Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research article

The efficacy of nudge theory strategies in influencing adult dietary behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis

verfasst von: Anneliese Arno, Steve Thomas

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Obesity has become a world-wide epidemic and is spreading to countries with emerging economies. Previously tested interventions are often too costly to maintain in the long term. This leaves a need for improved strategies for management of the epidemic. Nudge Theory presents a new collection of methods, deemed “nudges”, which have the potential for low-cost and broad application to guide healthier lifestyle choices without the need for restrictive regulation. There has not yet been a large-scale examination of the effectiveness of nudges, despite several policy making bodies now considering their use.

Methods

To address this gap in knowledge, an adapted systematic review methodology was used to collect and consolidate results from current Nudge papers and to determine whether Nudge strategies are successful in changing adults’ dietary choices for healthier ones.

Results

It was found that nudges resulted in an average 15.3 % increase in healthier dietary or nutritional choices, as measured by a change in frequency of healthy choices or a change in overall caloric consumption. All of the included studies were from wealthy nations, with a particular emphasis on the United States with 31 of 42 included experiments.

Conclusions

This analysis demonstrates Nudge holds promise as a public health strategy to combat obesity. More research is needed in varied settings, however, and future studies should aim to replicate previous results in more geographically and socioeconomically diverse countries.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s12889-016-3272-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Background

Despite widespread education and healthy eating campaigns, the prevalence of excessive body weight remains stubbornly high in many countries such as the United States, the United Kingdom, and Mexico while steadily climbing in others, including India and China [1].
From these climbing rates it would appear that most interventions previously attempted by public health policy are insufficiently effective. While there are several intensive interventions which have shown success in altering individuals’ body-mass indices (BMI) as well as their nutritional choices, these are largely short-term successes [2]. Moreover, they require massive time and monetary resources for each individual targeted. Many only function at a small scale, in isolated and easily tracked communities. Hence, these previously tested interventions are highly inefficient and would be too costly to implement at a national or population level [3]. Moreover, a less costly population-level intervention would enable under-resourced government bodies an affordable option, and encourage better health equity in the long term.
This situation demands that public health practitioners seek alternative strategies and interventions, in particular, those which could be applied at a population level and are a better value for public spending. From a public health standpoint, people are not generally making good decisions for their own health, or indeed for the health of society at large. It seems that individuals “irrationally” choose to ignore health warnings about obesity and recommendations for their nutritional choices, forming the basis of the obesity issue. Despite their irrational behaviour, predictable patterns may nonetheless be discernible when examined carefully. If research can reveal a method to use these patterns to encourage people to tend towards healthier options rather than the short-term pleasurable choices, a novel and powerful strategy to fight obesity may be established. Importantly, such a strategy would improve population health without coercion or restrictive legislation, instead using the sub-conscious processes present in all individual choices to gain a favourable outcome.

Nudge theory

These assertions are the central tenets of Nudge Theory, a new collection of ideas put forth by Thaler and Sunstein in their 2008 book, Nudge: Improving decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness [4]. The authors contend that there exists a “choice architecture” which involves all of the outside forces that may subtly guide one’s decisions in one direction or another. Given the unavoidable existence of a choice architecture, the crucial next assumption is that a choice architect exists as well: a person or collection of persons who design the environment in order to make a certain option more likely to be chosen. For instance, items placed at eye-level in a supermarket may be selected more frequently than those near the floor. Another example might be manipulations to perception of serving sizes by using smaller serving utensils.
This benignly intentioned manipulation is labelled as “libertarian paternalism”, meant to improve the directions of people’s choices while maintaining freedom of choice. With this approach, they aim to reap the benefits of strong governmental oversight without the restrictive regulations and negative consequences. Nudge and libertarian paternalism are not necessarily new ideas, but the collation of these ideas within one umbrella term is a novel field. Consequently, not much work has been done in order to test the efficacy of adjustments in the choice architecture. Most of the previous work around these ideas has been speculative, and the field has been dominated by an ongoing debate about the morality of libertarian paternalism, which some consider to be an infringement of individual freedom of choice (e.g., [5, 6]). This focus on the ethics has left a gap in the literature as to whether or not nudge is an effective strategy for combatting obesity. Nudge previously has been examined in individual studies for its capacity to positively influence several other behaviours, including decreasing tobacco-use, increasing physical exercise, and encouraging financial planning, as described in [7]. However, it has not been systematically and quantitatively assessed for its effectiveness.
Much of the previous experimentation that would be classified as Nudge today was thus not originally published under this term, despite the fact that the interventions used would indeed fall under that category. Nevertheless, as governments have begun to use Nudge Theory strategies as a public health policy informer [7], it is essential to determine how effective a strategy it is. A previous systematic review [8] showed the majority of current Nudge literature is in the area of nutritional research, and as nutrition lies at the heart of one of the greatest health threats in the world – obesity – it seems a reasonable place to begin to answer a fundamental question: Does Nudge work, or does it not?

Scope of research

In order to have a proper debate about whether to use these nudges, and particularly how to use them, it must first be understood whether or not they actually work. To do that, existing literature must be examined and the results compiled into an easily understood and communicated ‘bottom line’. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses have been established as an effective means of collecting and summarising current data and determining effectiveness of specific interventions for use in health care and policy [9]. Therefore, this research used this methodology to answer the research questions.

