Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research

The effect of impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits in the saccadic behavior of premanifest Huntington’s disease individuals

verfasst von: Filipa Júlio, Gina Caetano, Cristina Januário, Miguel Castelo-Branco

Erschienen in: Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

This study aims to test response inhibition in premanifest Huntington’s disease individuals (Pre-HD), in the context of a saccadic paradigm with working memory demands and fronto-executive load as a way to measure inhibitory control deficits and impulsive behavior in Huntington’s disease (HD).

Methods

The oculomotor function of 15 Pre-HD and 22 Control individuals was assessed using an experimental paradigm comprising four horizontal saccadic tasks: prosaccade (PS), antisaccade (AS), 1- or 2-back memory prosaccade (MPS), and 1- or 2-back memory antisaccade (MAS). Success rate, latency, directional and timing errors were calculated for each task. A comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests was also used to assess the overall cognitive functioning of study participants. Statistical correlations between oculomotor, clinical and cognitive measures were computed for the Pre-HD group.

Results

Pre-HD participants showed reduced success rate in the AS task, increased direction errors in the AS and MAS tasks and decreased latency in the MAS task when compared to Controls, despite presenting similar executive and memory scores in the conventional neuropsychological tests applied. Significant associations were identified between specific AS and MAS parameters and disease-related measures, cognitive skills and other oculomotor results of Pre-HD participants.

Conclusions

Our results show that oculomotor performance in premanifest Huntington’s disease deteriorates once inhibitory control, working memory and/or fronto-executive load are added to the task. A more automatic pattern of performance, including a faster response time and directionally erroneous eye movements were detected in the oculomotor behavior of the Pre-HD group—these alterations were significantly correlated with disease stage and cognitive status. Our saccadic paradigm was able to capture impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits in a group of Pre-HD individuals on average far from symptom onset, thus holding the potential to identify the earliest disease-related changes.

Background

Huntington’s disease (HD) is an autosomal inherited neurodegenerative disorder caused by a Cytosine-Adenine-Guanine (CAG) trinucleotide repeat expansion in the huntingtin gene. HD is characterized by motor abnormalities, emotional and behavioral changes, and a general cognitive decline [13]. Testing positive for HD supplies only information on gene status, but not on disease state, as the test result gives little indication on how and when the triad of symptoms will start [4, 5]. The proper identification and quantification of the signs and symptoms exhibited by individuals that tested positive for HD but are still in a premanifest stage is vital to implement and assess the efficacy of any therapeutic interventions [6].
Although there is now a consensual idea that cognitive impairments emerge years before HD clinical diagnosis and that the progression of cognitive decline is gradual [710], the conversion of an individual from a premanifest to a manifest HD status is classically defined solely on the basis of motor signs, with no consideration for cognitive and/or psychiatric disturbances [1, 2, 11]. Nevertheless, the cognitive changes associated with HD need also to be fully addressed in disease progression and characterization [12]—impairments in inhibitory control, attention, working memory, executive functions, mental flexibility, psychomotor functions, planning, processing speed, multitasking, organization, problem solving, implicit learning, visuospatial functions, timing and movement sequencing, face and emotion processing and recognition [4, 911, 1325].
One of the cognitive symptoms most peculiar to HD is the executive dysfunction syndrome, a condition that encompasses disinhibition, attentional deficits, poor impulse control, and perseveration [12, 26]. In HD, these changes in different aspects of top-down control mechanisms are associated with the disruption of the corticostriatal circuitry, especially the prefrontal-striatal connections [19, 2730]. This circuitry is important for the planning of an instrumental performance, temporal control over motor output, and response inhibition in general [27, 28, 31]. Accordingly, individuals with established basal ganglia damage, such as those with HD or Parkinson’s Disease (PD), experience difficulty selecting a preferred motor activity and inhibiting undesired responses, frequently displaying impulsivity and altered behavior inhibition in their performance [29, 3237].
Thus, impulsivity can be defined as the observable behavioral manifestation of a failure of the prefrontal cortex in inhibiting an overt motor act or response [28]. Despite the multitude of studies about impulsivity in PD [3639], impulsive behavior in HD needs to be further investigated. Harrington et al. [8] pinpoint that fact, referring to a large, multi-centered prospective study (PREDICT-HD) to indicate that one of the domains that has been inadequately assessed in HD is inhibition. Also, as stated by Bari and co-authors [28], there are many unanswered questions about the mechanisms underlying abnormal impulsive behavior.
Considering these outstanding questions, we aimed at assessing response inhibition and, hence, impulsivity, in an objective manner, by using an oculomotor paradigm with a component of inhibitory motor control and increasing cognitive load. Oculomotor impairments are precisely among the first manifestations of HD, with saccadic abnormalities having been frequently described in HD patients [1, 4, 22, 4047]. Although mixed findings have been reported about premanifest HD individuals’ oculomotor performance [4, 4043, 4850], studies have shown significant alterations in antisaccade and memory-guided saccade measures of latency, higher variability of saccade latency and increased error rates [4, 40, 43, 45, 50]. Findings include higher disinhibition (impaired saccade suppression), higher number of anticipatory saccades (that is, timing errors), increased errors in memory-guided saccade tasks, prolonged latency for initiating voluntary saccades, and an increase in latency for reflexive prosaccades [40, 41, 46, 48, 51]. Nevertheless, Gorges et al. [33] suggest that a comprehensive explanation for the lack of inhibition control at the saccadic or eye movement level in HD remains to be identified. Saccadic paradigms designed to assess inhibition and impulsivity processes in HD can further help to identify underlying deficits and mechanisms. Also, most cognitive/executive tasks, including those explicitly devised as a measure of behavioral inhibition, have been criticized for suffering from low reliability [28]. Thus, as stated by Zhang and colleagues [37], the use of saccadic measures to test deficits in inhibitory oculomotor control with an emphasis on impulsive response patterns can benefit the objective assessment of this cognitive and behavioral trait.
Finally, a number of studies suggest that task complexity (higher cognitive/executive load) is essential for discriminating Pre-HD individuals and controls in the majority of saccadic paradigms [44, 46, 51]. The known frontostriatal impairment in Huntington’s disease, and the proven influence of this circuitry in the inhibitory component of antisaccades, imply that increasingly complex executive and memory saccadic tasks are expected to be more sensitive to disease onset than simple ones [5254].
This study aims to test if inhibitory control demanding oculomotor paradigms, embedded with an increasing fronto-executive and memory load, may provide a sensitive and objective measure of impulsivity, hence failure in inhibiting a motor act, in premanifest HD individuals.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-seven participants completed the neuropsychological assessment and thirty-six participants completed the saccade/eye-tracking protocol (due to technical problems, the oculomotor data of one Pre-HD participant could not be recorded).
Study participants were primarily recruited from the Neurological Department – Movement Disorder Unit of Coimbra University Hospital. They were also recruited through the Huntington’s disease Portuguese Association. All participants gave their informed written consent after the study protocol had been explained to them. Informed consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures were approved by the local Ethics Committee (Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra).
Exclusion criteria included history of alcohol or drug abuse/dependence, concurrent neurological illness, severe ophthalmic disease, and use of psychotropic medication (the last criterion only applied to Controls). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment test, a mild cognitive impairment and dementia screening tool, was also an exclusion criterion [55, 56]—a below the established normative reference score based on age and education [57] was presumed to indicate the presence of mild cognitive impairment and, thus, the participant would no longer take part in the study.
Clinical history, current medications (see Additional file 1: Table S1), and any other information considered to be important for taking part in this study were registered as well. Participants were assigned to two groups (see Table 1):
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the CTRL and Pre-HD groups
 
CTRL = 22
Pre-HD = 15
Chi-Square /Mann-Whitney
χ2/ U
p
 
Gender (F:M) 15:7
Gender (F:M) 8:7
0.836
0.361
 
Handedness (R:L) 19:3
Handedness (R:L) 15:0
3.058
0.383
 
Median
IQR
Median
IQR
  
Age (years)
34
12
37
12
161.5
0.914
Education (years)
11.5
2
12
7
155.5
0.766
CAG repeats
41
2
Time to HD Onset (years)
21.1
11
UHDRS - TMS
1
3
UHDRS - OculoTMS
0
1
UHDRS-TFC
13
0
No significant differences were found between Pre-HD and Controls in any of the Demographic variables
IQR Interquartile Range, CAG repeats CAG repeat expansion confirmed by a genetic test, Time to HD Onset estimated number of years to the formal diagnosis of manifest HD, calculated with the Langbehn’s formula [58], UHDRS Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale [59], TMS Total Motor Score of the UHDRS, OculoTMS a composite score extracted from the sum of the oculomotor items of UHDRS-Motor scale, TFC Total Functional Capacity scale of the UHDRS
Premanifest gene carriers (Pre-HD): 15 individuals with an expanded HD gene (≥36 CAG repeats) who demonstrated either no signs or soft signs of motor abnormalities, i.e., had a diagnostic confidence score of 0–3 on the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale – Motor scale (UHDRS-Motor), a Total Motor Score (TMS) of ≤5, and a Total Functional Capacity (TFC) score of 13 in this UHDRS subscale [59].
Controls (CTRL): 22 non-gene carriers, defined as those individuals with two unexpanded HD alleles (< 36 CAG repeats - gene negative status), or healthy volunteers who were not at risk for HD and had no known neurological disorder (spouses and healthy participants from the community).

Clinical evaluation

An experienced movement disorder neurologist administered the motor subscale of the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale [59] to the Pre-HD participants to establish, with at least 99% certainty, whether individuals had motor manifestations of HD. The neurologist assigned an overall confidence rating that represented the likelihood of motor abnormalities be attributable to HD. The individuals with a Total Motor Score (TMS) of ≤5 and a rating from 0 to 3 in the diagnostic confidence score were classified as Pre-HD. A higher TMS indicates worse clinical symptoms. A cut-off of 5 points was used to determine the premanifest status of the participant, in accordance with the EHDN – Registry study’s guidelines [60]. A composite score (OculoTMS) was computed from the oculomotor component of the UHDRS-Motor scale—ocular pursuit, saccade initiation and saccade velocity items. The Total Functional Capacity subscale (TFC) of the UHDRS was also administered to all the participants of the clinical group, to assess their functional status and determine their premanifest HD stage [45, 59]. The TFC uses a rating between 0 and 13 of different functional domains, and a higher score means higher autonomy and independence in the activities of daily living.

Oculomotor experiment

Participants had to complete four horizontal saccadic tasks, where saccadic movements were recorded using an iViewX Hi-speed eye tracking system (1.06, Sensor Motoric Instruments, Teltow) – see Fig. 1. This paradigm was designed taking into account former findings in healthy individuals, that showed specific disruption of saccadic inhibition when the oculomotor task was conjoint to an increasing executive load via an n-back memory task [52, 54].

Oculomotor testing procedure

Participants were seated in front of a 17-in. monitor with their heads placed in a stable chin rest that was positioned 52 cm from the screen. Prior to each oculomotor task, the examiner instructed verbally the participant to ensure that the goal of each task was fully understood, followed by a practice block to discard potential novelty effects in task performance. Eye-tracking recordings were performed after a 9-point calibration using the subject’s dominant eye. The saccade protocol was administered over a period of 40 to 60 min, in a fixed order, with an increasing working memory and fronto-executive load.
The experiment was compound of four conditions. In each, a central fixation point was defined (cross, 1o diameter in visual angle), and peripheral visual targets (black square, 0.6o visual angle) were randomly assigned to four possible positions at ±6o or ± 12o visual angle. Small position cues were present throughout the experiment at each of the four possible target positions (* symbols, 0.24 o visual angle, light gray in color).
Prosaccade task (PS): The participant was instructed to fixate gaze on a central illuminated green cross, and to look to the peripheral target as rapidly as possible once it appeared, and then return to the central fixation cross. This task consisted of 60 trials.
Antisaccade task (AS): The participant was instructed to fixate gaze on a central illuminated red cross, and to look to the opposite direction of the visual target once it appeared, i.e., the mirror-image location of the target at an equal distance from the central fixation cross. Afterwards, the participant was asked to return to the central fixation cross. This task also consisted of 60 trials.
1- or 2-back Memory Prosaccade task (MPS): The participant was instructed to fixate gaze on a central green cross. While two peripheral squares appeared, the participant was asked to continue to fixate the central green cross. The task period was assigned once the central fixation cross was replaced by a digit, either a green one or a green two, when the participant had to generate a saccade to a remembered position. If the digit was one, the participant was asked to look at the remembered position where the first square had appeared. If the digit was two, the participant was asked to look at the remembered position where the second square had appeared. Then, the participant had to return to the central fixation cross. This task consisted of 96 trials.
1- or 2-back Memory Antisaccade task (MAS): The participant was instructed to fixate gaze on a central red cross. The task period was assigned once the central fixation cross was replaced by a digit, either a red one or a red two, when the participant had to generate a saccade. If the digit was one, the participant was asked to look to the opposite direction (i.e., the mirror-image location) of the remembered position where the first square appeared. If the digit was two, the participant was asked to look to the opposite direction of the remembered position where the second square appeared. Then, the participant had to return to the central fixation red cross. This task also consisted of 96 trials.

Oculomotor data processing

Regarding the psychophysics task, BeGaze software (3.4, Sensor Motoric Instruments, Teltow) was used to create experiments based on saccade detection: peak velocity threshold 400/ms; saccade velocity initiation and termination of 150/ms and 850/ms, respectively; minimum fixation duration of 50 ms; minimum saccade duration of 22 ms. Computed data on saccades and blinks were extracted and further analyzed with the Matlab software toolbox (R2013a).
Identification of valid trials for each task was performed applying the following criteria: i) initiation and termination had to be within a region of interest (± 2.50 x ± 40 visual angle) of the fixation and target position, respectively; ii) the primary saccade initiated within the central fixation ROI, had an amplitude enabling termination outside the ROI (horizontally), was performed in the correct direction, and had a latency higher than 80 ms; iii) if the latency was below 80 ms it was considered an anticipatory saccade error (latency-type error); iv) if the saccade was performed in the opposite horizontal direction it was considered a direction error; v) the primary saccade had a latency below 700 ms (PS and AS tasks) or 1000 ms (MPS and MAS tasks), otherwise it was considered a long-latency error (latency-type error); vi) the total saccadic movement finished within the ROI for the intended target position, prior to return to the central fixation position. Additionally, trials contaminated by blinks or other abnormalities were discarded from the analysis.
For every participant, measures were computed for each of the PS, AS, MPS and MAS tasks, namely: percentage of successful trials – trials free of errors; percentage of direction errors – resulting from a reflexive saccade in the opposite direction of the correct hit; percentage of anticipatory saccade errors – resulting from a premature saccade, in which the participant took less than 80 ms to start the saccade; latency – saccadic reaction time, that is time from stimulus appearance to the onset of the primary saccade (milliseconds). The calculation of mean latencies included only correct trials that met the inclusion criteria.
Only participants that had at least 25% of successful trials (i.e., trials free of any kind of error type) were included in the analysis, for each of the oculomotor tasks (see Additional file 2: Table S2).

Neuropsychological assessment

We have used a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery that was designed to maximize sensitivity to the frontostriatal neural circuitry and cognitive control abilities, and that mainly incorporated widely used executive and memory tests (see Table 2). We aimed at tapping the major cognitive functions known to be affected in the early stages of HD [10, 11, 18, 22, 60, 77]. We computed two main composite scores from this battery—Executive and Memory—to have a baseline depiction of the cognitive abilities involved in the saccadic paradigm created for this study, and to acknowledge any significant differences between the overt cognitive profile of Pre-HD and Control participants that could otherwise explain the potential differences found in their oculomotor behavior.
Table 2
Neuropsychological assessment protocol
Tool
Goal/Assessment domain
Montreal Cognitive Assessment - MoCA [55, 56]
mild cognitive impairment and dementia screening
Stroop Test - Color Naming, Word Reading, and Interference tasks [61, 62]
executive function – cognitive flexibility and processing speed
Edinburgh Handedness Inventory - Portuguese adaptation [63]
handedness definition
Digit Symbol subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III [64, 65]
psychomotor speed and working memory
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test - Portuguese version [66, 67] - total trials 1–5
verbal memory
12-item short form of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices [68]
indication of non-verbal intelligence and to control for individual differences in IQ that are unrelated to illness
Corsi Block-Tapping task [69, 70]
psychomotor speed, working memory and executive functioning - the product of the total number of correct trials and the length of the largest sequence was calculated
Benton Visual Retention test [71]
visual memory
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test - Portuguese version [66, 67]– recall and recognition trials
verbal memory
Benton Visual Form Discrimination test [72]
visual perception
Phonemic Verbal Fluency test: 3 letters – P, M, R [73]
executive function – working memory, word generation and inhibition
Semantic Verbal Fluency test: category – animals [74]
executive function – working memory, word generation and inhibition
Vocabulary of the WAIS-III [64, 65]
indication of verbal intelligence and control for individual differences in intelligence that are unrelated to illness
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Snaith Irritability Scale (HADS-SIS) [75, 76]
psychiatric symptoms and prevalence of depression and anxiety
An Executive Composite Score was computed from six neuropsychological test scores [Stroop word reading test – total correct; Stroop color naming test – total correct; Stroop interference test – total correct; Symbol digit modality test – total correct; Verbal fluency test (letters-PMR) – total correct; Verbal fluency test (category-animals) – total correct]. A Memory Composite Score was computed from six neuropsychological test scores [Benton visual retention test – total correct; Auditory verbal learning test (trials-1-5) – total correct; Auditory verbal learning test (recall) – total correct; Auditory verbal learning test (recognition) – total correct; Corsi block tapping task (direct) – total correct; Corsi block tapping task (inverse) – total correct].
Additionally, we have assessed the global cognitive status, the verbal and non-verbal intelligence level, the visual perception abilities, and the neuropsychiatric symptoms of study participants using standardized measures of these domains.
The neuropsychological battery was administered over a period of one and a half hours, in a strictly prescribed order, to avoid interference problems related to evaluating the same contents or assessing the same domain in several tasks in a row, and to respect the time intervals required by certain tests.

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed with the software IBM SPSS Statistics, version 24, adopting a level of significance of α = 0.05, and only significant results were reported and further debated in the “Results” and “Discussion” sections.
Outliers were excluded from data analysis for each oculomotor parameter in the four saccadic tasks―values below Q1–1.5xIQR and above Q3 + 1.5xIQR (see Additional file 3: Table S3). When comparing the neuropsychological and saccadic performance of Pre-HD and Control groups, ANCOVA statistical analysis was performed with age as a covariate, given that this variable is known to affect cognition and reflexive and voluntary eye movements both in clinical and healthy populations [43, 48, 78, 79]. Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare the demographic variables of the two groups. Comparisons of nominal/categorical variables between groups were performed resorting to Chi-square tests of independence. Wilcoxon-Signed rank tests were used to further examine the effects of task condition (PS, AS, MPS and MAS) in the Pre-HD participants’ saccadic performance. Spearman rank correlation coefficients were calculated to analyze the associations between the performance of the Pre-HD participants in the oculomotor measures where a group difference was found and other clinical, cognitive and oculomotor data of the Pre-HD group. Benjamini–Hochberg corrections with false positive rate established at 0.05 were used to deal with multiple comparisons.

Results

The Pre-HD and CTRL participants enrolled in our study were matched in terms of age, education level, gender, and handedness (see Table 1).

Oculomotor results

The comparison of the saccadic performance of the two groups (see Fig. 2 and Table 3) revealed that alterations of oculomotor performance were present in the clinical group compared to controls, especially in the tasks with higher executive and/or memory load, namely the AS and MAS tasks. In addition, the analysis of the performance of Pre-HD participants across the four saccadic conditions revealed that both accuracy and timing measures reflected the impact of the incremental executive and memory demands of the saccadic tasks (see Additional file 4: Table S4).
Table 3
Comparison of the Oculomotor results of the CTRL and Pre-HD groups across the four saccadic tasks
 
% Successful trials
% Direction errors
% Anticipatory saccade errors
Latency
F
p
F
p
F
p
F
p
PS
(1,30) 3.299
0.079
(1,31) 0.203
0.655
(1,31) 0.124
0.728
(1,32) 0.954
0.336
AS
(1,32) 5.200
0.029*
(1,31) 7.278
0.011*
(1,31) 0.984
0.329
(1,32) 0.132
0.719
MPS
(1,30) 0.081
0.778
(1,30) 0.158
0.694
(1,30) 0.516
0.478
(1,31) 1.186
0.285
MAS
(1,30) 1.287
0.266
(1,28) 5.480
0.027*
(1,27) 1.984
0.170
(1,29) 12.272
0.002*
PS Prosaccade, AS Antisaccade, MPS 1- or 2-back memory Prosaccade, MAS 1- or 2-back memory Antisaccade
*Pre-HD ≠ CTRL (ANCOVA, p ≤ 0.05 – controlling for the effect of age)
For the percentage of successful trials, an important statistically significant difference was found between Pre-HD and CTRL participants in the AS condition (F(1,32) = 5.200, p = 0.029). This result suggests that once an executive load or inhibitory demand was introduced into an otherwise simple reflexive saccadic task, the Pre-HD group started to show an abnormal oculomotor behavior, with a significant decrease in their success rate due to the switch in the protocol.
Notably, for the percentage of direction errors, again a statistically significant difference was found between Pre-HD and CTRL participants in the AS condition (F(1,31) = 7.278, p = 0.011) and in the MAS condition (F(1,28) = 5.480, p = 0.027). These results suggest that, when an executive load is added to the task, either independently or combined with memory demands, the oculomotor performance of Pre-HD participants fails to adapt to the new goal and inhibition deficits emerge.
For the percentage of anticipatory saccade errors, no statistically significant differences were found between Pre-HD and CTRL participants across the four saccadic conditions. These results suggest that both groups exhibit a similar rate of premature saccades along the different task conditions, albeit the reduced accuracy displayed by Pre-HD participants in the more demanding AS and MAS tasks.
Finally, for the primary saccade latency, a statistically significant difference was found between Pre-HD and CTRL participants in the MAS condition, where Pre-HD participants showed a faster saccadic reaction time compared to controls (F(1,29) = 12.272, p = 0.002). These findings suggest that for the premanifest HD participants, latency in the context of the most demanding saccadic condition can illustrate a more automatic response pattern when the task demands increase.
The analysis of the saccadic performance of the Pre-HD participants across the four different task conditions (PS, AS, MPS and MAS) revealed significant effects of the increasing cognitive load in the percentage of successful trials, percentage of direction errors, percentage of anticipatory errors and latency (see Additional file 4: Table S4). Latency and the percentage of directions errors seemed to be particularly sensitive measures for capturing the decremental impact of the increasing executive and memory demands of the oculomotor task on the behavior of premanifest HD participants [all significant differences |Z| ≥ 2.803, p ≤ 0.05]. Interestingly, only the primary saccade latency differed between the AS and MAS oculomotor performance of the Pre-HD group (Z = − 3.059, p = 0.002), which suggests that in saccadic conditions with inhibition demands the behavior of the clinical group is globally similar (and equally compromised), whereas a more automatic response pattern emerges when the task demands increase (MAS task).

Neuropsychological results

No significant differences were found between Pre-HD and CTRL participants in the Executive and Memory Composite scores computed from the neuropsychological battery, nor in any of the other neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric measures used (see Table 4). These results suggest that both groups had a similar cognitive and psychiatric status, as assessed with conventional tests and scales, which might indicate that the oculomotor differences found between the two groups cannot be explained by disparate executive, memory or psychiatric conditions.
Table 4
Neuropsychological test results of the CTRL and Pre-HD groups
 
CTRL
Pre-HD
ANCOVA
Median
IQR
Median
IQR
F
p
Executive Composite Score
321
65
307
74
(1,34) 0.313
0.579
Memory Composite Score
206.5
32
203
61
(1,34) 0.225
0.638
Vocabulary - WAIS III
36.5
15
37
13
(1,34) 0.271
0.606
Raven Matrices
8
2
9
3
(1,34) 0.105
0.748
Montreal Cognitive Assessment
26
3
26
3
(1,34) 0.248
0.622
Benton Visual Form Discrimination
30.5
3
30
4
(1,34) 0.310
0.861
HADS-SIS – Depression
4
5
4
6
(1,34) 0.150
0.700
HADS-SIS – Anxiety
6
7
5
5
(1,34) 0.197
0.660
No significant differences found between Pre-HD and Controls in any of the Neuropsychological Measures
IQR Interquartile Range, HADS-SIS Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Snaith Irritability Scale
Executive Composite Score = Stroop word reading test (total correct) + Stroop color naming test (total correct) + Stroop interference test (total correct) + Symbol digit modality test (total correct) + Verbal fluency test (letters-PMR) (total correct) + Verbal fluency test (category-animals) (total correct)
Memory Composite Score = Benton visual retention test (total correct) + Auditory verbal learning test (trials-1-5) (total correct) + Auditory verbal learning test (recall) (total correct) + Auditory verbal learning test (recognition) (total correct) + Corsi block tapping task (direct) (total correct) + Corsi block tapping task (inverse) (total correct)

Correlational analysis

In the Pre-HD group, the four oculomotor parameters that statistically differed from the CTRL group were significantly correlated with their results in other clinical, cognitive and oculomotor measures included in our study protocol (see Table 5 and Additional file 5: Table S5). Importantly, the percentage of direction errors of the Pre-HD group in the AS and MAS conditions were significantly correlated with the UHDRS-OculoTMS (rs = 0.533, p = 0.049 and rs = 0.609, p = 0.027, respectively), which reflects oculomotor abnormalities detected at neurological examination. Moreover, the Pre-HD primary saccade latency in the MAS condition was significantly correlated with the Time to HD Onset (rs = − 0.620, p = 0.032). Finally, we have found that the memory composite score was significantly associated with the AS percentage of successful trials and the MAS percentage of direction errors in the Pre-HD group (rs = 0.533, p = 0.050 and rs = − 0.660, p = 0.014, respectively). These results suggest that changes in specific oculomotor parameters prior to the onset of clinically relevant motor disturbances are significantly associated with important disease-related features and cognitive skills in premanifest HD individuals. Additionally, the significant associations found between antisaccade trajectory and timing measures in the Pre-HD group indicate that executively demanding oculomotor tasks seem to induce a consistently erroneous and impulsive saccadic behavior in premanifest HD individuals.
Table 5
Correlations between the oculomotor, clinical and cognitive results of the Pre-HD group
Pre-HD
AS
Successful trials
n = 14
AS
Direction errors
n = 14
MAS
Direction errors
n = 13
MAS
Latency
n = 12
rho
p
rho
p
rho
p
rho
p
CAG
−0.089
0.763
0.000
1.000
−0.220
0.469
0.303
0.339
Time to HD Onset (years)
0.420
0.135
−0.310
0.281
−0.014
0.964
−0.620
0.032*
UHDRS-TMS
−0.356
0.212
0.314
0.274
0.284
0.347
−0.096
0.766
UHDRS-OculoTMS
−0.473
0.088
0.533
0.049*
0.609
0.027*
0.096
0.767
Executive Score
0.459
0.099
−0.257
0.375
−0.325
0.279
0.133
0.681
Memory Score
0.533
0.050*
−0.300
0.298
−0.660
0.014*
0.186
0.564
AS Antisaccade, MAS 1- or 2-back memory Antisaccade
CAG repeats CAG repeat expansion confirmed by a genetic test; Time to HD Onset – estimated number of years to the formal diagnosis of manifest HD, UHDRS Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale, TMS Total Motor Score of the UHDRS, OculoTMS a composite score extracted from the sum of the oculomotor items of UHDRS-Motor scale
*Correlation is significant at 0.05 level (two-tailed)

Discussion

The current study addressed the role of saccadic movement parameters, and specifically saccadic inhibition with or without memory and fronto-executive load, as a potential marker of impulsive behavior in premanifest Huntington’s disease. We hypothesized that an oculomotor experiment embedded with a cognitively demanding paradigm [28, 52, 54], aimed at increasing fronto-executive load whilst tapping onto the inhibitory component of saccadic eye movements, could be more sensitive in detecting the earliest HD-related alterations than formerly investigated paradigms [42, 46, 49, 51, 80, 81], conventional cognitive tests that evaluate executive function and working memory [61, 62, 69, 70, 73, 74], and standard clinical evaluation of oculomotor function [59, 81]. Particularly, the influence of frontal-executive load in oculomotor inhibition processes was analyzed in this study and tested as a potential trigger of impulsive response patterns in premanifest HD individuals.
We have found that Pre-HD participants with a similar executive and memory performance in conventional tests to controls, show statistically significant saccadic impairments in an oculomotor paradigm that encloses inhibition and increasing cognitive demands.
The Pre-HD group has shown impairments particularly in oculomotor tasks with an inhibitory component, exhibiting a decreased success rate in the AS task, a higher percentage of direction errors in the AS and MAS tasks, and a reduced response latency in the MAS condition when compared to controls. The timing and trajectory abnormalities shown by the Pre-HD group of our study illustrate the impaired saccade suppression in premanifest HD reported by Anderson & MacAskill [40], the higher incidence of unusually early saccades in premanifest individuals reported by Antoniades et al. [48, 51], and the HD patients’ inability to stifle saccades especially in highly demanding memory and executive tasks reported by Ali et al. [82]. The lower percentage of success rate in the AS task and the reduced saccade latency shown by the premanifest HD group in the MAS task may be interpreted as indicators of more impulsive oculomotor behavior / automatic response pattern due to early impairments in inhibitory control mechanisms. Farrow et al. [83] suggested that in cognitive tasks with increasing executive load, premanifest HD individuals have greater difficulty overcoming the more demanding executive conditions and are more likely to inappropriately make more automatic responses. Our data seems to be in line with this statement—control participants seem to show a stable oculomotor performance along the four different saccadic tasks, increasing their response latency in the more demanding conditions, as part of the strategy to ensure a successful performance and to keep good accuracy levels, whereas Pre-HD participants tend to give more erroneous responses in the tasks with higher executive and memory load, and show a faster saccadic reaction time compared to controls. The changes in saccade timing (latency) observed in the clinical group might represent automatic processes and work as a proxy for the impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits often described in Huntington’s disease. This impulsivity-related response pattern matches the speed-accuracy tradeoff described by Heitz [84] where faster responses entail less accumulated evidence, and hence less informed decisions. This finding is also in line with the study of Vaportzis et al. [85] that reported that HD participants were affected differently than controls with respect to the competing goals of speed and accuracy. Moreover, these results seem to have similarities with the reflection impulsivity attributed to PD patients during rapid decision paradigms [86], that is, a tendency to “jump to conclusions” without gathering sufficient information [36].
Rao et al. [31] claim that response-inhibition failure in premanifest HD is associated with functional changes in inhibitory control, attentional reorienting, and motor-control systems. Because neural degeneration in HD begins in the basal ganglia, and saccadic suppression and inhibitory control mechanisms appear to be affected directly by these changes, measures of saccadic suppression, specifically, may be an effective early indicator of disease onset and impulsivity symptoms in premanifest HD, as response inhibition can serve as a “proxy” for the study of impulsivity and its neurobiological underpinnings [28, 44].
We hypothesized that an oculomotor paradigm with increment of executive and/or working-memory load might be more sensitive to the earliest HD-related changes if tapping onto the inhibition of saccades, since the frontostriatal circuitry is known to be affected one to two decades prior to estimated disease clinical onset [8789]. This is relevant when searching for sensitive and low-cost markers of earliest functional changes due to HD neurodegenerative processes. In contrast to studies in healthy individuals [52, 54] we embedded the n-back memory component in the saccadic task, instead of a separate auditory or visual presentation of letters, respectively. We envisioned this would allow to discard interference from other sensory modalities and to better disentangle impairment in oculomotor inhibition in scope of HD neurodegeneration. Despite former findings of oculomotor alterations in premanifest HD [20, 34, 4144, 46, 4851, 53, 82], our study remains one of the few to have significant results on a sample of Pre-HD participants that are on average far from estimated clinical onset [20, 46, 50]. Furthermore, even though age is known to affect performance of reflexive and voluntary eye movements, both in healthy and clinical populations, previous studies have not controlled systematically for such effects, which might affect the positive results reported. Also, in former studies, the criterion for the categorization of premanifest and manifest HD individuals has been based on subjective confidence ratings (for example, see [49]), and not in a clear and standardized cut-off score as in the UHDRS-Motor scale [59].
At last, the application of pattern classification algorithms to oculomotor data has already shown promising results in differentiating premanifest HD individuals from control participants [90, 91], yet the interpretation of results in view of the dysfunction of inhibitory motor control remains elusive.
Regarding the conventional neuropsychological assessment results, the comparable cognitive baseline performance of the Pre-HD and Control participants in our study is in accordance with previous studies that did not detect differences between the cognitive profile of gene positive and gene negative/healthy control individuals [11, 17, 20, 51, 83, 92]. Even in large sample size studies (e.g., PREDICT-HD and TRACK-HD), the only robust cognitive deficits were detected in individuals that were close to estimated clinical onset (HD symptom presentation) and in the more executive demanding tests [19, 22, 45]. Our sample of Pre-HD participants was composed by individuals that were on average far from estimated clinical onset (73% had 15 or more years to the time of HD clinical diagnosis, according to Langbehn’s formula [58]), which might have had an important impact in our overall results (e.g. small effect sizes). Furthermore, these results suggest that the differences found in oculomotor performance between Pre-HD and CTRL individuals cannot be otherwise explained by the two groups having a distinct overt cognitive baseline.
Finally, the significant correlations found between specific oculomotor parameters and HD clinical and cognitive features reinforce the view that the saccadic behavior of Pre-HD individuals, particularly under more executively demanding conditions, reliably mirrors the often subtle and underestimated cognitive and motor alterations that characterize the premanifest stage of HD, and also gives important information about disease onset and progression. This is in line with former findings in premanifest HD individuals: impaired oculomotor functioning was shown to be associated to worse performance on cognitive tasks [24]; response accuracy in a visual processing task was found to be significantly correlated with an index of disease progression [23]; reaction time in a sequential button pressing task significantly associated with estimated time to disease onset [83]; increased error rates in antisaccade and memory guided saccade tasks were demonstrated to be associated to more abnormalities in the UHDRS motor scale and to a closer estimated disease onset [20, 49]; higher cognitive impairment was shown to be significantly related to increased oculomotor changes [93]; and, antisaccade error rate has been found to increase proportionally with disease progression [94]. Thus, quantitative measures of oculomotor inhibitory control and impulsivity such as the ones computed from the AS and MAS tasks of our study protocol seem to be sensitive indicators of the disease status and progression stage of premanifest HD individuals.
In conclusion, our results indicate that the temporal and spatial properties of oculomotor function in Pre-HD individuals reflect an imbalance between goal oriented and automatic behavior, due to early inhibitory control deficits. Moreover, our data suggest that the failure of the inhibitory control mechanisms that are involved in simple and complex oculomotor responses can induce an impulsive eye movement pattern in otherwise asymptomatic carriers of the genetic mutation that matches the HD executive dysfunction syndrome described by Rosenblatt [26]. Hence, saccadic timing and trajectory measures may be an effective early indicator of disease onset in HD, namely of motor disinhibition and impulsivity signs. Further, the manifestation of timing or spatial deviations in the saccadic behavior of premanifest HD individuals might depend on the task, and the levels of inhibition involved as well as executive load.

Limitations

The small sample size makes it difficult to further subdivide the Pre-HD group into those far and close from estimated clinical onset. Large longitudinal studies like TRACK-HD or PREDICT-HD found the most significant differences between the cognitive performance of asymptomatic HD gene carriers and controls in those participants close to clinical onset [19, 22, 45]; this might indicate that a stratification is necessary if one wants to find robust evidence of cognitive changes in premanifest HD. Moreover, the relatively small sample size enrolled in our study prevents us from being able to generalize our results—further work is essential to validate and replicate our findings in a larger sample. Finally, the fact that significant differences were absent at the level of conventional neuropsychological test results between Pre-HD and CTRL participants leads us to hypothesize that the neuropsychological test battery used, even if extensive, was not sufficiently sensitive to the subtle and earliest changes that occur in HD cognition—subtle changes synonymous of small effect sizes, might need larger samples of premanifest gene carriers for testing novel hypotheses. Also, having a set of more ecological neuropsychological tests would probably help to better distinguish between the Pre-HD and CTRL groups, as cognitive assessment methods that resemble daily-life tasks have proven to be more successful at differentiating premanifest HD individuals far from estimated disease onset and controls [95, 96].

Conclusion

Our saccadic task results suggest that the performance of Pre-HD individuals deteriorates when a fronto-executive or/and memory load is added to the task. Moreover, the Pre-HD group appears to have deficits in goal-oriented oculomotor behavior—more automatic responses or impulsivity at the cost of timed-strategy for accurate decision making. Our findings also suggest that specific horizontal saccadic parameters that enclose inhibition and memory demands seem to be accurate indicators of disease-related features in premanifest HD individuals. Hence, measures of inhibitory control mechanisms in the context of eye movement paradigms may provide sensitive markers of clinical disease onset in Huntington’s disease and help understand the neurobehavioral underpinnings of impulsivity as a trait of HD phenotype. Lastly, new quantitative tools that are able to detect the earliest disease-related changes and provide information about premanifest HD subtle signs and symptoms are thought to be extremely relevant for the design and implementation of interventional strategies aimed at delaying the onset or progression of Huntington’s disease.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13023-019-1218-y.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the study participants and Associação Portuguesa dos Doentes de Huntington (APDH) for the support in participant’s recruitment and the Laboratory of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Coimbra (LBIM).
All study procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, and an informed consent was signed by participants prior to study participation.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
2.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Dumas EM, van den Bogaard SJ, Middelkoop HA, Roos RA. A review of cognition in Huntington’s disease. Front Biosci (Schol Ed). 2013;5:1–18.CrossRef Dumas EM, van den Bogaard SJ, Middelkoop HA, Roos RA. A review of cognition in Huntington’s disease. Front Biosci (Schol Ed). 2013;5:1–18.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Keeffe GC, Michell AW, Barker RA. Biomarkers in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1180:97–110.CrossRefPubMed O’Keeffe GC, Michell AW, Barker RA. Biomarkers in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1180:97–110.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Weir DW, Sturrock A, Leavitt BR. Development of biomarkers for Huntington's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(6):573–90.CrossRefPubMed Weir DW, Sturrock A, Leavitt BR. Development of biomarkers for Huntington's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(6):573–90.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Diamond R, White RF, Myers RH, Mastromauro C, Koroshetz WJ, Butters N, et al. Evidence of presymptomatic cognitive decline in Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14(6):961–75.CrossRefPubMed Diamond R, White RF, Myers RH, Mastromauro C, Koroshetz WJ, Butters N, et al. Evidence of presymptomatic cognitive decline in Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14(6):961–75.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Harrington DL, Smith MM, Zhang Y, Carlozzi NE, Paulsen JS. Group tP-HIotHS. Cognitive domains that predict time to diagnosis in prodromal Huntington disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(6):612–9.CrossRefPubMed Harrington DL, Smith MM, Zhang Y, Carlozzi NE, Paulsen JS. Group tP-HIotHS. Cognitive domains that predict time to diagnosis in prodromal Huntington disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(6):612–9.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ross CA, Aylward EH, Wild EJ, Langbehn DR, Long JD, Warner JH, et al. Huntington disease: natural history, biomarkers and prospects for therapeutics. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10(4):204–16.CrossRefPubMed Ross CA, Aylward EH, Wild EJ, Langbehn DR, Long JD, Warner JH, et al. Huntington disease: natural history, biomarkers and prospects for therapeutics. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10(4):204–16.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Stout JC, Paulsen JS, Queller S, Solomon AC, Whitlock KB, Campbell JC, et al. Neurocognitive signs in prodromal Huntington disease. Neuropsychology. 2011;25(1):1–14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Stout JC, Paulsen JS, Queller S, Solomon AC, Whitlock KB, Campbell JC, et al. Neurocognitive signs in prodromal Huntington disease. Neuropsychology. 2011;25(1):1–14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Solomon AC, Stout JC, Weaver M, Queller S, Tomusk A, Whitlock KB, et al. Ten-year rate of longitudinal change in neurocognitive and motor function in prediagnosis Huntington disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23(13):1830–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Solomon AC, Stout JC, Weaver M, Queller S, Tomusk A, Whitlock KB, et al. Ten-year rate of longitudinal change in neurocognitive and motor function in prediagnosis Huntington disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23(13):1830–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Novak MJ, Tabrizi SJ. Huntington’s disease: clinical presentation and treatment. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2011;98:297–323.CrossRefPubMed Novak MJ, Tabrizi SJ. Huntington’s disease: clinical presentation and treatment. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2011;98:297–323.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat van Asselen M, Almeida I, Júlio F, Januário C, Campos EB, Simões M, et al. Implicit contextual learning in prodromal and early stage Huntington’s disease patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(4):689–96.CrossRefPubMed van Asselen M, Almeida I, Júlio F, Januário C, Campos EB, Simões M, et al. Implicit contextual learning in prodromal and early stage Huntington’s disease patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(4):689–96.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat van Asselen M, Júlio F, Januário C, Bobrowicz Campos E, Almeida I, Cavaco S, et al. Scanning patterns of faces do not explain impaired emotion recognition in Huntington disease: evidence for a high level mechanism. Front Psychol. 2012;3:31. van Asselen M, Júlio F, Januário C, Bobrowicz Campos E, Almeida I, Cavaco S, et al. Scanning patterns of faces do not explain impaired emotion recognition in Huntington disease: evidence for a high level mechanism. Front Psychol. 2012;3:31.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Hart EP, Dumas EM, Schoonderbeek A, Wolthuis SC, van Zwet EW, Roos RA. Motor dysfunction influence on executive functioning in manifest and premanifest Huntington’s disease. Mov Disord. 2014;29(3):320–6.CrossRefPubMed Hart EP, Dumas EM, Schoonderbeek A, Wolthuis SC, van Zwet EW, Roos RA. Motor dysfunction influence on executive functioning in manifest and premanifest Huntington’s disease. Mov Disord. 2014;29(3):320–6.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Papoutsi M, Labuschagne I, Tabrizi SJ, Stout JC. The cognitive burden in Huntington’s disease: pathology, phenotype, and mechanisms of compensation. Mov Disord. 2014;29(5):673–83.CrossRefPubMed Papoutsi M, Labuschagne I, Tabrizi SJ, Stout JC. The cognitive burden in Huntington’s disease: pathology, phenotype, and mechanisms of compensation. Mov Disord. 2014;29(5):673–83.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Papp KV, Kaplan RF, Snyder PJ. Biological markers of cognition in prodromal Huntington’s disease: a review. Brain Cognit. 2011;77(2):280–91.CrossRef Papp KV, Kaplan RF, Snyder PJ. Biological markers of cognition in prodromal Huntington’s disease: a review. Brain Cognit. 2011;77(2):280–91.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulsen JS. Early detection of Huntington disease. Future Neurol. 2010;5(1):85–104.CrossRef Paulsen JS. Early detection of Huntington disease. Future Neurol. 2010;5(1):85–104.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Rupp J, Blekher T, Jackson J, Beristain X, Marshall J, Hui S, et al. Progression in prediagnostic Huntington disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81(4):379–84.CrossRefPubMed Rupp J, Blekher T, Jackson J, Beristain X, Marshall J, Hui S, et al. Progression in prediagnostic Huntington disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81(4):379–84.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Stout JC, Queller S, Baker KN, Cowlishaw S, Sampaio C, Fitzer-Attas C, et al. HD-CAB: a cognitive assessment battery for clinical trials in Huntington’s disease1,2,3. Mov Disord. 2014;29(10):1281–8.CrossRefPubMed Stout JC, Queller S, Baker KN, Cowlishaw S, Sampaio C, Fitzer-Attas C, et al. HD-CAB: a cognitive assessment battery for clinical trials in Huntington’s disease1,2,3. Mov Disord. 2014;29(10):1281–8.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabrizi SJ, Scahill RI, Owen G, Durr A, Leavitt BR, Roos RA, et al. Predictors of phenotypic progression and disease onset in premanifest and early-stage Huntington's disease in the TRACK-HD study: analysis of 36-month observational data. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(7):637–49.CrossRefPubMed Tabrizi SJ, Scahill RI, Owen G, Durr A, Leavitt BR, Roos RA, et al. Predictors of phenotypic progression and disease onset in premanifest and early-stage Huntington's disease in the TRACK-HD study: analysis of 36-month observational data. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(7):637–49.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Nasr S, Rosas H. Impact of Huntington’s disease on mental rotation performance in motor pre-symptomatic individuals. J Huntington’s Dis. 2019;8(3):339–56.CrossRef Nasr S, Rosas H. Impact of Huntington’s disease on mental rotation performance in motor pre-symptomatic individuals. J Huntington’s Dis. 2019;8(3):339–56.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Carvalho JO, Long JD, Westervelt HJ, Smith MM, Bruce JM, Kim J-I, et al. The impact of oculomotor functioning on neuropsychological performance in Huntington disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2016;38(2):217–26.CrossRef Carvalho JO, Long JD, Westervelt HJ, Smith MM, Bruce JM, Kim J-I, et al. The impact of oculomotor functioning on neuropsychological performance in Huntington disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2016;38(2):217–26.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Coppen EM, van der Grond J, Hart EP, Lakke EAJF, Roos RAC. The visual cortex and visual cognition in Huntington’s disease: an overview of current literature. Behav Brain Res. 2018;351:63–74.CrossRefPubMed Coppen EM, van der Grond J, Hart EP, Lakke EAJF, Roos RAC. The visual cortex and visual cognition in Huntington’s disease: an overview of current literature. Behav Brain Res. 2018;351:63–74.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Balci F, Day M, Rooney A, Brunner D. Disrupted temporal control in the R6/2 mouse model of Huntington's disease. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123(6):1353–8.CrossRefPubMed Balci F, Day M, Rooney A, Brunner D. Disrupted temporal control in the R6/2 mouse model of Huntington's disease. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123(6):1353–8.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79.CrossRefPubMed Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69(4):680–94.CrossRefPubMed Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69(4):680–94.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosas HD, Hevelone ND, Zaleta AK, Greve DN, Salat DH, Fischl B. Regional cortical thinning in preclinical Huntington disease and its relationship to cognition. Neurology. 2005;65(5):745–7.CrossRefPubMed Rosas HD, Hevelone ND, Zaleta AK, Greve DN, Salat DH, Fischl B. Regional cortical thinning in preclinical Huntington disease and its relationship to cognition. Neurology. 2005;65(5):745–7.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Rao JA, Harrington DL, Durgerian S, Reece C, Mourany L, Koenig K, et al. Disruption of response inhibition circuits in prodromal Huntington disease. Cortex. 2014;58:72–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rao JA, Harrington DL, Durgerian S, Reece C, Mourany L, Koenig K, et al. Disruption of response inhibition circuits in prodromal Huntington disease. Cortex. 2014;58:72–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Duff K, Paulsen JS, Beglinger LJ, Langbehn DR, Stout JC. Psychiatric symptoms in Huntington’s disease before diagnosis: the predict-HD study. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(12):1341–6.CrossRefPubMed Duff K, Paulsen JS, Beglinger LJ, Langbehn DR, Stout JC. Psychiatric symptoms in Huntington’s disease before diagnosis: the predict-HD study. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(12):1341–6.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Gorges M, Pinkhardt EH, Kassubek J. Alterations of eye movement control in neurodegenerative movement disorders. J Ophthalmol. 2014;2014:11. Gorges M, Pinkhardt EH, Kassubek J. Alterations of eye movement control in neurodegenerative movement disorders. J Ophthalmol. 2014;2014:11.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Henderson T, Georgiou-Karistianis N, White O, Millist L, Williams DR, Churchyard A, et al. Inhibitory control during smooth pursuit in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington's disease. Mov Disord. 2011;26(10):1893–9.CrossRefPubMed Henderson T, Georgiou-Karistianis N, White O, Millist L, Williams DR, Churchyard A, et al. Inhibitory control during smooth pursuit in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington's disease. Mov Disord. 2011;26(10):1893–9.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Manfré G, Doyere V, Bossi S, Riess O, Nguyen HP, El Massioui N. Impulsivity trait in the early symptomatic BACHD transgenic rat model of Huntington disease. Behav Brain Res. 2016;299:6–10.CrossRefPubMed Manfré G, Doyere V, Bossi S, Riess O, Nguyen HP, El Massioui N. Impulsivity trait in the early symptomatic BACHD transgenic rat model of Huntington disease. Behav Brain Res. 2016;299:6–10.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang J, Nombela C, Wolpe N, Barker RA, Rowe JB. Time on timing: dissociating premature responding from interval sensitivity in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31(8):1163–72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Zhang J, Nombela C, Wolpe N, Barker RA, Rowe JB. Time on timing: dissociating premature responding from interval sensitivity in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31(8):1163–72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang J, Rittman T, Nombela C, Fois A, Coyle-Gilchrist I, Barker RA, et al. Different decision deficits impair response inhibition in progressive supranuclear palsy and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2016;139(1):161–73.CrossRefPubMed Zhang J, Rittman T, Nombela C, Fois A, Coyle-Gilchrist I, Barker RA, et al. Different decision deficits impair response inhibition in progressive supranuclear palsy and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2016;139(1):161–73.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayard S, Joly E, Ghisletta P, Rossignol A, Herades Y, Geny C, et al. A multidimensional approach to impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease: measurement and structural invariance of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale. Psychol Med. 2016;46(14):2931–41.CrossRefPubMed Bayard S, Joly E, Ghisletta P, Rossignol A, Herades Y, Geny C, et al. A multidimensional approach to impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease: measurement and structural invariance of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale. Psychol Med. 2016;46(14):2931–41.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Vela L, Martínez Castrillo JC, García Ruiz P, Gasca-Salas C, Macías Macías Y, Pérez Fernández E, et al. The high prevalence of impulse control behaviors in patients with early-onset Parkinson's disease: a cross-sectional multicenter study. J Neurol Sci. 2016;368:150–4.CrossRefPubMed Vela L, Martínez Castrillo JC, García Ruiz P, Gasca-Salas C, Macías Macías Y, Pérez Fernández E, et al. The high prevalence of impulse control behaviors in patients with early-onset Parkinson's disease: a cross-sectional multicenter study. J Neurol Sci. 2016;368:150–4.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson TJ, MacAskill MR. Eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2013;9(2):74–85.CrossRefPubMed Anderson TJ, MacAskill MR. Eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2013;9(2):74–85.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Blekher T, Yee RD, Kirkwood SC, Hake AM, Stout JC, Weaver MR, et al. Oculomotor control in asymptomatic and recently diagnosed individuals with the genetic marker for Huntington's disease. Vis Res. 2004;44(23):2729–36.CrossRefPubMed Blekher T, Yee RD, Kirkwood SC, Hake AM, Stout JC, Weaver MR, et al. Oculomotor control in asymptomatic and recently diagnosed individuals with the genetic marker for Huntington's disease. Vis Res. 2004;44(23):2729–36.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Golding CV, Danchaivijitr C, Hodgson TL, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Identification of an oculomotor biomarker of preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology. 2006;67(3):485–7.CrossRefPubMed Golding CV, Danchaivijitr C, Hodgson TL, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Identification of an oculomotor biomarker of preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology. 2006;67(3):485–7.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Patel SS, Jankovic J, Hood AJ, Jeter CB, Sereno AB. Reflexive and volitional saccades: biomarkers of Huntington disease severity and progression. J Neurol Sci. 2012;313(1–2):35–41.CrossRefPubMed Patel SS, Jankovic J, Hood AJ, Jeter CB, Sereno AB. Reflexive and volitional saccades: biomarkers of Huntington disease severity and progression. J Neurol Sci. 2012;313(1–2):35–41.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Peltsch A, Hoffman A, Armstrong I, Pari G, Munoz DP. Saccadic impairments in Huntington’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2008;186(3):457–69.CrossRefPubMed Peltsch A, Hoffman A, Armstrong I, Pari G, Munoz DP. Saccadic impairments in Huntington’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2008;186(3):457–69.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabrizi SJ, Reilmann R, Roos RAC, Durr A, Leavitt B, Owen G, et al. Potential endpoints for clinical trials in premanifest and early Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: analysis of 24 month observational data. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(1):42–53.CrossRefPubMed Tabrizi SJ, Reilmann R, Roos RAC, Durr A, Leavitt B, Owen G, et al. Potential endpoints for clinical trials in premanifest and early Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: analysis of 24 month observational data. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(1):42–53.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Turner TH, Goldstein J, Hamilton JM, Jacobson M, Pirogovsky E, Peavy G, et al. Behavioral measures of saccade latency and inhibition in manifest and premanifest Huntington’s disease. J Mot Behav. 2011;43(4):295–302.CrossRefPubMed Turner TH, Goldstein J, Hamilton JM, Jacobson M, Pirogovsky E, Peavy G, et al. Behavioral measures of saccade latency and inhibition in manifest and premanifest Huntington’s disease. J Mot Behav. 2011;43(4):295–302.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Vaca-Palomares I, Brien DC, Coe BC, Ochoa-Morales A, Martínez-Ruano L, Munoz DP, et al. Implicit learning impairment identified via predictive saccades in Huntington's disease correlates with extended cortico-striatal atrophy. Cortex. 2019;121:89–103.CrossRefPubMed Vaca-Palomares I, Brien DC, Coe BC, Ochoa-Morales A, Martínez-Ruano L, Munoz DP, et al. Implicit learning impairment identified via predictive saccades in Huntington's disease correlates with extended cortico-striatal atrophy. Cortex. 2019;121:89–103.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Antoniades CA, Xu Z, Mason SL, Carpenter RH, Barker RA. Huntington’s disease: changes in saccades and hand-tapping over 3 years. J Neurol. 2010;257(11):1890–8.CrossRefPubMed Antoniades CA, Xu Z, Mason SL, Carpenter RH, Barker RA. Huntington’s disease: changes in saccades and hand-tapping over 3 years. J Neurol. 2010;257(11):1890–8.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Blekher T, Johnson SA, Marshall J, White K, Hui S, Weaver M, et al. Saccades in presymptomatic and early stages of Huntington disease. Neurology. 2006;67(3):394–9.CrossRefPubMed Blekher T, Johnson SA, Marshall J, White K, Hui S, Weaver M, et al. Saccades in presymptomatic and early stages of Huntington disease. Neurology. 2006;67(3):394–9.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Rupp J, Dzemidzic M, Blekher T, West J, Hui S, Wojcieszek J, et al. Comparison of vertical and horizontal saccade measures and their relation to gray matter changes in premanifest and manifest Huntington disease. J Neurol. 2012;259(2):267–76.CrossRefPubMed Rupp J, Dzemidzic M, Blekher T, West J, Hui S, Wojcieszek J, et al. Comparison of vertical and horizontal saccade measures and their relation to gray matter changes in premanifest and manifest Huntington disease. J Neurol. 2012;259(2):267–76.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Antoniades CA, Altham PM, Mason SL, Barker RA, Carpenter R. Saccadometry: a new tool for evaluating presymptomatic Huntington patients. Neuroreport. 2007;18(11):1133–6.CrossRefPubMed Antoniades CA, Altham PM, Mason SL, Barker RA, Carpenter R. Saccadometry: a new tool for evaluating presymptomatic Huntington patients. Neuroreport. 2007;18(11):1133–6.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Gilchrist ID. Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2002;14(1):95–103.CrossRef Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Gilchrist ID. Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2002;14(1):95–103.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Robert MPA, Nachev PC, Hicks SL, Golding CVP, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Saccadometry of conditional rules in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1164(1):444–50.CrossRefPubMed Robert MPA, Nachev PC, Hicks SL, Golding CVP, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Saccadometry of conditional rules in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1164(1):444–50.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Stigchel S. The search for oculomotor inhibition: interactions with working memory. Exp Psychol. 2010;57(6):429–35.CrossRefPubMed Van der Stigchel S. The search for oculomotor inhibition: interactions with working memory. Exp Psychol. 2010;57(6):429–35.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–9.CrossRefPubMed Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–9.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Simões MR, Freitas S, Santana I, Firmino H, Martins C, Nasreddine Z, et al. MoCA. Versão final portuguesa [MoCA 7.1. Portuguese final version]. 2008. Simões MR, Freitas S, Santana I, Firmino H, Martins C, Nasreddine Z, et al. MoCA. Versão final portuguesa [MoCA 7.1. Portuguese final version]. 2008.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): normative study for the Portuguese population. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2011;33(9):989–96.CrossRef Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): normative study for the Portuguese population. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2011;33(9):989–96.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Langbehn DR, Brinkman RR, Falush D, Paulsen JS, Hayden MR, International Huntington’s Disease Collaborative G. A new model for prediction of the age of onset and penetrance for Huntington's disease based on CAG length. Clin Genet. 2004;65(4):267–77.CrossRefPubMed Langbehn DR, Brinkman RR, Falush D, Paulsen JS, Hayden MR, International Huntington’s Disease Collaborative G. A new model for prediction of the age of onset and penetrance for Huntington's disease based on CAG length. Clin Genet. 2004;65(4):267–77.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Huntington-Study-Group. Unified Huntington’s disease rating scale: reliability and consistency. Mov Disord. 1996;11(2):136–42.CrossRef Huntington-Study-Group. Unified Huntington’s disease rating scale: reliability and consistency. Mov Disord. 1996;11(2):136–42.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Orth M, Handley OJ, Schwenke C, Dunnett SB, Craufurd D, Ho AK, et al. Observing Huntington’s disease: the European Huntington’s disease network’s REGISTRY. PLoS Curr Huntington Dis. 2010;2:RRN1184.CrossRefPubMedCentral Orth M, Handley OJ, Schwenke C, Dunnett SB, Craufurd D, Ho AK, et al. Observing Huntington’s disease: the European Huntington’s disease network’s REGISTRY. PLoS Curr Huntington Dis. 2010;2:RRN1184.CrossRefPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.CrossRef Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Trenerry MR, Crosson B, DeBoe J, Lebel WR. Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test (SNTC). USA: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989. Trenerry MR, Crosson B, DeBoe J, Lebel WR. Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test (SNTC). USA: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. Wechsler D. WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Escala de Inteligência de Wechsler para adultos - Manual. 3rd ed. Portuguese Version [Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - WAIS-III]. Lisbon: Cegoc; 2008. Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Escala de Inteligência de Wechsler para adultos - Manual. 3rd ed. Portuguese Version [Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - WAIS-III]. Lisbon: Cegoc; 2008.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Cavaco S, Pinto C, Gonçalves A, Gomes F, Pereira A, Malaquias C. Auditory verbal learning test: dados normativos dos 21 aos 65 anos. Psychologica. 2008;49:208–21. Cavaco S, Pinto C, Gonçalves A, Gomes F, Pereira A, Malaquias C. Auditory verbal learning test: dados normativos dos 21 aos 65 anos. Psychologica. 2008;49:208–21.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Rey A. Rey verbal learning test. L’examen clinique en psychologie. 2nd ed. Paris: Presses universitaires de France; 1964. Rey A. Rey verbal learning test. L’examen clinique en psychologie. 2nd ed. Paris: Presses universitaires de France; 1964.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Sections 1–7. 1993 ed. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1993. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Sections 1–7. 1993 ed. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1993.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Berch DB, Krikorian R, Huha EM. The Corsi block-tapping task: methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cognit. 1998;38(3):317–38.CrossRef Berch DB, Krikorian R, Huha EM. The Corsi block-tapping task: methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cognit. 1998;38(3):317–38.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Kessels RP, van Zandvoort MJ, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EH. The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7(4):252–8.CrossRefPubMed Kessels RP, van Zandvoort MJ, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EH. The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7(4):252–8.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Benton AL. The revised visual retention test : clinical and experimental applications. 4th ed. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974. Benton AL. The revised visual retention test : clinical and experimental applications. 4th ed. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Benton AL, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: a clinical manual. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983. Benton AL, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: a clinical manual. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solís F, Bernal B. Cognitive testing toward the future: the example of Semantic Verbal Fluency (ANIMALS). Int J Psychol. 2006;41(5):324–32.CrossRef Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solís F, Bernal B. Cognitive testing toward the future: the example of Semantic Verbal Fluency (ANIMALS). Int J Psychol. 2006;41(5):324–32.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Snaith RP, Constantopoulos AA, Jardine MY, McGuffin P. A clinical scale for the self-assessment of irritability. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;132(2):164–71.CrossRefPubMed Snaith RP, Constantopoulos AA, Jardine MY, McGuffin P. A clinical scale for the self-assessment of irritability. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;132(2):164–71.CrossRefPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70.CrossRefPubMed Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Snowden JS, Craufurd D, Thompson J, Neary D. Psychomotor, executive, and memory function in preclinical Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2002;24(2):133–45.CrossRef Snowden JS, Craufurd D, Thompson J, Neary D. Psychomotor, executive, and memory function in preclinical Huntington’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2002;24(2):133–45.CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Butler KM, Zacks RT, Henderson JM. Suppression of reflexive saccades in younger and older adults: age comparisons on an antisaccade task. Mem Cogn. 1999;27(4):584–91.CrossRef Butler KM, Zacks RT, Henderson JM. Suppression of reflexive saccades in younger and older adults: age comparisons on an antisaccade task. Mem Cogn. 1999;27(4):584–91.CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Robert MPA, Nachev PC, Hicks SL, Golding CVP, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Saccadometry of conditional rules in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Basic Clin Aspects Vertigo Dizziness. 2009;1164:444–50. Robert MPA, Nachev PC, Hicks SL, Golding CVP, Tabrizi SJ, Kennard C. Saccadometry of conditional rules in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Basic Clin Aspects Vertigo Dizziness. 2009;1164:444–50.
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Winder JY, Roos RAC. Premanifest Huntington's disease: examination of oculomotor abnormalities in clinical practice. PLoS One. 2018;13(3):e0193866.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Winder JY, Roos RAC. Premanifest Huntington's disease: examination of oculomotor abnormalities in clinical practice. PLoS One. 2018;13(3):e0193866.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Ali FR, Michell AW, Barker RA, Carpenter RHS. The use of quantitative oculometry in the assessment of Huntington’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2006;169(2):237–45.CrossRefPubMed Ali FR, Michell AW, Barker RA, Carpenter RHS. The use of quantitative oculometry in the assessment of Huntington’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2006;169(2):237–45.CrossRefPubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Farrow M, Chua P, Churchyard A, Bradshaw JL, Chiu E, Georgiou-Karistianis N. Proximity to clinical onset influences motor and cognitive performance in presymptomatic Huntington disease gene carriers. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19(4):208–16.CrossRefPubMed Farrow M, Chua P, Churchyard A, Bradshaw JL, Chiu E, Georgiou-Karistianis N. Proximity to clinical onset influences motor and cognitive performance in presymptomatic Huntington disease gene carriers. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19(4):208–16.CrossRefPubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Vaportzis E, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Churchyard A, Stout JC. Dual task performance in Huntington’s disease: a comparison of choice reaction time tasks. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(5):703–12.CrossRefPubMed Vaportzis E, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Churchyard A, Stout JC. Dual task performance in Huntington’s disease: a comparison of choice reaction time tasks. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(5):703–12.CrossRefPubMed
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Kagan J, Pearson L, Welch L. Conceptual impulsivity and inductive reasoning. Child Dev. 1966;37(3):583–94.PubMed Kagan J, Pearson L, Welch L. Conceptual impulsivity and inductive reasoning. Child Dev. 1966;37(3):583–94.PubMed
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulsen JS, Langbehn DR, Stout JC, Aylward E, Ross CA, Nance M, et al. Detection of Huntington’s disease decades before diagnosis: the predict-HD study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79(8):874–80.CrossRefPubMed Paulsen JS, Langbehn DR, Stout JC, Aylward E, Ross CA, Nance M, et al. Detection of Huntington’s disease decades before diagnosis: the predict-HD study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79(8):874–80.CrossRefPubMed
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosas HD, Tuch DS, Hevelone ND, Zaleta AK, Vangel M, Hersch SM, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in presymptomatic and early Huntington’s disease: selective white matter pathology and its relationship to clinical measures. Mov Disord. 2006;21(9):1317–25.CrossRefPubMed Rosas HD, Tuch DS, Hevelone ND, Zaleta AK, Vangel M, Hersch SM, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in presymptomatic and early Huntington’s disease: selective white matter pathology and its relationship to clinical measures. Mov Disord. 2006;21(9):1317–25.CrossRefPubMed
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Gomez-Tortosa E, MacDonald ME, Friend JC, Taylor SA, Weiler LJ, Cupples LA, et al. Quantitative neuropathological changes in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2001;49(1):29–34.CrossRefPubMed Gomez-Tortosa E, MacDonald ME, Friend JC, Taylor SA, Weiler LJ, Cupples LA, et al. Quantitative neuropathological changes in presymptomatic Huntington’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2001;49(1):29–34.CrossRefPubMed
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Miranda Â, Lavrador R, Júlio F, Januário C, Castelo-Branco M, Caetano G. Classification of Huntington’s disease stage with support vector machines: a study on oculomotor performance. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48(4):1667–77.CrossRefPubMed Miranda Â, Lavrador R, Júlio F, Januário C, Castelo-Branco M, Caetano G. Classification of Huntington’s disease stage with support vector machines: a study on oculomotor performance. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48(4):1667–77.CrossRefPubMed
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiecki TV, Antoniades CA, Stevenson A, Kennard C, Borowsky B, Owen G, et al. A computational cognitive biomarker for early-stage Huntington’s disease. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):e0148409.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wiecki TV, Antoniades CA, Stevenson A, Kennard C, Borowsky B, Owen G, et al. A computational cognitive biomarker for early-stage Huntington’s disease. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):e0148409.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Witjes-Ané M-NW, Vegter-van der Vlis M, van Vugt JPP, Lanser JBK, Hermans J, Zwinderman AH, et al. Cognitive and motor functioning in gene carriers for Huntington’s disease: a baseline study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;15(1):7–16.CrossRefPubMed Witjes-Ané M-NW, Vegter-van der Vlis M, van Vugt JPP, Lanser JBK, Hermans J, Zwinderman AH, et al. Cognitive and motor functioning in gene carriers for Huntington’s disease: a baseline study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;15(1):7–16.CrossRefPubMed
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Biglan KM, Zhang Y, Long JD, Geschwind M, Kang GA, Killoran A, et al. Refining the diagnosis of Huntington disease: the PREDICT-HD study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5:12.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Biglan KM, Zhang Y, Long JD, Geschwind M, Kang GA, Killoran A, et al. Refining the diagnosis of Huntington disease: the PREDICT-HD study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5:12.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabrizi SJ, Langbehn DR, Leavitt BR, Roos RAC, Durr A, Craufurd D, et al. Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington’s disease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: cross-sectional analysis of baseline data. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(9):791–801.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tabrizi SJ, Langbehn DR, Leavitt BR, Roos RAC, Durr A, Craufurd D, et al. Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington’s disease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: cross-sectional analysis of baseline data. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(9):791–801.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Júlio F, Ribeiro MJ, Patrício M, Malhão A, Pedrosa F, Gonçalves H, et al. A novel ecological approach reveals early executive function impairments in Huntington’s disease. Front Psychol. 2019;10:585.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Júlio F, Ribeiro MJ, Patrício M, Malhão A, Pedrosa F, Gonçalves H, et al. A novel ecological approach reveals early executive function impairments in Huntington’s disease. Front Psychol. 2019;10:585.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Stout JC, Glikmann-Johnston Y, Andrews SC. Cognitive assessment strategies in Huntington’s disease research. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;265:19–24.CrossRefPubMed Stout JC, Glikmann-Johnston Y, Andrews SC. Cognitive assessment strategies in Huntington’s disease research. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;265:19–24.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
The effect of impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits in the saccadic behavior of premanifest Huntington’s disease individuals
verfasst von
Filipa Júlio
Gina Caetano
Cristina Januário
Miguel Castelo-Branco
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1750-1172
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-019-1218-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe