus on impulsivity in adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD (NSSI − BPD), adolescents with NSSI disorder and BPD (NSSI + BPD), a clinical control group, and a nonclinical control group. As expected, the groups showed distinct personality profiles. The JTCI scales as well as most YSR scales indicate a staircase-like appearance ranging from nonclinical adolescents to adolescents with NSSI + BPD. Adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD scored higher on novelty seeking and harm avoidance and lower on self-directedness, persistence and cooperativeness than clinical controls. In adolescents with NSSI + BPD this personality pattern was even more pronounced than in adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Thus, we were able to replicate the personality pattern consisting of high harm avoidance and novelty seeking in adolescents with NSSI + BPD, similar to Cloninger [16] and Kaess et al. [18]. The approach-avoidance conflict generated from this pattern might be a reason for the emotional instability patients with BPD experience [19]. In addition, we extended these findings to adolescents with NSSI disorder without BPD. In these patients, the personality pattern described above was less pronounced. Nevertheless, the harm avoidance score above cut off indicates that adolescents with NSSI − BPD are more careful, fearful, insecure, and negativistic than the adolescents from the CC and the NC groups. Adolescents with NSSI − BPD differed from adolescents with NSSI + BPD regarding psychopathology and partially in borderline symptomatology but nevertheless showed a similar personality pattern to adolescents with NSSI + BPD. This result underlines the need for a dimensional personality assessment to better understand adolescents with NSSI − BPD. Further research should focus on maladaptive personality traits that do not constitute a formal personality disorder and on the validation of the dimensional personality model suggested in section III of the DSM-5.
Results of the present study replicated a profile of lower levels of self-directedness in adolescents with NSSI (−BPD and +BPD) than adolescents without NSSI, similar to Hefti et al. [20] and Joyce et al. [21]. In contrast to Ohmann et al. [22], we found lower levels of cooperativeness in adolescents with NSSI compared to adolescents without NSSI, however this result is similar to the low level of cooperativeness found in adolescents with BPD [53]. Lower cooperativeness may cause more interpersonal conflict and distress through socially intolerant, critical, and destructive conflict behavior. In fact, previous research indicates that adolescents with NSSI frequently report problems in social interactions [54] that can trigger NSSI [55]. Compared to the CC group, the level of persistence in adolescents with NSSI was low but still in the normal range. Previous studies have shown that adolescents with NSSI give up faster when pursuing goals, while adolescents without NSSI are more diligent and persevering [40]. All groups were similar regarding self-transcendence, therefore, we could not find supporting evidence for a higher self-transcendence as previously reported in adolescents with NSSI [20] and adults with BPD [14]. This may be explained by differences in the study populations (school sample vs. clinical sample, female vs. male adolescents, adolescents vs. adults and NSSI vs. BPD).
To summarize, there was a significant difference in temperament and character traits between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD, despite the small NSSI + BPD sample size (n = 14). Compared to the other groups, the NSSI − BPD group displayed higher standard deviations on the subscales of the JTCI, indicating the heterogeneity of this group. Considerable diagnostic heterogeneity among adolescents with NSSI has been described in earlier studies [2].
Adolescents with NSSI disorder (−BPD and +BPD) showed more novelty seeking than the CC group as well as higher scores on all subscales of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (attentional, non-planning, and motor impulsivity). However, this difference was not evident in the Go/NoGo task with neither a group effect, nor an emotion effect emerging. Happy faces were associated with faster reactions and a lower error rate compared to angry faces, indicating that happy faces are easier to discern than angry faces. Our results are in line with several other studies that indicated more self-reported impulsivity in adolescents [26, 29] and adults with NSSI [32], but failed to show this difference on behavioral measures. This leaves the question open, as to whether adolescents with NSSI perceive themselves as more impulsive than they actually are. However, this discrepancy between self-report and behavioral measures is not only observed in adolescents with NSSI, but also represents a general difficulty in the measurement of impulsivity that may be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs [32]. It remains to be investigated, if the difference between self-reported and experimentally assessed impulsivity can be explained by the measurement of different impulsivity constructs, or if adolescents with NSSI are able to suppress their impulsivity for an experimental task. Adolescents with NSSI + BPD reported even more impulsivity than adolescents with NSSI − BPD, especially more non-planning impulsivity (lack of future orientation and foresight). Highly impulsive individuals may be especially motivated to act rashly in the context of negative emotions because long-term benefits become less important compared to short-term gains of emotion regulation, e.g. The Theory of Urgency [56], also see [57]. Therefore, individuals with high levels of non-planning impulsivity may be highly motivated to obtain the immediate benefits of NSSI (e.g., relief of negative emotions) with less concern for the long-term consequences of NSSI. There was no significant difference between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and with NSSI − BPD in the Go/NoGo task.