Methods

Research question

This review endeavoured to answer the following primary research question:
Is nudge theory an effective strategy in influencing adults aged 18 to 65 to change their dietary choices for healthier ones?

Definition of terms

Healthier food was defined as more nutrient dense, lower calorie, lower salt, lower sugar, lower cholesterol, or lower fat. This definition was based on previous literature [10, 11]. More nutrient dense outcomes included consumption or purchase of more vegetables, fruits, whole grains, and other items reasonably identified and justified in the literature as healthy alternatives. Healthier outcomes also included decreased consumption of foods identified as “unhealthy,” such as snacks high in fat, salt, or sugar. Because healthier outcomes included both overall caloric consumption as well as nutrient dense food consumption, the terms ‘dietary’ and ‘nutritional’ are used interchangeably in this report.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

In addition to ensuring included studies were strictly responding to the research question, inclusion and exclusion criteria were also designed to exclude any study which demonstrated any risk of bias. These criteria are described in more detail in the corresponding sections below.

Population

In conjunction with the study question, only studies aimed at adults were used. Adulthood was defined by both legal and employment norms – i.e., a minimum of 18 years to a maximum 65 years of age. The age of 65 was selected as the upper limit because it is the age of retirement in many developed countries, while the lower limit of 18 is typical for the majority of legal recognitions. Studies with a range including above 65 (e.g., 22 to 75 years old) were included so long as the median age of participants remains below 65. In contrast, any study including children under 18 as well as adults was not included, unless the data was detailed in sub-group analysis allowing the researchers to only use adult data. Children under the age of 18 are the subjects of many studies, but these interventions would likely not be applicable to the general population as they would predominantly take place in schools. Finally, studies were not required to cover this entire age range, but were allowed to cover only a portion (e.g., 18–22 year olds).
Experiments were also required to include both men and women for the sake of general population value of the results. Numerous studies have noted that men and women respond differently to dietary interventions [1214], and results obtained from only one gender may therefore be skewed. Indeed, many of the papers reviewed at the full-text and abstract level showed different results and levels of significance for the men and women included in the study. Studies using convenience sampling methods were not required to report the sample’s gender balance. For laboratory-based trials, however, included studies were not permitted to have a ratio higher than 70–30 in either direction.
Studies were excluded if participants were exclusively patients of a specific medical ailment, such as diabetes. Similarly, the subject pool could not be limited to obese or overweight people, as they would have well-documented differences in appetite, satiety, and restraint ratings [15]. Laboratory studies were required to exclude already-dieting participants, as they may have abnormal baseline eating patterns. Several studies included restraint ratings in their participant characteristics. Results for “restrained” and “unrestrained” eaters were pooled in some cases to remove this confounding effect.
In summary, included studies had to be aimed at a generic population, and not be primarily focused on a subset (e.g., women, diabetics, current dieters, etc.). Studies which reported sub-group analyses were not automatically excluded, provided they reported aggregated results or sufficient raw data to independently combine sub-group results in secondary analysis.

Study design

Studies must have been aimed at influencing behaviour relating to food consumption. Those focusing exclusively on beverages or alcohol consumption were not accepted. All study designs were accepted given they included a form of control or baseline comparator. For studies with parallel intervention groups, allocation of interventions had to be randomised, and baseline characteristics reported to ensure no significant pre-treatment group differences. Qualitative studies were excluded, though papers providing both qualitative and quantitative data were included so long as the quantitative data was reported according to the other standards described in this section.

Interventions (Comparisons)

Fundamentally, the intervention of interest must change the choice architecture while maintaining the autonomy of the test subject. Alterations to choice architecture include changes to the environment (e.g., olfactory or social), perception (e.g., emotional priming), availability of food (such as convenience or portion size), or knowledge-based changed (e.g., labelling). Any intervention which involved directly asking tests subjects about the experimental condition was excluded, as it would bias the participants. Similarly, interventions were not accepted which changed the fundamental properties of the food, such as energy density or fat content. Food appearance could be altered, however, such as a change in food unit size, colour, or odour. No options could be forbidden to the consumer or test subject, and no direct financial incentive to a participant could be involved.
Many of the reviewed and included studies examined several interventions simultaneously. For those examining both nudge and a non-nudge strategy, such as menu arrangement and pricing, respectively, results were required to be reported as a main effect of the nudge. If only the interaction effect was reported, the study was excluded.

Outcomes

Results had to be presented in terms of one of the following: calories, joules, grams, or purchases (either quantity purchased or a monetary amount). These had to be measured either directly or via purchase receipts; Participants were not permitted to report their own intake. Both absolute measurements and those reporting a change relative to baseline were accepted.
Studies which did not report a measurement of error – standard deviation, standard error, or confidence interval – were excluded. Studies that provided sufficient raw data for the independent secondary calculation of error, however, were included.

Search strategy

The initial search strategy was based on a snowball method using the references from previously published similar systematic reviews, in particular [8]. Any other reviews identified during the preliminary search were also used to snowball articles, though these largely resulted in duplicates. These articles were reviewed at the title level to immediately identify those to be excluded. Those that were tentatively included were then reviewed at the abstract level, followed by the full text for those that continued to fit the criteria.
Following completion of screening of records retrieved via snowball, a systematic search of several databases was completed. The methodology for the search was adapted from [8]. The databases used were: EconLit, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, Embase, PubMed, and the Cochrane Library. As a final check point, Google Scholar was used to ensure the majority of literature had been screened.
Search terms included combinations, plurals, and various conjugations of the words relating to identified nudge strategies. The search strategy from [8] was used as a basis for search terms, but adjusted to reflect the more specific nutritional aim of this thesis. Studies published in the decade prior to the completion of our search phase (i.e., 2004–2014) were eligible for this review.
Following retrieval of all records, duplicates were removed and records screened at the title level. Records were reviewed at title and abstract level simultaneously if this option was available via the database. Those not excluded due to key words in the titles were then reviewed at the abstract level. This was followed by a full text examination to finally determine inclusion or exclusion. Figure 1 summarises the search strategy and provides details of numbers of records at each stage.
All searching, screening, and data extraction was completed by author AA. Author ST approved the search and screening protocols, checked results of screening following full text review, and statistical results after data extraction and final calculations.

Statistical analysis

In order to be able to compare heterogeneous outcomes, results were calculated as the absolute value percent change in frequency of a choice, or in consumption/purchases. These values were compiled into a forest plot and weighted according to their variance in order to demonstrate an overall trend in nudge intervention outcomes. The calculations necessary for the forest plot, including weighting of studies according to sample size and variance, were conducted using a previously published template [16]. This method of weighting results is in line with previously published reviews [9]. Statistical analyses were performed using Microsoft Excel 2010, while descriptive analyses of the included studies were performed using IBM SPSS v.21.

Results

Included study characteristics

This review identified 37 eligible papers [12, 1752], with five included that reported results from several discrete experiments, resulting in 42 individual study results. Of these, 31, or 74 %, were randomised control trials (RCTs), two (5 %) were cohort studies, and nine (21 %) were cross-sectional studies. Laboratories were the most frequent study setting, accounting for 48 % of the included papers followed by canteens with 17 %. A notable majority of studies took place in the United States, and all were conducted in relatively wealthy Global North nations. Sample sizes used were most often below 100, with a further 31 % using between 100 and 500 participants. All of this information can be found in Table 1.
Table 1
Included studies characteristics
Author(s), Year
Study design
Sample Size
Setting
Country
Outcome [% change]a
Intervention (Choice Architecture alteration) description
Walsh and Kiviniemi, 2014 [17]
RCT
117
Laboratory
USA
28.3 ± 0.0343
Subjects’ affective associations with fruit and vegetables were experimentally altered using an implicit priming paradigm. They were then asked to choose between fruit or a granola bar for a snack.
Privitera and Zuraikat, 2014 [18]
RCT
56
Laboratory
USA
79.3 ± 17.7 [outlier]
Snacks were placed closer or farther away from subjects and their snack choices recorded and compared between control and experimental groups.
Gittelsohn et al., 2013 [19]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
145
Market
USA (Navajo)
7.84 ± 12.7
Availability of healthy options was increased over a 14-month period. Outcomes were measured in change of purchases of healthy items.
Kiesel and Villas-Boas, 2013 [20]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
4000
Market
USA
12.8 ± 17.2
Nutritional labelling was implemented over a four-week period. Outcomes were measured by purchases of healthy items.
van Kleef et al., 2012 [21]
RCT
67
Laboratory
USA
41.4 ± 11.8
Size of bowl used to serve snacks was varied and consumption measured and compared between control and experimental group.
Marchiori et al., 2012 [22]
RCT
88
Laboratory
Belgium
129 ± 0.703 [outlier]
Participants were given snacks in differently sized containers and their consumption measured.
Dumanovsky et al., 2011 [23]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
7311
Restaurant (fast-food)
USA
2.20 ± 5.02
Measurements of average energy content of purchases were made before and 21 months after addition of calorie labels to menu.
Finkelstein et al., 2011 [24]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
9823
Restaurant (fast-food)
USA
−1.31 ± 3.37
Calorie labelling was implemented in one county and compared to a county where it was not, using a five-month baseline period and six-month post-intervention period for results calculation.
Hoefkens et al., 2011 [25]
Cohort
657
Canteen
Belgium
2.20 ± 10.4
Nutritional information was posted in a workplace canteen and employees surveyed regarding their lunchtime choices pre- and post-intervention.
Ogawa et al., 2011 [26]
RCT
1684
Market
Japan
9.10 ± 1.26
Point-of-purchase nutritional information was added. Over a 60 day period, sales of healthy items were measured and compared between a control store and an experimental store.
Pulos and Leng, 2010 [27]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
16000
Restaurant (other)
USA
2.00 ± 1.09
Nutritional labelling was added in a restaurant. Entrée sales from 30 days before and 30 days after implementation were used for comparison.
Roberto et al., 2010 [28]
RCT
303
Restaurant (other)
USA
11.3 ± 5.34
Experimental subjects were given a label with calorie information while the control group was given a normal menu. Calorie intake was compared between the groups.
Shimizu et al., 2010 [29]
RCT
122
Laboratory
USA
21.7 ± 10.5
Presentational cues were used to indicate either a “meal” or a “snack” condition, and intake compared between control and experimental groups.
Wisdom et al., 2010 [30] - A
RCT
290
Restaurant (fast-food)
USA
5.80 ± 4.31
Calories were added to fast-food restaurant menus and average calorie intake per purchase compared between control and experimental groups.
Wisdom et al., 2010 [30] - B
RCT
342
Restaurant (fast-food)
USA
9.00 ± 7.64
In the experimental group, selection of unhealthy items was made less convenient by adding a step to the ordering process and moving them to the second page of the menu.
Chu et al., 2009 [31]
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
13951
Canteen
USA
1.98 ± 0.14
Calorie information was posted in a workplace canteen. Calorie content of meals was calculated from sales data before and after calorie posting.
Gerend, 2009 [32]
RCT
288
Laboratory
USA
5.54 ± 5.52
In the experimental group, subjects received a menu with calorie information, while the control group received a regular menu. Calories per meal requested were compared between groups.
Kelly et al., 2009 [33]
RCT
43
Laboratory
UK (Northern Ireland)
11.7 ± 7.97
Portion size was randomly varied over four days in the lab and consumption measured and compared between conditions.
Stroebele et al., 2009 [34]
RCT
59
Laboratory
USA
44.8 ± 2.68
Subjects randomly received snack packs of different sizes for a week. Consumption measured and compared between groups.
Ueland et al., 2009 [35]
RCT
33
Laboratory
USA
7.17 ± 17.3
Subjects were told that they had received a standard portion or a larger portion, though in fact the amounts were the same. Consumption measured and compared between conditions.
Viskaal-van Dongen et al., 2009 [36]
RCT
51
Laboratory
Netherlands
36.8 ± 2.13
Foods with similar caloric content and either hidden or visible fat were presented and consumption measured and compared.
Bodor et al., 2008 [37]
Cross-sectional (internal comparison)
102
Neighbour-hood
USA
35.7 ± 1.73
Household survey regarding healthy food consumption. Groups were compared based on their proximity to a local grocery store.
Raynor and Wing, 2007 [38] - A
RCT
28
Home
USA
44.7 ± 2.68
Portion size adjusted and consumption measured and compared between groups.
Raynor and Wing, 2007 [38] - B
RCT
28
Home
USA
6.43 ± 32.2
Size of individual foot unit adjusted and consumption measured and compared between conditions.
Rolls et al., 2007 [39] (1)
RCT
23
Home
USA
14.6 ± 2.49
Portion size adjusted and consumption measured and compared between groups
Rolls et al., 2007 [40] (2)
RCT
119
Laboratory
USA
2.62 ± 1.85
Size of plate used was changed between control and experimental conditions, while the amount of food was held constant. Consumption was measured and compared between groups.
Antonuk and Block, 2006 [41]
RCT
67
Laboratory
USA
37.0 ± 18.2
Subjects were given snack food with randomly varying nutritional labelling. Their consumption was measured and compared.
Wansink et al., 2006 [42] - A
RCT
85
Laboratory
USA
12.6 ± 11.9
Larger serving utensils were used in the experimental group and consumption measured and compared.
Wansink et al., 2006 [42] - B
RCT
85
Laboratory
USA
23.6 ± 15.3
Larger bowls but identical serving sizes were used in the experimental group and consumption measured and compared.
Hetherington et al., 2006 [43] - A
RCT
37
Laboratory
UK (England)
11.2 ± 2.82
Subjects ate a meal either in the company of friends or of strangers. Consumption in the two conditions was compared.
Hetherington et al., 2006 [43] - B
RCT
37
Laboratory
UK (England)
15.4 ± 2.82
Subjects ate a meal either while watching TV or while alone. Consumption in the two conditions was compared.
Huang et al., 2006 [44]
RCT
456
Online
Australia
0.620 ± 0.165
Availability of healthier options in online supermarket was adjusted. Change in sales of healthy items was compared.
Norton et al., 2006 [12]
RCT
30
Laboratory
UK (England)
13.7 ± 4.02
Sandwiches were provided either with a variety of fillings or with a homogenous filling, though energy content was constant. Calorie intake was measured and compared.
Wansink and Kim, 2005 [45]
RCT
72
Movie Theatre
USA
31.2 ± 10.2
Popcorn in a movie theatre was sold in containers of various sizes. Consumption was measured and compared.
Wansink et al., 2005 [46]
RCT
54
Canteen
USA
42.2 ± 18.8
Experimental subjects were unknowingly given self-refilling bowls to make second helpings more convenient. Consumption was measured and compared.
Devitt and Mattes, 2004 [47]
RCT
20
Laboratory
USA
6.04 ± 5.70
Food unit size was randomly adjusted on four separate days. Consumption was measured and compared.
Diliberti et al., 2004 [48]
RCT
180
Canteen
USA
20.2 ± 0.567
Portion size of canteen entrées randomly varied in a workplace canteen. Sales data used to calculate energy content of meals.
Levitsky and Youn, 2004 [49]
RCT
13
Canteen
USA
23.6 ± 2.42
Portion size of canteen entrées randomly adjusted. Consumption measured and compared between conditions.
Rolls et al., 2004 [50] (1)
RCT
60
Laboratory
USA
12.1 ± 1.70
Subjects were given snacks of differing portion sizes. Consumption between control and experimental groups compared.
Rolls et al., 2004 [51] (2)
Cohort
75
Laboratory
USA
44.7 ± 1.90
Over four weeks, subjects were given sandwiches of varying sizes at a once weekly lab lunch. Consumption between conditions compared.
Steenhuis et al., 2004 [52] - A
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
290
Canteen
Netherlands
−4.51 ± 8.92
In a workplace canteen, availability of healthy options was increased or held constant at seventeen worksites that were randomly assigned. Sales were used to calculate calorie content of meals and compared between conditions.
Steenhuis et al., 2004 [52] - B
Cross-sectional (pre-post comparison)
215
Canteen
Netherlands
−8.05 ± 9.98
At 17 randomly assigned worksites, healthy items were given additional labelling or left in original state. Sales were used to calculate calorie content of meals and compared between conditions.
aNegative values indicate behavioural changes opposite to those intended or desired (i.e., unhealthier)

Meta-analysis

When each study was weighted according to previously published methods [16], this analysis demonstrated that nudge interventions on average cause a 15.3 % (95 % confidence: 7.58, 23.0) increase in healthier consumption decisions, as measured by frequency of healthy choices or by overall intake. This result is shown in Fig. 2.

Discussion

This analysis demonstrates that overall, nudge strategies successfully increase healthy nutritional choices. This review also demonstrated the current availability of evidence relating to Nudges and food policy, and highlighted the literature’s weaknesses. Our work built significantly upon the results of Hollands et al. (2013) by performing meta-analysis on the results of many studies initially identified in that work.
A significant limitation of this review is the lack of inter-rater reliability data due to the methods used, in which all studies were screened by a single reviewer with the second acting as a check point following completion of each screening stage. However, given the reasonable consistency of results across the articles it would suggest that this is not a major problem for the overall findings. Nevertheless, the validity of this work could have been improved by using a larger team for screening and reporting an inter-rater reliability metric such as Cohen’s Kappa.
As with any systematic review, this review and its results were limited by the presence of heterogeneous results and studies. Despite the presence of several sources of heterogeneity in this study’s included papers, discussed in the succeeding section, these are unlikely to have a large effect on the overall findings.
Firstly, the included studies differed in the precise population targeted. For example, a record retrieved during an initial trial search was aimed at a particular socioeconomic demographic by nature of its geographical setting. It is possible that interventions specifically designed for one demographic may not apply to another, or that compiling the results of such interventions could present heterogeneity to the results. However, by not excluding these studies, it was hoped to maintain the generalizability of these findings, as an entire population would include a plethora of socioeconomic statuses.
Similarly, that 100 % of the included studies were conducted in Global North nations, with a particular emphasis on the United States, may weaken the ability of the results of this review to be extrapolated to a more worldwide population, or to be applied in different national settings. This exclusive focus on relatively wealthy nations is indicative of an over-emphasis of obesity research on those nations; although this focus may be valid given the health trends of the past several decades, emerging economies are also poised to suffer the next wave of obesity and overweight, and this situation must be addressed.
A significant source of heterogeneity will likely arise, as in many past reviews, from the differences in how researchers measured their results. Results were evaluated as a proportional change in behaviour, outcome, or status, in order to be able to compile different results into one cohesive statement. While selection of this measure was necessary in order to be able to compile paper results and perform meta-analysis, it did necessitate the exclusion of some studies on the basis of lack of specific statistical data. Thus differences in measurement of outcomes not only presented heterogeneity of results calculated in this meta-analysis, but also shrank the pool of available data for statistical analysis. With more rigid reporting measures, the statistical results of this review could be updated to provide more robust results, such as effect size
Though nearly a third of the included papers reported a statistically insignificant result, there is always concern over publication bias [53], which would skew the results towards desirable and more statistically significant outcomes. Moving forward with Nudge Theory research, the field would benefit from reporting of all experimentation, whether its results are successful, unsuccessful, significant, or insignificant.

Suggestions for future research

As described in the results section, included papers were exclusively from high-income countries (HICs) and from the United States in particular. Though these results provide a good theoretical framework for future work in other HICs, it is difficult to confidently ascertain the viability of these interventions in a low or middle-income country setting. Therefore, researchers should begin to conduct similar studies in more diverse country settings. By obtaining these results, the ability to extrapolate general policy themes from data will be strengthened.
While this review retrieved substantial data, our analyses would be strengthened by further availability of experiment results. The literature would benefit from repetition of nudges with demonstrated success at a broader, more population-based, level.
The research reported here focused on generally healthy male and female adults, as described in the methodology section and participant criteria. Due to the frequent reporting of different eating restraint scores between men and women [39, 45], it was chosen to focus only on mixed groups, but a worthwhile future venture would be to examine these same results as they are different between the sexes. It could be useful for policy makers to be aware of these discrepancies, and to examine the best intervention for each of them independently if nudges are to be considered in any gender-specific settings. At a minimum, such a review could confirm the results found for the general population for each group independently. These results could allow policy-makers to better tailor nudge approaches to specific settings.
Finally, a crucial step in future research will be to perform sub-group analyses on the results reported here and any additional studies that become available. These analyses could include differentiating results among different targeted populations to determine the best strategy for particular target groups, as mentioned above. They should certainly include a taxonomy of Nudge strategies and individual analysis for each of them, to determine if a particular method is better or worse than others. This would add benefit for health policy makers to be certain they are spending resources in the most effective ways.

Conclusions

The results of this review and meta-analysis demonstrate that Nudge Theory strategies provide an effective and viable public health strategy in encouraging healthier eating choices in adults. As governments and policy-making bodies continue to consider the use of Nudge, it is essential they draw on strong evidence for its effectiveness. This work provides some of the first meta-analysis results to contribute to this required evidence base. It also clarifies some of the gaps in the current Nudge literature, highlighting where more research is needed to further strengthen the available evidence base.

Abbreviation

HIC, high-income country

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the entire staff of the Centre for Global Health at Trinity College, Dublin for their support throughout this research.

Funding

No outside funding for this research was received.

Availability of data and materials

The data supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the submitted Additional files 1, 2, 3 and 4.

Authors’ contributions

ST conceived of the study, and collaborated with AA in developing the design. AA completed the screening, data extraction, and meta-analysis. ST carried out secondary review of the meta-analysis, results, and conclusions. AA drafted the manuscript. Both authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Not applicable.
This research was approved in February 2014 by the Health Policy & Management / Centre for Global Health Research Ethics Committee of Trinity College, Dublin.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Avenell A, et al. Systematic Review of the Long-Term Effects and Economic Consequences of Treatments for Obesity and Implications for Health Improvement. 2004. Avenell A, et al. Systematic Review of the Long-Term Effects and Economic Consequences of Treatments for Obesity and Implications for Health Improvement. 2004.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Forster M, Veerman J, Barendregt J, Vos T. Cost-effectiveness of diet and exercise interventions to reduce overweight and obesity. Int J Obes. 2011;35:1071–8.CrossRef Forster M, Veerman J, Barendregt J, Vos T. Cost-effectiveness of diet and exercise interventions to reduce overweight and obesity. Int J Obes. 2011;35:1071–8.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Blumenthal-Barby JS, Burroughs H. Seeking better health care outcomes: the ethics of using the “nudge”. Am J Bioeth. 2012;12:1–10.CrossRefPubMed Blumenthal-Barby JS, Burroughs H. Seeking better health care outcomes: the ethics of using the “nudge”. Am J Bioeth. 2012;12:1–10.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodwin T. Why we should reject ‘nudge’. Politics. 2012;32:85–92.CrossRef Goodwin T. Why we should reject ‘nudge’. Politics. 2012;32:85–92.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Marteau T. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? Br Med J. 2011;342:3. M. Young, The Technical Writer’s Handbook. Mill Valley, CA: University Science, 1989.CrossRef Marteau T. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? Br Med J. 2011;342:3. M. Young, The Technical Writer’s Handbook. Mill Valley, CA: University Science, 1989.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hollands G, et al. Altering micro-environments to change population health behaviour: towards an evidence base for choice architecture interventions. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1218.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hollands G, et al. Altering micro-environments to change population health behaviour: towards an evidence base for choice architecture interventions. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1218.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1. 0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. 2011. Green, S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1. 0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. 2011.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat US Dept. of Health and Human Services, P.H.S. Healthy People 2000: National Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Objectives-Nutrition Priority Area. Nutr Today. 1990;25:29–39.CrossRef US Dept. of Health and Human Services, P.H.S. Healthy People 2000: National Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Objectives-Nutrition Priority Area. Nutr Today. 1990;25:29–39.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Mozaffarian D, Hao T, Rimm EB, Willett WC, Hu FB. Changes in diet and lifestyle and long-term weight gain in women and Men. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:2392–404.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mozaffarian D, Hao T, Rimm EB, Willett WC, Hu FB. Changes in diet and lifestyle and long-term weight gain in women and Men. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:2392–404.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Norton GNM, Anderson AS, Hetherington MM. Volume and variety: relative effects on food intake. Physiol Behav. 2006;87:714–22.CrossRefPubMed Norton GNM, Anderson AS, Hetherington MM. Volume and variety: relative effects on food intake. Physiol Behav. 2006;87:714–22.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Scott ML, Nowlis SM, Mandel N, Morales AC. The effects of reduced food size and package size on the consumption behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. J Consum Res. 2008;35:391–405.CrossRef Scott ML, Nowlis SM, Mandel N, Morales AC. The effects of reduced food size and package size on the consumption behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. J Consum Res. 2008;35:391–405.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Coelho JS, Polivy J, Peter Herman C, Pliner P. Wake up and smell the cookies. Effects of olfactory food-cue exposure in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite. 2009;52:517–20.CrossRefPubMed Coelho JS, Polivy J, Peter Herman C, Pliner P. Wake up and smell the cookies. Effects of olfactory food-cue exposure in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite. 2009;52:517–20.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Dykes J, Brunner E, Martikainen P, Wardle J. Socioeconomic gradient in body size and obesity among women: the role of dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger in the Whitehall II study. Int J Obes. 2003;28:262–8. Dykes J, Brunner E, Martikainen P, Wardle J. Socioeconomic gradient in body size and obesity among women: the role of dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger in the Whitehall II study. Int J Obes. 2003;28:262–8.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Neyeloff JL, Fuchs SC, Moreira LB. Meta-analyses and Forest plots using a microsoft excel spreadsheet: step-by-step guide focusing on descriptive data analysis. BMC Res Notes. 2012;5:52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Neyeloff JL, Fuchs SC, Moreira LB. Meta-analyses and Forest plots using a microsoft excel spreadsheet: step-by-step guide focusing on descriptive data analysis. BMC Res Notes. 2012;5:52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh EM, Kiviniemi MT. Changing how I feel about the food: experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors. J Behav Med. 2014;37:322–31. Walsh EM, Kiviniemi MT. Changing how I feel about the food: experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors. J Behav Med. 2014;37:322–31.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Privitera GJ, Zuraikat FM. Proximity of foods in a competitive food environment influences consumption of a low calorie and a high calorie food. Appetite. 2014;76:175–79. Privitera GJ, Zuraikat FM. Proximity of foods in a competitive food environment influences consumption of a low calorie and a high calorie food. Appetite. 2014;76:175–79.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Gittelsohn J, Kim EM, He S, Pardilla M. A Food Store–Based Environmental Intervention Is Associated with Reduced BMI and Improved Psychosocial Factors and Food-Related Behaviors on the Navajo Nation. J Nutr. 2013;143:1494–1500. Gittelsohn J, Kim EM, He S, Pardilla M. A Food Store–Based Environmental Intervention Is Associated with Reduced BMI and Improved Psychosocial Factors and Food-Related Behaviors on the Navajo Nation. J Nutr. 2013;143:1494–1500.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiesel K, Villas-Boas SB. Can information costs affect consumer choice? Nutritional labels in a supermarket experiment. Int J Ind Organ. 2013;31:153–63. Kiesel K, Villas-Boas SB. Can information costs affect consumer choice? Nutritional labels in a supermarket experiment. Int J Ind Organ. 2013;31:153–63.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Kleef E, Shimizu M, Wansink B. Serving bowl selection biases the amount of food served. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2012;44:66–70. Van Kleef E, Shimizu M, Wansink B. Serving bowl selection biases the amount of food served. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2012;44:66–70.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Marchiori D, Corneille O, Klein, O. Container size influences snack food intake independently of portion size. Appetite. 2012;58:814–17. Marchiori D, Corneille O, Klein, O. Container size influences snack food intake independently of portion size. Appetite. 2012;58:814–17.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Dumanovsky T, Huang CY, Nonas CA, Matte TD, Bassett MT, Silver LD. Changes in energy content of lunchtime purchases from fast food restaurants after introduction of calorie labelling: cross sectional customer surveys. BMJ. 2011;343. Dumanovsky T, Huang CY, Nonas CA, Matte TD, Bassett MT, Silver LD. Changes in energy content of lunchtime purchases from fast food restaurants after introduction of calorie labelling: cross sectional customer surveys. BMJ. 2011;343.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Finkelstein EA, Strombotne KL, Chan NL, Krieger J. Mandatory menu labeling in one fast-food chain in King County, Washington. Am J Prev Med. 2011;40:122–27. Finkelstein EA, Strombotne KL, Chan NL, Krieger J. Mandatory menu labeling in one fast-food chain in King County, Washington. Am J Prev Med. 2011;40:122–27.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoefkens C, Lachat C, Kolsteren P, Van Camp J, Verbeke W. Posting point-of-purchase nutrition information in university canteens does not influence meal choice and nutrient intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:562–70. Hoefkens C, Lachat C, Kolsteren P, Van Camp J, Verbeke W. Posting point-of-purchase nutrition information in university canteens does not influence meal choice and nutrient intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:562–70.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogawa Y, Tanabe N, Honda A, Azuma T, Seki N, Suzuki T, Suzuki H. Point-of-purchase health information encourages customers to purchase vegetables: objective analysis by using a point-of-sales system. Environ Health Prev Med. 2011;16:239–46. Ogawa Y, Tanabe N, Honda A, Azuma T, Seki N, Suzuki T, Suzuki H. Point-of-purchase health information encourages customers to purchase vegetables: objective analysis by using a point-of-sales system. Environ Health Prev Med. 2011;16:239–46.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Pulos E, Leng K. Evaluation of a voluntary menu-labeling program in full-service restaurants. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:1035–39. Pulos E, Leng K. Evaluation of a voluntary menu-labeling program in full-service restaurants. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:1035–39.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberto CA, Larsen PD, Agnew H, Baik J, Brownell KD. Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:312. Roberto CA, Larsen PD, Agnew H, Baik J, Brownell KD. Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:312.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Shimizu M, Payne CR, Wansink B. When snacks become meals: How hunger and environmental cues bias food intake. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:63. Shimizu M, Payne CR, Wansink B. When snacks become meals: How hunger and environmental cues bias food intake. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:63.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Wisdom J, Downs JS, Loewenstein G. Promoting healthy choices: Information versus convenience. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2010;164–178. Wisdom J, Downs JS, Loewenstein G. Promoting healthy choices: Information versus convenience. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2010;164–178.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Chu YH, Frongillo EA, Jones SJ, Kaye GL. Improving patrons' meal selections through the use of point-of-selection nutrition labels. Am J Public Health. 2009;99:2001. Chu YH, Frongillo EA, Jones SJ, Kaye GL. Improving patrons' meal selections through the use of point-of-selection nutrition labels. Am J Public Health. 2009;99:2001.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerend MA. Does calorie information promote lower calorie fast food choices among college students? J Adolesc Health. 2009;44:84–86. Gerend MA. Does calorie information promote lower calorie fast food choices among college students? J Adolesc Health. 2009;44:84–86.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly MT, Wallace JM, Robson PJ, Rennie KL, Welch RW, Hannon-Fletcher MP, Brennan S, Fletcher A, Livingstone M. Increased portion size leads to a sustained increase in energy intake over 4 d in normal-weight and overweight men and women. British journal of nutrition. 2009;102:470–77. Kelly MT, Wallace JM, Robson PJ, Rennie KL, Welch RW, Hannon-Fletcher MP, Brennan S, Fletcher A, Livingstone M. Increased portion size leads to a sustained increase in energy intake over 4 d in normal-weight and overweight men and women. British journal of nutrition. 2009;102:470–77.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Stroebele N, Ogden LG, Hill JO. Do calorie-controlled portion sizes of snacks reduce energy intake? Appetite. 2009;52:793–96. Stroebele N, Ogden LG, Hill JO. Do calorie-controlled portion sizes of snacks reduce energy intake? Appetite. 2009;52:793–96.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Ueland Ø, Cardello AV, Merrill EP, Lesher LL. Effect of portion size information on food intake. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:124–27. Ueland Ø, Cardello AV, Merrill EP, Lesher LL. Effect of portion size information on food intake. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:124–27.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Viskaal-Van Dongen M, De Graaf C, Siebelink E, Kok FJ. Hidden fat facilitates passive overconsumption. J Nutr. 2009;139:394–99. Viskaal-Van Dongen M, De Graaf C, Siebelink E, Kok FJ. Hidden fat facilitates passive overconsumption. J Nutr. 2009;139:394–99.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bodor JN, Rose D, Farley TA, Swalm C, Scott SK. Neighbourhood fruit and vegetable availability and consumption: the role of small food stores in an urban environment. Public Health Nutr. 2008;11:413–20. Bodor JN, Rose D, Farley TA, Swalm C, Scott SK. Neighbourhood fruit and vegetable availability and consumption: the role of small food stores in an urban environment. Public Health Nutr. 2008;11:413–20.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Raynor HA, Wing RR. Package unit size and amount of food: do both influence intake? Obesity. 2007;15:2311–19. Raynor HA, Wing RR. Package unit size and amount of food: do both influence intake? Obesity. 2007;15:2311–19.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS. The effect of large portion sizes on energy intake is sustained for 11 days. Obesity. 2007;15:1535–43. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS. The effect of large portion sizes on energy intake is sustained for 11 days. Obesity. 2007;15:1535–43.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Halverson KH, Meengs JS. Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite. 2007;49:652–60. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Halverson KH, Meengs JS. Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite. 2007;49:652–60.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Antonuk B, Block LG. The Effect of Single Serving Versus Entire Package; 2006. Antonuk B, Block LG. The Effect of Single Serving Versus Entire Package; 2006.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Wansink B, Van Ittersum K, Painter JE. Ice cream illusions: bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med. 2006;31:240–43. Wansink B, Van Ittersum K, Painter JE. Ice cream illusions: bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med. 2006;31:240–43.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington MM, Anderson AS, Norton GN, Newson L. Situational effects on meal intake: A comparison of eating alone and eating with others. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:498–505. Hetherington MM, Anderson AS, Norton GN, Newson L. Situational effects on meal intake: A comparison of eating alone and eating with others. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:498–505.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang A, Barzi F, Huxley R, Denyer G, Rohrlach B, Jayne K, Neal B. The effects on saturated fat purchases of providing internet shoppers with purchase-specific dietary advice: a randomised trial. PLoS clinical trials. 2006;1:e22. Huang A, Barzi F, Huxley R, Denyer G, Rohrlach B, Jayne K, Neal B. The effects on saturated fat purchases of providing internet shoppers with purchase-specific dietary advice: a randomised trial. PLoS clinical trials. 2006;1:e22.
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Wansink B, Painter JE, North J. Bottomless Bowls: Why Visual Cues of Portion Size May Influence Intake**. Obes Res. 2005;13:93–100. Wansink B, Painter JE, North J. Bottomless Bowls: Why Visual Cues of Portion Size May Influence Intake**. Obes Res. 2005;13:93–100.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Wansink B, Kim J. Bad popcorn in big buckets: portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005;37:242–45. Wansink B, Kim J. Bad popcorn in big buckets: portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005;37:242–45.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Devitt AA, Mattes RD. Effects of food unit size and energy density on intake in humans. Appetite. 2004;42:213–20. Devitt AA, Mattes RD. Effects of food unit size and energy density on intake in humans. Appetite. 2004;42:213–20.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Diliberti N, Bordi PL, Conklin MT, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal. Obes Res. 2004;12:562–68. Diliberti N, Bordi PL, Conklin MT, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal. Obes Res. 2004;12:562–68.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Levitsky DA, Youn T. The more food young adults are served, the more they overeat. J Nutr. 2004;134:2546–549. Levitsky DA, Youn T. The more food young adults are served, the more they overeat. J Nutr. 2004;134:2546–549.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Kral TV, Meengs JS, Wall DE. Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite. 2004;42:63–69. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Kral TV, Meengs JS, Wall DE. Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite. 2004;42:63–69.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS, Wall DE. Increasing the portion size of a sandwich increases energy intake. J Am Dietetic Assoc. 2004;104:367–72. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS, Wall DE. Increasing the portion size of a sandwich increases energy intake. J Am Dietetic Assoc. 2004;104:367–72.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Steenhuis I, Van Assema P, Van Breukelen G, Glanz K, Kok G, De Vries H. The impact of educational and environmental interventions in Dutch worksite cafeterias. Health Promotion Int. 2004;19:335–43. Steenhuis I, Van Assema P, Van Breukelen G, Glanz K, Kok G, De Vries H. The impact of educational and environmental interventions in Dutch worksite cafeterias. Health Promotion Int. 2004;19:335–43.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M. Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments. Hoboken: Wiley; 2006. Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M. Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments. Hoboken: Wiley; 2006.
Metadaten
Titel
The efficacy of nudge theory strategies in influencing adult dietary behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis
verfasst von
Anneliese Arno
Steve Thomas
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3272-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

BMC Public Health 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe