Skip to main content
Erschienen in:

Open Access 08.08.2023

The Foundations of Autistic Flourishing

verfasst von: Elizabeth Pellicano, Melanie Heyworth

Erschienen in: Current Psychiatry Reports | Ausgabe 9/2023

Abstract

Purpose of Review

All people—including Autistic people—deserve to live flourishing lives. But what does a flourishing life look like for Autistic people? We suggest that the hidden biases, methodological errors, and key assumptions of autism science have obscured answers to this question. Here, we seek to initiate a broader discussion about what the foundations for a good Autistic life might be and how this discussion might be framed.

Recent Findings

We identify five ways in which autism science can help us all to secure those foundations, including by (1) giving Autistic well-being prominence in research, (2) amplifying Autistic autonomy, (3) attending better to everyday experiences, (4) acknowledging context, and (5) working in partnership with Autistic people and their families and allies to ensure that they are at the heart of research decision-making.

Summary

Such an approach would direct the focus of autism research to help shape good Autistic lives.
Hinweise

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

At the core of all good societies is an effort to enable all people to live full and flourishing lives, regardless of their background or characteristics. There has been much debate about the key elements of human flourishing, dating back to at least Aristotle, who wrote of people’s need to “live well and do well” or, in short, to live a good life. Yet, remarkably, the idea of flourishing lives has rarely been applied to Autistic1 people. Its absence, we contend, is rooted in the field’s historic commitment to the conventional medical model, in which a person’s functional limitations or “impairments” are the source of any disadvantages experienced, which can be remedied by treatment or cure [13]. The absence of any meaningful discussion of flourishing could be because the challenges facing Autistic people can be substantial and lifelong [4••], with the fundamental idea of a “good life” thus seeming out of reach. Or it might be that Autistic people have seldom been invited into the conversation about what such a good life constitutes for them, meaning that conventional accounts of human flourishing seem a long way from any putative Autistic norm. Whatever the reasons, it means that we do not know what a flourishing life might look like for Autistic people nor do we fully understand how we can help to build a foundation for such lives.
Here, our aim is not to provide a full account of Autistic flourishing. That is not our right, nor could it be achieved in a single paper. Rather, we aim here to outline a framework to help Autistic people, their families and allies, researchers, practitioners, and policymakers alike to begin a meaningful conversation about what Autistic flourishing may involve. We suggest that such understanding will require us to step away from key assumptions that have structured autism science for decades now—and to acknowledge the ways in which both hidden biases and methodological errors too often obscure understanding the key elements of Autistic flourishing.2 In what follows below, we outline five ways in which we, as an autism research community, need to reconsider Autistic flourishing, functioning almost as a toolkit for those seeking to have conversations about such flourishing well into the future.

From Health to Well-Being

The first among these is a need to move from a perspective that says the core to Autistic flourishing is simply conventional “physical and mental health” to one that says it is the far broader notion of “well-being” [5]. Autism researchers have largely inherited a medical model that says “physical and mental health” is what matters. They have historically seen autism as a disability in the medical sense: an individual health problem with a biological cause, which requires diagnosing, treating, and fixing [69].3 As such, autism research often remains firmly embedded within the conventional medical paradigm [10•]—prioritizing a putatively objective standard of “health” over a first-person understanding of “well-being,” which is often dismissed as being overly subjective or biased to personal perception [11]. This priority given to the conventional medical model is evidenced by the way autism science routinely seeks to identify the genetic, neurobiological, and cognitive mechanisms that might explain the highly heterogenous behavioral manifestations of autism [1215]. Autistic people’s behavioral, cognitive, and neural functionings are therefore frequently compared to some typical or “normal” level of ability, cast as an ideal “state of health.” Interventions and treatments are designed to be applied to the individual to remediate any apparent shortcomings, bringing Autistic people into line with the accepted norm [16, 17].
As should be immediately apparent, this narrow focus on health results in a radically constrained understanding of the value and shape of Autistic lives. It also leads to a much-discussed tendency to see elements of the Autistic experience that stand outside the norm as “deficits,” “impairments,” or “deviations” [10•, 18], independent of what those elements happen to be. Even when Autistic people outperform non-autistic people in scientific experiments, it is sometimes supposed that those achievements reveal a problem with Autistic people that needs to be fixed, rather than simply revealing a different (even potentially “better”) way of being [16, 17, 19, 20]. As we have argued elsewhere [21•, 22••], the medical model, therefore, does not only narrow the range of issues that are given focus, it also “colors” the broader community’s perceptions of Autistic people’s capacities. All of this has sharp real-world implications. It is an approach that is linked to heightened experiences of stigma [23]. It also continues to shape clinical, educational, and public policy responses, with Autistic people being encouraged to fit norms and pursue aspirations apparently demanded by their “health” rather than attending to their wider sense of well-being. If we want to work toward a theory of Autistic flourishing, our contention here is that we need to shift away from this conventional medical paradigm [21•].
The good news is that there are changes afoot already in this regard. The World Health Organization (WHO) has long held that “health is a state of complete physical, mental and social wellbeing and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” [24] and more recently has recognized “that persons with disabilities have the right to the enjoyment of the highest attainable standard of health without discrimination on the basis of disability” [25]. Consistent with this view, the WHO’s International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) seeks to provide a comprehensive, integrated framework, recognizing the role of social and environmental factors in the creation of disability, as well as an individual’s health conditions and their effects [26, 27]. Critics may understandably continue to worry that these efforts do not go far enough to shift the emphasis of biomedical views of disability [28, 29], but there is an apparent consensus emerging that a broader range of concerns need to be considered for Autistic people to consider themselves to be leading good lives than is apparent to those constrained by the more conventional medical approach.

From Other-Defined to Self-Defined

As efforts are made to broaden the range of issues that are taken to be constitutive of a good Autistic life—that is, away from narrow medical concerns to other elements that can meaningfully be considered to be integral to well-being —it is crucial to identify, next, who it is that is empowered to determine what an Autistic good life might be. In the past, Autistic people have been expected to fit a series of requirements laid down by conventional medical practitioners and other experts, who are themselves not Autistic. A properly full account of Autistic flourishing cannot, however, be satisfied with that approach. To capture the fundamental aspects of Autistic life, any attempt to outline the key elements of Autistic flourishing must be primarily determined and defined by Autistic people themselves.
The second essential move is therefore from an “other-defined” framework to a “self-defined” one. At the moment, even when researchers and policymakers use the language of “well-being” and “quality of life” rather than medicalized concerns, these are often derived from a set of standard “life achievements,” irrespective of whether those outcomes are considered meaningful by Autistic adults themselves [30, 31•]. Autistic people repeatedly fare badly on normatively conventional life goals, including friendships, intimate relationships, and workplace success [3234], and it is often concluded, as such, that Autistic people have a poor quality of life [though see 3536]. Of course, many of these normative life achievements are, in fact, important both for Autistic and non-autistic people. But they do not by themselves provide a sufficient account of Autistic well-being nor do they recognize that well-being for Autistic people might differ from well-being for non-autistic people. That is because judgments as to the quality of Autistic lives have all too often been made from outside Autistic experience rather than from within [37••, 38•]. Ne’eman [31•] describes a “disturbing trend” in the way that “researchers, clinicians, and paraprofessionals are using measures that prioritise reducing diagnostic traits that are neither harmful nor personally distressing” to Autistic people themselves (p. 569).
Again, we have begun to see some progress in this regard. There are now a series of studies comparing traditional, researcher-defined objective measures with more subjective measures. What those studies have demonstrated is that outcomes on these measures do not always match up [3942]. That is, normatively defined “successful” outcomes (independent living, for example) do not always go hand-in-hand with better quality of life, highlighting the importance of both examining the intersection between the person and their environment [42, 43••] and understanding the ways in which Autistic people can flourish on their own terms.
Notwithstanding, it is not just that we are simply more prone to obtaining misleading results and/or misinterpreting these results if we do not attend to subjective perspectives. We fail also to grant Autistic people the dignity, agency, and respect they deserve. It can be taken as fundamental that everyone, including Autistic people, has the opportunity to identify their own desired path in life and, as such, that quality of life should always be measured, at least in part, by the ways in which actual life maps on to the person’s own aspirations.
Another way of putting this is that if we want to take well-being seriously, we need to take Autistic autonomy seriously too [43••, 44•]. As political philosophers Allen et al. [45] explain: “there is no single picture of the flourishing life. What is shared, however… is that human beings are creatures who need to chart their own courses in life. Humans thrive on autonomy, the opportunity for self-creation and self-governance” (p. 18). That is true even of people who are largely dependent on others to help meet their needs [46]. Despite the importance of personal autonomy in allowing people to live a good life on their own terms [47], there has been remarkably little focus on Autistic people’s autonomy [48, 49].
None of this is to say, of course, that Autistic lives are best lived entirely independently or that autonomy is the only value that matters. Achieving full-blown “independence” or self-sufficiency with minimal (or no) support is an impossible standard for anyone [50] and can be a difficult expectation for Autistic people to bear since it denies that interconnectedness shapes everyone’s life [51]. We all depend on others throughout our lives—that is, we are interdependent—and that is, in fact, a good and vital part of life [5254]. There are, furthermore, always limits on autonomy. Actions that are injurious to others or even those that are profoundly injurious to ourselves are generally understood to be the legitimate subject of external constraint. This was the foundation of the famous “harm principle” in the Victorian philosophy of John Stuart Mill [55], from where the modern concept of autonomy is often drawn. What matters, though, is that each of us is treated as the primary source of our own fundamental judgments and is supported to direct our lives as free from external control as possible.
One reason why this matters so much is that Autistic people, who are non-speaking, have an intellectual disability and/or have high support needs, have sometimes been considered unable to communicate or conceptualize their precise wishes, and, as such, are said to be incapable of being in control, even in part, of their own lives [44•, 56••]. This view is both overly pessimistic and can have damaging consequences—for individuals and communities. There is much to learn from the literature on self-determination in people with intellectual disability. This work clearly demonstrates that those who show more self-determined, agentic behaviors (e.g., choice/decision-making, problem-solving, and self-regulation) also have a greater quality of life [57] and has also identified the many ways in which environments can be created that empower intellectually disabled people to make decisions for themselves [58]. If we want to come to a theory of Autistic flourishing, we need to understand what a good life means to Autistic people themselves, regardless of their support or communication needs, as well as to acknowledge that Autistic lives can indeed be flourishing ones.

From the Big to the Small

This focus on Autistic autonomy then takes us to our third “building block” of a theory of Autistic flourishing: the move from the big to the small. This may initially seem counter-intuitive. Flourishing, after all, sounds like a “big” idea, a grand theory of what it means to live well. But once we move from an other-defined to a self-defined theory, and begin to ask Autistic people what actually matters in life to them, research reveals that (like most other human beings) they quickly tell us it is not generally the “moonshots” of academic research or public policy-making that excites them, but a range of everyday concerns that build up to be the weft of ordinary life.
This first became apparent to one of us 10 years ago; when together with colleagues, we examined the state of autism research in the UK—how much had been spent on autism research and what it had been spent on [59]. We then asked Autistic people, their families, practitioners, and researchers about their thoughts on the decisions made and where the funds toward autism research should be prioritized in the future. While Autistic people and their families were impressed by the quality of British autism research, they were not at all convinced that it had anything to do with what we now call Autistic flourishing. As one woman said “I fill in all these questionnaires and do everything I can to help… but when it comes down to it, it’s not real life” [60]. Too many people felt that there was a huge gap between the more grand, abstract knowledge that was often produced by research and their real-life, everyday experiences [61]. It fails to tell us, for example, how to help Autistic people catch the bus by themselves, do the groceries, get a date, or complete the paperwork to secure their insurance or social security benefits.
One concrete example of this disconnect between “lab and life” comes from the field of executive function. Researchers have long held that Autistic people have executive problems and that such problems at least superficially map onto reported difficulties in everyday, adaptive behavior [62, 63]. Yet, despite decades of research on executive function in Autistic people, the existing scientific literature—conducted predominantly within carefully controlled lab-based settings where one can manipulate variables and discover causal relationships between one factor and another—has been branded as contradictory and “confusing” [64], with, according to a recent meta-analysis, group differences of only moderate effect [65]. Such effects are a far cry from the large effect sizes reported on (self-/parent-report) questionnaire measures of real-world planning/flexibility difficulties. Indeed, self-reports describe everyday executive issues to be highly variable and depending, critically, on the context in which the task is done (like cooking at home vs. cooking at school), a person's mental state (especially anxiety), the clarity of the task instructions, and the motivation or interest in doing the task [66, 67]. The exclusive reliance on lab-based work, however, has resulted in paradigms that are “simple, contrived and artificial” [68] and fail to achieve representativeness (the correspondence between the task and real-life settings) and generalizability (the degree to which task performance predicts problems in real-life settings) [69]. To put that another way, how can we hope to foster flourishing when the research is decontextualized from the lived reality of Autistic lives?
This issue was also emphasized by participants in a study we conducted examining Autistic people’s experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic [70]. What many Autistic people felt most deeply was the loss of their everyday routines and expected experiences, and they also felt that nobody had noticed how important that was. Owing to the lockdown, participants were unable to engage in everyday activities that many had strived hard to access in the past and which were essential for their well-being—like going to the library, the local swimming pool, the cinema, the mall, sports training or dance class, and the playground with the kids. As one Autistic parent put it, even “the meaningless stuff had become… meaningful and needed” (p. 922). As a research community, we have been too neglectful of the “mundane” aspects of life that matter to people. If we want to work toward an account of Autistic flourishing, then we need to move from the big to the small.

From the Individual to the “Individual-in-Context”

We have, then, often neglected the broader context which creates the range of real opportunities for Autistic people. That takes us to the fourth element—the need to move from an individual understanding of flourishing to a contextual one. Pervasive adoption of the medical model in conventional autism research has also meant that there is an overemphasis on specific attributes of individuals as opposed to the broader contexts in which Autistic people live [21•]. In the conventional medical view, autism and its associated disabilities are seen as something inherent to the individual. Biomedical research tends not to explain an Autistic person’s difficulties with reference to the context in which the difficulty occurs, but rather as a characteristic of the individual themselves [23]. It follows that the “fault” for difficulties in life resides with the individual themselves—and treatments and interventions are designed in such a way as to “correct” said faults.
The archetypal example of this phenomenon comes from the treatment of Autistic people’s “social deficits.” “Pervasive deficits in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts” are, according to diagnostic manuals, hallmark features of being Autistic [18]. These difficulties have long been thought to be rooted in impoverishments in the “theory of mind” [71] and/or “diminished social motivation” [72] and subsequently place Autistic people at a significant disadvantage in terms of some traditional life achievements to which we referred earlier—their ability to develop meaningful friendships and intimate relationships and to obtain and sustain work—and even to achieve “normal human experience” [73]. Seen through the lens of the conventional medical model, many treatments for Autistic people, especially young children, have been devised to correct such “deficiencies” [74, 75], helping to “guide brain and behavioural development back toward a normal pathway” [76, p. 776].
In designating these social difficulties as some sort of deficit needing to be fixed, we have neglected to notice that it is generally the broader context which creates the problematic response, not necessarily the behavior itself. By definition, social interactions are shaped by all parties—but, until recently, there has been remarkably little focus on the nature of these interactions, especially non-autistic contributions to these interactions. Milton’s [77••] influential double empathy problem has done much to address this concern, emphasizing that there may be a bidirectional misalignment between the minds of Autistic and non-autistic people, resulting in a breakdown in reciprocity and mutual understanding. This, in turn, may be a primary source of social communication difficulties between Autistic and non-autistic people [77••, 78•, 79, 80]. It is surely plausible to suggest that attention would be better directed toward social interventions that aim to shift the negative perceptions of Autistic people’s interaction styles and encourage more relational ways of being [81•]. It is the contextual response to the individual that we need to address.
All of this has powerful lessons for what we need to think about. For if the possibilities of flourishing depend not just upon the individual but instead what political philosopher Deva Woodly calls “the individual-in-context” [82], then we need to move our attention away from its sole focus on the individual and appreciate instead what it is that makes anything possible or impossible for them [22••]. That means accepting that the world needs to change, as well as the individual. As Woodly explains: “the context of the individual includes the home, family, community, and nation that they are born into, the physical environment that they exist in, and the structural conditions that link them to socially intelligible categories and political history, therefore organizing the consequences of their being in the world” [82, p. 118]. Without that, our account of flourishing will always remain partial.

From Researcher-Led to Autistic-Led

These four elements of a theory of Autistic flourishing—moving from health to well-being, from other-defined to self-defined, from the big to the small, and from the individual to the contextual—may initially seem self-evident. Consequently, the fifth element of a theory of Autistic flourishing asks why so many of us in the autism research community missed these first four for so long. In part, we believe this is because of a long history of theory of mind research, which suggested that Autistic people have an impaired ability to reflect on their own mental states [71]. This has led to the questioning of the veracity of Autistic people’s accounts of their own experiences: these are often seen as unreliable [83], and researchers have therefore often avoided attending to first-person testimony, privileging reports from other informants [84], or laboratory-based observation over the perspectives of the person themselves [85••, 86, 87].
As Autistic self-advocate Donna Williams [88] described “Right from the start, from the time someone came up with the word “autism,” the condition has been judged from the outside, by its appearances, and not from the inside according to how it is experienced” (p. 14). As well as shaping research findings, this lack of attention to Autistic people’s perspectives has the consequence of ensuring that Autistic people themselves have almost no say as to what gets researched in autism science, why, or how. So Autistic flourishing has not received attention, and we have missed the elements central to understanding it, because we simply have not asked Autistic people what it is for them to flourish. We have, until recently, assumed that, in order to flourish, Autistic people must need to be less Autistic and that their Autistic identity—their Autistic characteristics, traits, and behaviors—could not possibly contribute to their flourishing since (often) it contradicts or problematizes neuronormative or non-autistic experiences of flourishing.
Ever since the onset of autism research, autism science has typically been designed and conducted without any significant input from Autistic people and their families [89]. It is likely this has contributed toward research agendas and methods that rarely relate to the challenges Autistic people face. And when we say “input,” we mean beyond input as passive participants or subjects in research. What is required, in other words, is input in the decision-making processes around research, in the design and implementation of the research, and the analysis and interpretation of the findings, that is, being partners in the research. And, as we know from our own work, that lack of involvement results in real feelings of disenfranchisement [60, 90•, 91, 92].
Encouragingly, in the last decade, this too has begun to change. There is a slow but growing movement toward collaborating with Autistic people and their allies as part of the research process, where Autistic researchers and community members are actively involved in making decisions about research [93, 94], as well as leading research [95••]. These decisions can include what kind of research is done, how it is done, how research results are interpreted, and how the findings are used. These so-called participatory processes draw on the “practical wisdom” of non-scientists and have been shown outside the field of autism to have a dramatic effect on both the research agenda [96] and on the effectiveness of the research [97]. The idea is that we will learn more, understand more, and know more, once we put lived experience and research experience together.

Conclusion

Here, we have sought to re-direct the attention of autism researchers to the foundations of Autistic flourishing. Identifying what a flourishing Autistic life looks like should, we argue, be core to our collective endeavors in the years ahead. Getting that process started means recognizing that subjective well-being matters as well as bodily and mental health; stresses that Autistic people should have the same interest in choosing their own lives as anyone else; accepts that the everyday aspects of life matter to people, not just those that seem big from the outside; understands that the context in which people live always shapes opportunities and is therefore an appropriate subject for research; and contends that we will only notice any of this if we work side-by-side with Autistic people and their allies when designing and conducting research. Together, these points are, we contend, the framework for debating Autistic flourishing well and a means of ensuring that autism research can play an active role in helping to shape good Autistic lives.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Marc Stears for helpful discussion, and comments and edits on the manuscript.

Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Fußnoten
1
In the Autistic community, identity-first language (“Autistic person”) is often preferred to and considered less stigmatizing than person-first language (“person with autism”) [98••, 99, 100]. We therefore use identity-first language throughout. We have also chosen to capitalize the word Autistic to indicate a proper adjective, which reflects its status as a robust and valued identity and shared community, similar to the Deaf community.
 
2
In traditional positivist research paradigms, the researcher attempts to be impartial, minimizing bias in the search for some objective “truth.” Whatever the intent, however, the researcher’s own expertise, experiences, and assumptions undeniably shape the process. EP is a White non-autistic autism researcher and psychologist, committed both to expanding autism science to ensure it examines topics overlooked or systematically ignored but deemed worthy of research by Autistic people themselves and to conduct such research in partnership with Autistic people and their families and allies. MH is a White Autistic autism researcher and an advocate working for and with the Autistic and autism community to nurture Autistic identity and culture and to build Autistic well-being and dignity through education and co-produced research. We are methodological pluralists, believing in the benefits of the social model of disability and a neurodiversity approach to research and practice. The ideas expressed in this piece are the result of many in-depth conversations we have had over the years with each other and with our many collaborators, colleagues and friends, Autistic, and non-autistic.
 
3
We note here that, with the rise of chronic diseases and evidence-based medicine, the ethical imperative of the medical model has broadened to include the cure, prevention, and management of health and biological disease [101].
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Engel GL. The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science. 1977;196(4286):129–36.PubMedCrossRef Engel GL. The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science. 1977;196(4286):129–36.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Koon J. The medical model, with a human face. Philos Stud. 2022;179(12):3747–70.CrossRef Koon J. The medical model, with a human face. Philos Stud. 2022;179(12):3747–70.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Wakefield JC. The concept of mental disorder: diagnostic implications of the harmful dysfunction analysis. World Psychiatry. 2007;6(3):149–56.PubMedPubMedCentral Wakefield JC. The concept of mental disorder: diagnostic implications of the harmful dysfunction analysis. World Psychiatry. 2007;6(3):149–56.PubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Lord C, Charman T, Havdahl A, Carbone P, Anagnostou E, Boyd B, et al. The Lancet Commission on the future of care and clinical research in autism. Lancet. 2022;399(10321):271–334. From a first-of-its-kind commission, this report outlines a series of suggestions to improve autistic people’s medical care and clinical research on the condition. •• Lord C, Charman T, Havdahl A, Carbone P, Anagnostou E, Boyd B, et al. The Lancet Commission on the future of care and clinical research in autism. Lancet. 2022;399(10321):271–334. From a first-of-its-kind commission, this report outlines a series of suggestions to improve autistic people’s medical care and clinical research on the condition.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fusar-Poli P, Salazar de Pablo G, De Micheli A, Nieman DH, Correll CU, Kessing LV, et al. What is good mental health? A scoping review. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;31:33–46. Fusar-Poli P, Salazar de Pablo G, De Micheli A, Nieman DH, Correll CU, Kessing LV, et al. What is good mental health? A scoping review. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;31:33–46.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hogan AJ. Moving away from the “medical model”: the development and revision of the World Health Organization’s classification of disability. Bull Hist Med. 2019;93(2):241–69.PubMedCrossRef Hogan AJ. Moving away from the “medical model”: the development and revision of the World Health Organization’s classification of disability. Bull Hist Med. 2019;93(2):241–69.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Llewellyn A, Hogan K. The use and abuse of models of disability. Disabil Soc. 2000;15(1):157–65.CrossRef Llewellyn A, Hogan K. The use and abuse of models of disability. Disabil Soc. 2000;15(1):157–65.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Mold JW, Blake GH, Becker LA. Goal-oriented medical care. Fam Med. 1991;23(1):46–51.PubMed Mold JW, Blake GH, Becker LA. Goal-oriented medical care. Fam Med. 1991;23(1):46–51.PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat • Botha M, Cage E. “Autism research is in crisis”: a mixed method study of researcher’s constructions of autistic people and autism research. Front Psychol. 2022;13:1050897. This empirical study investigated how autism researchers construct autistic people and autism research. • Botha M, Cage E. “Autism research is in crisis”: a mixed method study of researcher’s constructions of autistic people and autism research. Front Psychol. 2022;13:1050897. This empirical study investigated how autism researchers construct autistic people and autism research.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Gough B, Madill A. Subjectivity in psychological science: from problem to prospect. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(3):374–84.PubMedCrossRef Gough B, Madill A. Subjectivity in psychological science: from problem to prospect. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(3):374–84.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(10):1218–20.PubMedCrossRef Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(10):1218–20.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacob S, Wolff JJ, Steinbach MS, Doyle CB, Kumar V, Elison JT. Neurodevelopmental heterogeneity and computational approaches for understanding autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jacob S, Wolff JJ, Steinbach MS, Doyle CB, Kumar V, Elison JT. Neurodevelopmental heterogeneity and computational approaches for understanding autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Warrier V, Zhang X, Reed P, Havdahl A, Moore TM, Cliquet F, et al. Genetic correlates of phenotypic heterogeneity in autism. Nat Genet. 2022;54(9):1293–304.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Warrier V, Zhang X, Reed P, Havdahl A, Moore TM, Cliquet F, et al. Genetic correlates of phenotypic heterogeneity in autism. Nat Genet. 2022;54(9):1293–304.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Lombardo MV, Lai M-C, Baron-Cohen S. Big data approaches to decomposing heterogeneity across the autism spectrum. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(10):1435–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lombardo MV, Lai M-C, Baron-Cohen S. Big data approaches to decomposing heterogeneity across the autism spectrum. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(10):1435–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Dinishak J. The deficit view and its critics. Disabil Stud Q. 2016;36(4). Dinishak J. The deficit view and its critics. Disabil Stud Q. 2016;36(4).
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson SM. Neurodiversity, quality of life, and autistic adults: shifting research and professional focuses onto real-life challenges. Disabil Stud Q. 2009;30(1). Robertson SM. Neurodiversity, quality of life, and autistic adults: shifting research and professional focuses onto real-life challenges. Disabil Stud Q. 2009;30(1).
18.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (R). Vol.5 No. 5. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2013. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (R). Vol.5 No. 5. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2013.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson M, Mottron L. Do autistics have cognitive strengths? Should ASC be defined as disorders? In: Bölte S, Hallmayer J, editors. Autism spectrum conditions: FAQs on autism, Asperger syndrome, and atypical autism answered by international experts. Boston: Hogrefe; 2011. p. 32–9. Dawson M, Mottron L. Do autistics have cognitive strengths? Should ASC be defined as disorders? In: Bölte S, Hallmayer J, editors. Autism spectrum conditions: FAQs on autism, Asperger syndrome, and atypical autism answered by international experts. Boston: Hogrefe; 2011. p. 32–9.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat • Pellicano E, den Houting J. Annual Research Review: Shifting from “normal science” to neurodiversity in autism science. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63(4):381–96. In this piece, the authors make the case for a radical new way of conceptualizing autism, namely the neurodiversity paradigm. • Pellicano E, den Houting J. Annual Research Review: Shifting from “normal science” to neurodiversity in autism science. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63(4):381–96. In this piece, the authors make the case for a radical new way of conceptualizing autism, namely the neurodiversity paradigm.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Pellicano E, Fatima U, Hall G, Heyworth M, Lawson W, Lilley R, et al. A capabilities approach to understanding and supporting autistic adulthood. Nat Rev Psychol. 2022;1(11):624–39. In this review, a team of autistic and non-autistic researchers re-examines adult autism research through a capabilities approach, highlighting issues to which researchers, clinicians, and policymakers should respond. •• Pellicano E, Fatima U, Hall G, Heyworth M, Lawson W, Lilley R, et al. A capabilities approach to understanding and supporting autistic adulthood. Nat Rev Psychol. 2022;1(11):624–39. In this review, a team of autistic and non-autistic researchers re-examines adult autism research through a capabilities approach, highlighting issues to which researchers, clinicians, and policymakers should respond.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillespie-Lynch K, Kapp SK, Brooks PJ, Pickens J, Schwartzman B. Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts. Front Psychol. 2017;8:438.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gillespie-Lynch K, Kapp SK, Brooks PJ, Pickens J, Schwartzman B. Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts. Front Psychol. 2017;8:438.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization (WHO). Constitution of the World Health Organization. Basic Documents, Geneva World Health Organization. 1946. World Health Organization (WHO). Constitution of the World Health Organization. Basic Documents, Geneva World Health Organization. 1946.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Bölte S. A more holistic approach to autism using the International Classification of Functioning: the why, what, and how of functioning. Autism. 2023;27(1):3–6.PubMedCrossRef Bölte S. A more holistic approach to autism using the International Classification of Functioning: the why, what, and how of functioning. Autism. 2023;27(1):3–6.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Bölte S, Lawson WB, Marschik PB, Girdler S. Reconciling the seemingly irreconcilable: the WHO’s ICF system integrates biological and psychosocial environmental determinants of autism and ADHD. BioEssays. 2021;43(9):e2000254. Bölte S, Lawson WB, Marschik PB, Girdler S. Reconciling the seemingly irreconcilable: the WHO’s ICF system integrates biological and psychosocial environmental determinants of autism and ADHD. BioEssays. 2021;43(9):e2000254.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Imrie R. Demystifying disability: a review of the International Classification of Functioning. Disabil Health Soc Health Illn. 2004;26(3):287–305.CrossRef Imrie R. Demystifying disability: a review of the International Classification of Functioning. Disabil Health Soc Health Illn. 2004;26(3):287–305.CrossRef
29.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat McConachie H, Wilson C, Mason D, Garland D, Parr JR, Rattazzi A, et al. What is important in measuring quality of life? Reflections by autistic adults in four countries. Autism Adulthood. 2020;2(1):4–12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McConachie H, Wilson C, Mason D, Garland D, Parr JR, Rattazzi A, et al. What is important in measuring quality of life? Reflections by autistic adults in four countries. Autism Adulthood. 2020;2(1):4–12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat • Ne’eman A. When disability is defined by behavior, outcome measures should not promote “passing.” AMA J Ethics. 2021;23(7):E569–575. This opinion piece presents a powerful call to researchers to rethink the measures they use to evaluate autistic people’s outcomes. • Ne’eman A. When disability is defined by behavior, outcome measures should not promote “passing.” AMA J Ethics. 2021;23(7):E569–575. This opinion piece presents a powerful call to researchers to rethink the measures they use to evaluate autistic people’s outcomes.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(3):351–60.PubMedCrossRef Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(3):351–60.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Eaves LC, Ho HH. Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(4):739–47.PubMedCrossRef Eaves LC, Ho HH. Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(4):739–47.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Farley MA, McMahon WM, Fombonne E, Jenson WR, Miller J, Gardner M, et al. Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Res. 2009;2(2):109–18.PubMedCrossRef Farley MA, McMahon WM, Fombonne E, Jenson WR, Miller J, Gardner M, et al. Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Res. 2009;2(2):109–18.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Henninger NA, Taylor JL. Outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a historical perspective. Autism. 2013;17(1):103–16.PubMedCrossRef Henninger NA, Taylor JL. Outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a historical perspective. Autism. 2013;17(1):103–16.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Howlin P, Magiati I. Autism spectrum disorder: outcomes in adulthood. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2017;30(2):69–76.PubMedCrossRef Howlin P, Magiati I. Autism spectrum disorder: outcomes in adulthood. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2017;30(2):69–76.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Lam GYH, Sabnis S, Migueliz Valcarlos M, Wolgemuth JR. A critical review of academic literature constructing well-being in autistic adults. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(1):61–71. This review critiques how well-being in autistic adults is constructed in current research, which constrains our understanding of autistic well-being. The authors urge researchers to be open to diversifying their methods of enquiry and their research questions. •• Lam GYH, Sabnis S, Migueliz Valcarlos M, Wolgemuth JR. A critical review of academic literature constructing well-being in autistic adults. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(1):61–71. This review critiques how well-being in autistic adults is constructed in current research, which constrains our understanding of autistic well-being. The authors urge researchers to be open to diversifying their methods of enquiry and their research questions.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat • Murray D, Milton D, Green J, Bervoets J. The human spectrum: a phenomenological enquiry within neurodiversity. Psychopathology. 2023;56(3):220–30. This study used a novel phenomenological approach to identify patterns of (dis)similarities within the wider “human” spectrum of experience. • Murray D, Milton D, Green J, Bervoets J. The human spectrum: a phenomenological enquiry within neurodiversity. Psychopathology. 2023;56(3):220–30. This study used a novel phenomenological approach to identify patterns of (dis)similarities within the wider “human” spectrum of experience.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Hong J, Smith LE, Makuch RA, Greenberg JS, Mailick MR. Characterizing objective quality of life and normative outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorder: an exploratory latent class analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(8):2707–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Hong J, Smith LE, Makuch RA, Greenberg JS, Mailick MR. Characterizing objective quality of life and normative outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorder: an exploratory latent class analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(8):2707–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Müller E, Cannon L. Parent perspectives on outcomes and satisfaction levels of young adults with autism and cognitive impairments. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2016;31(2):92–103.CrossRef Müller E, Cannon L. Parent perspectives on outcomes and satisfaction levels of young adults with autism and cognitive impairments. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2016;31(2):92–103.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Pellicano E, Cribb S, Kenny L. Patterns of continuity and change in the psychosocial outcomes of young autistic people: a mixed-methods study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(2):301–13.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano E, Cribb S, Kenny L. Patterns of continuity and change in the psychosocial outcomes of young autistic people: a mixed-methods study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(2):301–13.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruble LA, Dalrymple NJ. An alternative view of outcome in autism. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 1996;11(1):3–14.CrossRef Ruble LA, Dalrymple NJ. An alternative view of outcome in autism. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 1996;11(1):3–14.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Leadbitter K, Buckle KL, Ellis C, Dekker M. Autistic self-advocacy and the neurodiversity movement: implications for autism early intervention research and practice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:635690. This perspective piece from a team of autistic and non-autistic researchers makes the case for the relevance of neurodiversity as a concept and movement in autism intervention research. •• Leadbitter K, Buckle KL, Ellis C, Dekker M. Autistic self-advocacy and the neurodiversity movement: implications for autism early intervention research and practice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:635690. This perspective piece from a team of autistic and non-autistic researchers makes the case for the relevance of neurodiversity as a concept and movement in autism intervention research.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat • Späth EMA, Jongsma KR. Autism, autonomy, and authenticity. Med Health Care Philos. 2020;23(1):73–80. This perspective piece challenges the assumption that autistic people lack the capacity to be self-reflective. • Späth EMA, Jongsma KR. Autism, autonomy, and authenticity. Med Health Care Philos. 2020;23(1):73–80. This perspective piece challenges the assumption that autistic people lack the capacity to be self-reflective.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Shogren KA, Little TD, Wehmeyer ML. Human agentic theories and the development of self-determination. In: Wehmeyer M, Shogren K, Little T, Lopez S, editors. Development of self-determination through the life-course. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2017. p. 17–26.CrossRef Shogren KA, Little TD, Wehmeyer ML. Human agentic theories and the development of self-determination. In: Wehmeyer M, Shogren K, Little T, Lopez S, editors. Development of self-determination through the life-course. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2017. p. 17–26.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan RM, Huta V, Deci EL. Living well: a self-determination theory perspective on eudaimonia. J Happiness Stud. 2008;9(1):139–70.CrossRef Ryan RM, Huta V, Deci EL. Living well: a self-determination theory perspective on eudaimonia. J Happiness Stud. 2008;9(1):139–70.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim SY. The experiences of adults with autism spectrum disorder: self-determination and quality of life. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2019;60:1–15.CrossRef Kim SY. The experiences of adults with autism spectrum disorder: self-determination and quality of life. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2019;60:1–15.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat White K, Flanagan TD, Nadig A. Examining the relationship between self-determination and quality of life in young adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2018;30(6):735–54.CrossRef White K, Flanagan TD, Nadig A. Examining the relationship between self-determination and quality of life in young adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2018;30(6):735–54.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Cribb S, Kenny L, Pellicano E. “I definitely feel more in control of my life”: the perspectives of young autistic people and their parents on emerging adulthood. Autism. 2019;23(7):1765–81.PubMedCrossRef Cribb S, Kenny L, Pellicano E. “I definitely feel more in control of my life”: the perspectives of young autistic people and their parents on emerging adulthood. Autism. 2019;23(7):1765–81.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Kittay EF. The ethics of care, dependence, and disability: the ethics of care, dependence, and disability. Ratio Juris. 2011;24(1):49–58.CrossRef Kittay EF. The ethics of care, dependence, and disability: the ethics of care, dependence, and disability. Ratio Juris. 2011;24(1):49–58.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Lawson WB. Older adults and autism spectrum conditions: an introduction and guide. London, England: Jessica Kingsley; 2015. Lawson WB. Older adults and autism spectrum conditions: an introduction and guide. London, England: Jessica Kingsley; 2015.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Oliver M. Social policy and disability: some theoretical issues. Disabil Handicap Soc. 1986;1(1):5–17.CrossRef Oliver M. Social policy and disability: some theoretical issues. Disabil Handicap Soc. 1986;1(1):5–17.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Mill JS. On liberty. Ontario, CA: Batouche Books Limited; 1859/2001. Mill JS. On liberty. Ontario, CA: Batouche Books Limited; 1859/2001.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Kapp SK. Profound concerns about “profound autism”: dangers of severity scales and functioning labels for support needs. Educ Sci. 2023;13(2):106. This state-of-the-art review challenges the validity of autism sublabels and urges researchers and clinicians to personalize provision for each individual’s communicative and cognitive capacities. •• Kapp SK. Profound concerns about “profound autism”: dangers of severity scales and functioning labels for support needs. Educ Sci. 2023;13(2):106. This state-of-the-art review challenges the validity of autism sublabels and urges researchers and clinicians to personalize provision for each individual’s communicative and cognitive capacities.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Wehmeyer ML. The importance of self-determination to the quality of life of people with intellectual disability: a perspective. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7121.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wehmeyer ML. The importance of self-determination to the quality of life of people with intellectual disability: a perspective. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7121.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Stancliffe RJ. Living with support in the community: predictors of choice and self-determination. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2001;7(2):91–8.PubMedCrossRef Stancliffe RJ. Living with support in the community: predictors of choice and self-determination. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2001;7(2):91–8.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Pellicano E, Dinsmore A, Charman T. Views on researcher-community engagement in autism research in the United Kingdom: a mixed-methods study. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109946. Pellicano E, Dinsmore A, Charman T. Views on researcher-community engagement in autism research in the United Kingdom: a mixed-methods study. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109946.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Roche L, Adams D, Clark M. Research priorities of the autism community: a systematic review of key stakeholder perspectives. Autism. 2021;25(2):336–48.PubMedCrossRef Roche L, Adams D, Clark M. Research priorities of the autism community: a systematic review of key stakeholder perspectives. Autism. 2021;25(2):336–48.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR, Maurer RG. A neurological model for childhood autism. Arch Neurol. 1978;35(12):777–86.PubMedCrossRef Damasio AR, Maurer RG. A neurological model for childhood autism. Arch Neurol. 1978;35(12):777–86.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenworthy L, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, Wallace GL. Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18(4):320–38.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kenworthy L, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, Wallace GL. Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18(4):320–38.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Demetriou EA, Lampit A, Quintana DS, Naismith SL, Song YJC, Pye JE, et al. Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23(5):1198–204.PubMedCrossRef Demetriou EA, Lampit A, Quintana DS, Naismith SL, Song YJC, Pye JE, et al. Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23(5):1198–204.PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenny L, Remington A, Pellicano E. Everyday executive function issues from the perspectives of autistic adolescents and their parents: theoretical and empirical implications. Manuscript in submission. Kenny L, Remington A, Pellicano E. Everyday executive function issues from the perspectives of autistic adolescents and their parents: theoretical and empirical implications. Manuscript in submission.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Heyworth M, Brett S, den Houting J, Magiati I, Steward R, Urbanowicz A, et al. “I’m the family ringmaster and juggler”: autistic parents’ experiences of parenting during the COVID-19 pandemic. Autism Adulthood. 2023;5(1):24–36.PubMed Heyworth M, Brett S, den Houting J, Magiati I, Steward R, Urbanowicz A, et al. “I’m the family ringmaster and juggler”: autistic parents’ experiences of parenting during the COVID-19 pandemic. Autism Adulthood. 2023;5(1):24–36.PubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Kingstone A, Smilek D, Eastwood JD. Cognitive ethology: a new approach for studying human cognition. Br J Psychol. 2008;99(Pt 3):317–40.PubMedCrossRef Kingstone A, Smilek D, Eastwood JD. Cognitive ethology: a new approach for studying human cognition. Br J Psychol. 2008;99(Pt 3):317–40.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Burgess PW, Alderman N, Forbes C, Costello A, Laure MC, Dawson DR, et al. The case for the development and use of “ecologically valid” measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(2):194–209. Burgess PW, Alderman N, Forbes C, Costello A, Laure MC, Dawson DR, et al. The case for the development and use of “ecologically valid” measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(2):194–209.
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Pellicano E, Brett S, den Houting J, Heyworth M, Magiati I, Steward R, et al. COVID-19, social isolation and the mental health of autistic people and their families: a qualitative study. Autism. 2022;26(4):914–27.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano E, Brett S, den Houting J, Heyworth M, Magiati I, Steward R, et al. COVID-19, social isolation and the mental health of autistic people and their families: a qualitative study. Autism. 2022;26(4):914–27.PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46.PubMedCrossRef
72.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Bottema-Beutel K, Park H, Kim SY. Commentary on social skills training curricula for individuals with ASD: social interaction, authenticity, and stigma. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(3):953–64.PubMedCrossRef Bottema-Beutel K, Park H, Kim SY. Commentary on social skills training curricula for individuals with ASD: social interaction, authenticity, and stigma. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(3):953–64.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Fletcher-Watson S, McConnell F, Manola E, McConachie H. Interventions based on the theory of mind cognitive model for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014;2014(3):CD008785. Fletcher-Watson S, McConnell F, Manola E, McConachie H. Interventions based on the theory of mind cognitive model for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014;2014(3):CD008785.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson G. Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2008 Summer;20(3):775–803. Dawson G. Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2008 Summer;20(3):775–803.
77.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Milton DEM. On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem.’ Disabil Soc. 2012;27(6):883–7. This influential paper proposes a misalignment between the minds of autistic and non-autistic people and highlights the imitations in many non-autistic people’s efforts to communicate with autistic people. •• Milton DEM. On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem.’ Disabil Soc. 2012;27(6):883–7. This influential paper proposes a misalignment between the minds of autistic and non-autistic people and highlights the imitations in many non-autistic people’s efforts to communicate with autistic people.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat • Davis R, Crompton CJ. What do new findings about social interaction in autistic adults mean for neurodevelopmental research? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2021;16(3):649–53. This paper reviews research examining the autistic-led notion of the “double-empathy problem” and discusses the potential implications of this new way of thinking. • Davis R, Crompton CJ. What do new findings about social interaction in autistic adults mean for neurodevelopmental research? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2021;16(3):649–53. This paper reviews research examining the autistic-led notion of the “double-empathy problem” and discusses the potential implications of this new way of thinking.
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Milton DEM, Heasman B, Sheppard E. Double empathy. In: Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2018. p. 1–8. Milton DEM, Heasman B, Sheppard E. Double empathy. In: Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2018. p. 1–8.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell P, Sheppard E, Cassidy S. Autism and the double empathy problem: implications for development and mental health. Br J Dev Psychol. 2021;39(1):1–18.PubMedCrossRef Mitchell P, Sheppard E, Cassidy S. Autism and the double empathy problem: implications for development and mental health. Br J Dev Psychol. 2021;39(1):1–18.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat • De Jaegher H. Seeing and inviting participation in autistic interactions. Transcult Psychiatry. 2021;13634615211009628. This paper puts forth a new approach, based on the enactive theory of participatory sense-making, for understanding how we interact across differences. • De Jaegher H. Seeing and inviting participation in autistic interactions. Transcult Psychiatry. 2021;13634615211009628. This paper puts forth a new approach, based on the enactive theory of participatory sense-making, for understanding how we interact across differences.
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodly D. On flourishing: political economy and the pursuit of well-being in the polity. In: Allen D, Benkler Y, Downey L, Henderson R, Simons J, editors. A political economy of justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2022. p. 118–39. Woodly D. On flourishing: political economy and the pursuit of well-being in the polity. In: Allen D, Benkler Y, Downey L, Henderson R, Simons J, editors. A political economy of justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2022. p. 118–39.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Frith U, Happe F. Theory of mind and self-consciousness: what is it like to be autistic? Mind Lang. 1999;14(1):82–9.CrossRef Frith U, Happe F. Theory of mind and self-consciousness: what is it like to be autistic? Mind Lang. 1999;14(1):82–9.CrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Mazefsky CA, Kao J, Oswald DP. Preliminary evidence suggesting caution in the use of psychiatric self-report measures with adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5(1):164–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mazefsky CA, Kao J, Oswald DP. Preliminary evidence suggesting caution in the use of psychiatric self-report measures with adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5(1):164–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Jaswal VK, Akhtar N. Being versus appearing socially uninterested: challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism. Behav Brain Sci. 2018;42(e82):e82. This perspective piece challenges widely held ideas about autistic sociality and urges researchers to take autistic testimony seriously in research. •• Jaswal VK, Akhtar N. Being versus appearing socially uninterested: challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism. Behav Brain Sci. 2018;42(e82):e82. This perspective piece challenges widely held ideas about autistic sociality and urges researchers to take autistic testimony seriously in research.
86.
Zurück zum Zitat McGeer V. Autistic self-awareness. Philos Psychiatr Psychol. 2004;11(3):235–51.CrossRef McGeer V. Autistic self-awareness. Philos Psychiatr Psychol. 2004;11(3):235–51.CrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Milton DE. Autistic expertise: a critical reflection on the production of knowledge in autism studies. Autism. 2014;18(7):794–802. This paper draws on sociological theory to consider the various claims to knowledge production and expertise within autism research. Milton DE. Autistic expertise: a critical reflection on the production of knowledge in autism studies. Autism. 2014;18(7):794–802. This paper draws on sociological theory to consider the various claims to knowledge production and expertise within autism research.
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams D. Autism: an inside-out approach: an innovative look at the “mechanics” of “autism” and its developmental “cousins”. London, England: Jessica Kingsley; 1996. Williams D. Autism: an inside-out approach: an innovative look at the “mechanics” of “autism” and its developmental “cousins”. London, England: Jessica Kingsley; 1996.
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Pellicano E, Stears M. Bridging autism, science and society: moving toward an ethically informed approach to autism research. Autism Res. 2011;4(4):271–82.PubMedCrossRef Pellicano E, Stears M. Bridging autism, science and society: moving toward an ethically informed approach to autism research. Autism Res. 2011;4(4):271–82.PubMedCrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat • Botha M. Academic, activist, or advocate? Angry, entangled, and emerging: a critical reflection on autism knowledge production. Front Psychol. 2021;12: 727542. This piece is a powerful reflection on how the differing roles of an autistic activist and autism research can intersect to inform autism research. • Botha M. Academic, activist, or advocate? Angry, entangled, and emerging: a critical reflection on autism knowledge production. Front Psychol. 2021;12: 727542. This piece is a powerful reflection on how the differing roles of an autistic activist and autism research can intersect to inform autism research.
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Fletcher-Watson S, Adams J, Brook K, Charman T, Crane L, Cusack J, et al. Making the future together: shaping autism research through meaningful participation. Autism. 2019;23(4):943–53.PubMedCrossRef Fletcher-Watson S, Adams J, Brook K, Charman T, Crane L, Cusack J, et al. Making the future together: shaping autism research through meaningful participation. Autism. 2019;23(4):943–53.PubMedCrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicolaidis C, Raymaker D, Kapp SK, Baggs A, Ashkenazy E, McDonald K, et al. The AASPIRE practice-based guidelines for the inclusion of autistic adults in research as co-researchers and study participants. Autism. 2019;23(8):2007–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nicolaidis C, Raymaker D, Kapp SK, Baggs A, Ashkenazy E, McDonald K, et al. The AASPIRE practice-based guidelines for the inclusion of autistic adults in research as co-researchers and study participants. Autism. 2019;23(8):2007–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Bertilsdotter Rosqvist H, Botha M, Hens K, O’Donoghue S, Pearson A, Stenning A. Cutting our own keys: new possibilities of neurodivergent storying in research. Autism. 2022;13623613221132108. This paper considers how to create better spaces for researchers—of all neurotypes—to work together. •• Bertilsdotter Rosqvist H, Botha M, Hens K, O’Donoghue S, Pearson A, Stenning A. Cutting our own keys: new possibilities of neurodivergent storying in research. Autism. 2022;13623613221132108. This paper considers how to create better spaces for researchers—of all neurotypes—to work together.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat den Houting J, Pellicano E. A portfolio analysis of autism research funding in Australia, 2008–2017. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(11):4400–8.CrossRef den Houting J, Pellicano E. A portfolio analysis of autism research funding in Australia, 2008–2017. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(11):4400–8.CrossRef
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Pellicano E, Lawson W, Hall G, Mahony J, Lilley R, Heyworth M, et al. “I knew she’d get it, and get me”: participants’ perspectives of a participatory autism research project. Autism Adulthood. 2022;4(2):120–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pellicano E, Lawson W, Hall G, Mahony J, Lilley R, Heyworth M, et al. “I knew she’d get it, and get me”: participants’ perspectives of a participatory autism research project. Autism Adulthood. 2022;4(2):120–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Bottema-Beutel K, Kapp SK, Lester JN, Sasson NJ, Hand BN. Avoiding ableist language: suggestions for autism researchers. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(1):18–29. This article analyzes how ableist ideologies have influenced autism research and provides strategies for avoiding ableist language in researchers’ future work. •• Bottema-Beutel K, Kapp SK, Lester JN, Sasson NJ, Hand BN. Avoiding ableist language: suggestions for autism researchers. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(1):18–29. This article analyzes how ableist ideologies have influenced autism research and provides strategies for avoiding ableist language in researchers’ future work.
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Gernsbacher MA. Editorial Perspective: The use of person-first language in scholarly writing may accentuate stigma. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(7):859–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gernsbacher MA. Editorial Perspective: The use of person-first language in scholarly writing may accentuate stigma. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(7):859–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenny L, Hattersley C, Molins B, Buckley C, Povey C, Pellicano E. Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism. 2016;20(4):442–62.PubMedCrossRef Kenny L, Hattersley C, Molins B, Buckley C, Povey C, Pellicano E. Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism. 2016;20(4):442–62.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The Foundations of Autistic Flourishing
verfasst von
Elizabeth Pellicano
Melanie Heyworth
Publikationsdatum
08.08.2023
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Current Psychiatry Reports / Ausgabe 9/2023
Print ISSN: 1523-3812
Elektronische ISSN: 1535-1645
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-023-01441-9

Neu im Fachgebiet Psychiatrie

Einer von sieben Frauen macht die Menopause sehr zu schaffen

Von mäßigen bis schweren vasomotorischen Beschwerden sind 14,7% der Frauen in der Postmenopause betroffen. Das haben kanadische Forscherinnen in einer Subgruppenanalyse der WARM-Studie herausgefunden.

Depressive Senioren fahren besonders riskant

Beim Autofahren machen ältere Depressive häufiger eine Vollbremsung, gehen öfter zu schnell in Kurven und halten sich seltener an Tempolimits als Senioren ohne Depression. Offenbar scheint eine Depression eine riskante Fahrweise im Alter zu verstärken.

Verhaltenstherapie plus Stimulanz reduziert Binge Eating

Die Kombination von kognitiver Verhaltenstherapie plus Lisdexamfetamin bringt Personen mit Binge-Eating-Störung deutlich häufiger in Remission als die jeweiligen Einzeltherapien. Zudem nimmt das Gewicht der Betroffenen spürbar ab.

Die elektronische Patientenakte kommt: Das sollten Sie jetzt wissen

Am 15. Januar geht die „ePA für alle“ zunächst in den Modellregionen an den Start. Doch schon bald soll sie in allen Praxen zum Einsatz kommen. Was ist jetzt zu tun? Was müssen Sie wissen? Wir geben in einem FAQ Antworten auf 21 Fragen.

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.