The results of the present study should be interpreted in the context of some limitations. The design of the study was cross-sectional. Therefore, the current study cannot explain whether certain temperament and character traits might favor the development of NSSI. This should be investigated in future prospective longitudinal studies. Nevertheless, results indicate an association between temperament and character traits and NSSI disorder. Due to the small sample sizes of adolescents with BPD, comorbidity with other personality disorders could not be included in the analyses. The recommendation of the DSM-5 is to apply a diagnosis of a personality disorder in children and adolescents when maladaptive personality traits appear to be pervasive, persistent, unlikely to be limited to a particular developmental stage or another mental disorder, and after one year of persistent symptoms. Given the mean age of the participants under 16 years of age, we were careful applying a diagnosis of a personality disorder. However, despite the small NSSI + BPD sample size, significant differences emerged between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD. The high prevalence of NSSI in inpatient samples (50%) [9] represented a challenge for the recruitment of a clinical inpatient sample without NSSI. Our sample consisted of female adolescents admitted to a psychiatric unit and therefore generalizations to male outpatients must be made with caution. Regarding the Go/NoGo task, the low error rate indicates that the response pressure was too low. Therefore, future studies should use a higher ratio of Go stimuli to NoGo stimuli.
A strength of this study was the use of the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for NSSI disorder in a clinical sample. In addition, a clinical control group of adolescents with other mental disorders without NSSI was included. This allowed us to identify temperament and character traits specific to NSSI disorder with and without BPD. To our knowledge, this is the first study comparing temperament and character traits in adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD in an inpatient setting. In addition to self-report measures, impulsivity was assessed using an experimental task.

Conclusions

Given the differences in temperament and character traits between adolescents with NSSI + BPD and adolescents with NSSI − BPD, a personality assessment using the JTCI [40] might be useful for the diagnostic distinction between adolescents with NSSI with and without BPD. A clear distinction of these two groups might be helpful when choosing a specific treatment for adolescents engaging in NSSI. As specific treatment programs for adolescents with NSSI are still in development, practitioners mostly use treatment programs for BPD [58]. The development of specific treatment programs for adolescents with NSSI may not only optimize treatment, but also allow an early intervention, preventing chronic conditions [59]. Future studies should investigate temperament and character traits of adolescents with NSSI in the long-term as well as the effects of psychotherapy on character and temperament development.

Authors’ contributions

TT and CR completed the data analyses and made substantial contributions to the interpretation of the data, the drafting, and the revision of the manuscript. TI and MS contributed to the ideas, the acquisition and interpretation of the data, the drafting and the revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank the participants in this study as well as the research assistants and graduate students on the project at the University of Basel for their assistance in data collection and management. The authors thank the following clinics for recruitment: Zentrum für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und -psychotherapie Clienia Littenheid AG, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrischer Dienst Koenigsfelden, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Kriens, St. Elisabethen-Krankenhaus Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Loerrach, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Chur, Universitaere Psychiatrische Kliniken Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Basel, Universitaetsklinik fuer Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie Bern, Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrische Klinik Solothurn, and Klinik Sonnenhof Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrisches Zentrum Ganterschwil.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have competing interests.

Availability of data and material

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
All participants and parents gave their written consent.
The local ethics committee (Ethikkommission Beider Basel, EKBB) approved the study.

Funding

This study is supported by grant project 100014_135205 awarded to Tina In-Albon in collaboration with Marc Schmid by the Swiss National Science Foundation.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Auerbach RP, Kim JC, Chango JM, Spiro WJ, Cha C, Gold J, et al. Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: examining the role of child abuse, comorbidity, and disinhibition. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:579–84.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Auerbach RP, Kim JC, Chango JM, Spiro WJ, Cha C, Gold J, et al. Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: examining the role of child abuse, comorbidity, and disinhibition. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:579–84.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Plener PL, Kapusta ND, Kölch MG, Kaess M, Brunner R. Non-suicidal self-injury as autonomous diagnosis-implications for research and clinic of the DSM-5 proposal to establish the diagnosis of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in adolescents. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2012;40:113–20.CrossRefPubMed Plener PL, Kapusta ND, Kölch MG, Kaess M, Brunner R. Non-suicidal self-injury as autonomous diagnosis-implications for research and clinic of the DSM-5 proposal to establish the diagnosis of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in adolescents. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2012;40:113–20.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Zetterqvist M, Lundh LG, Dahlström Ö, Svedin CG. Prevalence and function of non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) in a community sample of adolescents, using suggested DSM-5 criteria for a potential NSSI disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:759–73.CrossRefPubMed Zetterqvist M, Lundh LG, Dahlström Ö, Svedin CG. Prevalence and function of non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) in a community sample of adolescents, using suggested DSM-5 criteria for a potential NSSI disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:759–73.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Victor SE, Klonsky ED. Daily emotion in non-suicidal self-injury. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70:364–75.CrossRefPubMed Victor SE, Klonsky ED. Daily emotion in non-suicidal self-injury. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70:364–75.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrews T, Martin G, Hasking P, Page A. Predictors of onset for non-suicidal self-injury within a school-based sample of adolescents. Prev Sci. 2014;15:850–9.CrossRefPubMed Andrews T, Martin G, Hasking P, Page A. Predictors of onset for non-suicidal self-injury within a school-based sample of adolescents. Prev Sci. 2014;15:850–9.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Zlotnick C, Mattia JI, Zimmerman M. Clinical correlates of self-mutilation in a sample of general psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187:296–301.CrossRefPubMed Zlotnick C, Mattia JI, Zimmerman M. Clinical correlates of self-mutilation in a sample of general psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187:296–301.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Glenn CR, Klonsky ED. Nonsuicidal self-injury disorder: an empirical investigation in adolescent psychiatric patients. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013;42:496–507.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Glenn CR, Klonsky ED. Nonsuicidal self-injury disorder: an empirical investigation in adolescent psychiatric patients. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013;42:496–507.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Turner BJ, Dixon-Gordon KL, Austin SB, Rodriguez MA, Rosenthal MZ, Chapman AL. Non-suicidal self-injury with and without borderline personality disorder: differences in self-injury and diagnostic comorbidity. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230:28–35.CrossRefPubMed Turner BJ, Dixon-Gordon KL, Austin SB, Rodriguez MA, Rosenthal MZ, Chapman AL. Non-suicidal self-injury with and without borderline personality disorder: differences in self-injury and diagnostic comorbidity. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230:28–35.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Bracken-Minor KL, McDevitt-Murphy ME. Differences in features of non-suicidal self-injury according to borderline personality disorder screening status. Arch Suicide Res. 2014;18:88–103.CrossRefPubMed Bracken-Minor KL, McDevitt-Murphy ME. Differences in features of non-suicidal self-injury according to borderline personality disorder screening status. Arch Suicide Res. 2014;18:88–103.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:573–88.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:573–88.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–90.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–90.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnow DS, Rüge J, Spitzer C, Freyberger HJ. Temperament und Charakter bei Personen mit Borderline-Persönlichkeitsstörung. Nervenarzt. 2005;76:839–48.CrossRefPubMed Barnow DS, Rüge J, Spitzer C, Freyberger HJ. Temperament und Charakter bei Personen mit Borderline-Persönlichkeitsstörung. Nervenarzt. 2005;76:839–48.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmeck K, Schlüter-Müller S, Foelsch PA, Doering S. The role of identity in the DSM-5 classification of personality disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2013;7:27.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schmeck K, Schlüter-Müller S, Foelsch PA, Doering S. The role of identity in the DSM-5 classification of personality disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2013;7:27.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR. A practical way to diagnosis personality disorder: a proposal. J Pers Disord. 2000;14:99–108.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR. A practical way to diagnosis personality disorder: a proposal. J Pers Disord. 2000;14:99–108.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Joyce PR, Mulder RT, Luty SE, McKenzie JM, Sullivan PF, Cloninger RC. Borderline personality disorder in major depression: symptomatology, temperament, character, differential drug response, and 6-month outcome. Compr Psychiat. 2003;44:35–43.CrossRefPubMed Joyce PR, Mulder RT, Luty SE, McKenzie JM, Sullivan PF, Cloninger RC. Borderline personality disorder in major depression: symptomatology, temperament, character, differential drug response, and 6-month outcome. Compr Psychiat. 2003;44:35–43.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaess M, Resch F, Parzer P, von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna IA, Henze R, Brunner R. Temperamental patterns in female adolescents with Borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201:109–15.CrossRefPubMed Kaess M, Resch F, Parzer P, von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna IA, Henze R, Brunner R. Temperamental patterns in female adolescents with Borderline personality disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201:109–15.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Praybeck T, Svrakic DM, Wetzel R. The Temperament and Character Inventory: A guide to its development and use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality. St Louis: Washington University; 1994. Cloninger CR, Praybeck T, Svrakic DM, Wetzel R. The Temperament and Character Inventory: A guide to its development and use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality. St Louis: Washington University; 1994.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Hefti S, In-Albon T, Schmeck K, Schmid M. Temperaments-und Charaktereigenschaften und selbstverletzendes Verhalten bei Jugendlichen. Nervenheilkunde. 2013;32:45–53. Hefti S, In-Albon T, Schmeck K, Schmid M. Temperaments-und Charaktereigenschaften und selbstverletzendes Verhalten bei Jugendlichen. Nervenheilkunde. 2013;32:45–53.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Joyce PR, Light KJ, Rowe SL, Cloninger CR, Kennedy MA. Self-mutilation and suicide attempts: relationships to bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, temperament and character. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44:250–7.CrossRefPubMed Joyce PR, Light KJ, Rowe SL, Cloninger CR, Kennedy MA. Self-mutilation and suicide attempts: relationships to bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, temperament and character. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44:250–7.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Ohmann S, Schuch B, König M, Blaas S, Fliri C, Popow C. Self-injurious behavior in adolescent girls. Psychopathology. 2008;41:226–35.CrossRefPubMed Ohmann S, Schuch B, König M, Blaas S, Fliri C, Popow C. Self-injurious behavior in adolescent girls. Psychopathology. 2008;41:226–35.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim SJ, Lee SJ, Yune SK, Sung YH, Bae SC, Chung A, et al. The relationship between the biogenetic temperament and character and psychopathology in adolescents. Psychopathology. 2006;39:80–6.CrossRefPubMed Kim SJ, Lee SJ, Yune SK, Sung YH, Bae SC, Chung A, et al. The relationship between the biogenetic temperament and character and psychopathology in adolescents. Psychopathology. 2006;39:80–6.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLaren VV, Best LA. Nonsuicidal self-injury, potentially addictive behaviors, and the five factor model in undergraduates. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49:521–5.CrossRef MacLaren VV, Best LA. Nonsuicidal self-injury, potentially addictive behaviors, and the five factor model in undergraduates. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49:521–5.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Mullins-Sweatt SN, Lengel GJ, Grant DM. Non-suicidal self-injury: the contribution of general personality functioning. Personal Ment Health. 2013;7:56–68.CrossRefPubMed Mullins-Sweatt SN, Lengel GJ, Grant DM. Non-suicidal self-injury: the contribution of general personality functioning. Personal Ment Health. 2013;7:56–68.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Glenn CR, Klonsky DE. A multimethod analysis of impulsivity in nonsuicidal self-injury. Personal Disord. 2010;1:67–75.CrossRefPubMed Glenn CR, Klonsky DE. A multimethod analysis of impulsivity in nonsuicidal self-injury. Personal Disord. 2010;1:67–75.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Nock MK, Prinstein MJ. Contextual features and behavioral functions of self-mutilation among adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:140–6.CrossRefPubMed Nock MK, Prinstein MJ. Contextual features and behavioral functions of self-mutilation among adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:140–6.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans J, Platts H, Liebenau A. Impulsiveness and deliberate self-harm: a comparison of “first-timers” and “repeaters”. Acta Psychiat Scand. 1996;93:378–80.CrossRefPubMed Evans J, Platts H, Liebenau A. Impulsiveness and deliberate self-harm: a comparison of “first-timers” and “repeaters”. Acta Psychiat Scand. 1996;93:378–80.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:965–82.CrossRefPubMed Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:965–82.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat McCloskey MS, Look AE, Chen EY, Pajoumand G, Berman ME. Nonsuicidal self-injury: relationship to behavioral and self-rating measures of impulsivity and self-aggression. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2012;42:197–209.CrossRefPubMed McCloskey MS, Look AE, Chen EY, Pajoumand G, Berman ME. Nonsuicidal self-injury: relationship to behavioral and self-rating measures of impulsivity and self-aggression. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2012;42:197–209.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J. Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Heidelberg: Springer; 2009. Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J. Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Heidelberg: Springer; 2009.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider S, Margraf J. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen. 4th ed. Heidelberg: Springer; 2011. Schneider S, Margraf J. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen. 4th ed. Heidelberg: Springer; 2011.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Neuschwander M, In-Albon T, Adornetto C, Roth B, Schneider S. Interrater-Reliabilität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2013;41:319–34.CrossRefPubMed Neuschwander M, In-Albon T, Adornetto C, Roth B, Schneider S. Interrater-Reliabilität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2013;41:319–34.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen HUSKIDII. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997. Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen HUSKIDII. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Salbach-Andrae H, Bürger A, Klinkowski N, Lenz K, Pfeiffer E, Fydrich T, et al. Diagnostik von Persönlichkeitsstörungen im Jugendalter nach SKID-II. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2008;36:117–25.CrossRefPubMed Salbach-Andrae H, Bürger A, Klinkowski N, Lenz K, Pfeiffer E, Fydrich T, et al. Diagnostik von Persönlichkeitsstörungen im Jugendalter nach SKID-II. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2008;36:117–25.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Sender I, Gratwohl T, Stieglitz RD. Entwicklung der borderline-symptom-liste. PsychotherPsych Med. 2001;51:201–11. Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Sender I, Gratwohl T, Stieglitz RD. Entwicklung der borderline-symptom-liste. PsychotherPsych Med. 2001;51:201–11.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Chapman AL, Kühler T, Stieglitz RD. Psychometric properties of the borderline symptom list (BSL). Psychopathology. 2007;40:126–32.CrossRefPubMed Bohus M, Limberger MF, Frank U, Chapman AL, Kühler T, Stieglitz RD. Psychometric properties of the borderline symptom list (BSL). Psychopathology. 2007;40:126–32.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Goth K, Schmeck K. Das Junior Temperament und Charakter Inventar (JTCI) Manual. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2009. Goth K, Schmeck K. Das Junior Temperament und Charakter Inventar (JTCI) Manual. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2009.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Barratt ES. Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 1959;9:191–8.CrossRef Barratt ES. Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 1959;9:191–8.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartmann AS, Rief W, Hilbert A. Psychometric properties of the German version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, version 11 (BIS-11) for adolescents. Percept Mot Skills. 2011;112:353–68.CrossRefPubMed Hartmann AS, Rief W, Hilbert A. Psychometric properties of the German version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, version 11 (BIS-11) for adolescents. Percept Mot Skills. 2011;112:353–68.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991. Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Döpfner M, Melchers P, Fegert J, Lehmkuhl G, Lehmkuhl U, Schmeck K, et al. Deutschsprachige Konsensus-Versionen der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 4–18), der Teacher Report Form (TRF) und der Youth Self Report Form (YSR). Kindh Entwickl. 1994;3:54–9. Döpfner M, Melchers P, Fegert J, Lehmkuhl G, Lehmkuhl U, Schmeck K, et al. Deutschsprachige Konsensus-Versionen der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 4–18), der Teacher Report Form (TRF) und der Youth Self Report Form (YSR). Kindh Entwickl. 1994;3:54–9.
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C. Beck Depressions-Inventar II (BDI-II). Frankfurt am Main: Harcourt Test Services; 2006. Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C. Beck Depressions-Inventar II (BDI-II). Frankfurt am Main: Harcourt Test Services; 2006.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168:242–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168:242–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:927–34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:927–34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Trommer BL, Hoeppner JB, Lorber R, Armstrong KJ. The Go-No-Go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol. 1988;24:610–4.CrossRefPubMed Trommer BL, Hoeppner JB, Lorber R, Armstrong KJ. The Go-No-Go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol. 1988;24:610–4.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulz KP, Fan J, Magidina O, Marks DJ, Hahn B, Halperin JM. Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition?: convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:151–60.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schulz KP, Fan J, Magidina O, Marks DJ, Hahn B, Halperin JM. Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition?: convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:151–60.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Ladouceur CD, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Ryan ND, Casey BJ. Processing emotional facial expressions influences performance on a Go/NoGo task in pediatric anxiety and depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:1107–15.CrossRefPubMed Ladouceur CD, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Ryan ND, Casey BJ. Processing emotional facial expressions influences performance on a Go/NoGo task in pediatric anxiety and depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:1107–15.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Pacheco-Unguetti AP, Acosta A, Lupiáñez J, Román N, Derakshan N. Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. Q J Exp Psychol. 2012;65:646–60.CrossRef Pacheco-Unguetti AP, Acosta A, Lupiáñez J, Román N, Derakshan N. Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. Q J Exp Psychol. 2012;65:646–60.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown SA. Personality and non-suicidal deliberate self-harm: trait differences among a non-clinical population. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169:28–32.CrossRefPubMed Brown SA. Personality and non-suicidal deliberate self-harm: trait differences among a non-clinical population. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169:28–32.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Adrian M, Zeman J, Erdley C, Lisa L, Sim L. Emotional dysregulation and interpersonal difficulties as risk factors for nonsuicidal self-injury in adolescent girls. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2011;39:389–400.CrossRef Adrian M, Zeman J, Erdley C, Lisa L, Sim L. Emotional dysregulation and interpersonal difficulties as risk factors for nonsuicidal self-injury in adolescent girls. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2011;39:389–400.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Tschan T, Schmid M, In-Albon T. Parenting behavior in families of female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury in comparison to a clinical and a nonclinical control group. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:1–9.CrossRef Tschan T, Schmid M, In-Albon T. Parenting behavior in families of female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury in comparison to a clinical and a nonclinical control group. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:1–9.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:53.CrossRefPubMed Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:53.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Mehlum L, Tørmoen AJ, Ramberg M, Haga E, Diep LM, Laberg S, et al. Dialectical behavior therapy for adolescents with repeated suicidal and self-harming behavior: a randomized trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:1082–91.CrossRefPubMed Mehlum L, Tørmoen AJ, Ramberg M, Haga E, Diep LM, Laberg S, et al. Dialectical behavior therapy for adolescents with repeated suicidal and self-harming behavior: a randomized trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:1082–91.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Bradford Reich D, Silk KR. Prediction of the 10-year course of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:827–32.CrossRefPubMed Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Bradford Reich D, Silk KR. Prediction of the 10-year course of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:827–32.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Temperament and character traits in female adolescents with nonsuicidal self-injury disorder with and without comorbid borderline personality disorder
verfasst von
Taru Tschan
Claudia Peter-Ruf
Marc Schmid
Tina In-Albon
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1753-2000
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0142-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.