Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics 1/2020

Open Access 01.12.2020 | Research article

The influence of the age of dementia onset on college students’ stigmatic attributions towards a person with dementia

verfasst von: Perla Werner, Lilach Raviv-Turgeman, Patrick W. Corrigan

Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics | Ausgabe 1/2020

Abstract

Background

Research in the area of public stigma and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is limited to examining stigmatic beliefs towards persons aged 65 and over (i.e., persons with late-onset dementia). The aim of the present study was to compare college students’ stigmatic attributions towards an older and a younger person with AD, using an attributional model of stigma.

Method

A cross-sectional study was conducted with 375 college students (mean age = 25.5, 58.9% female, 64.3% Jewish) who answered a computerized, self-administered, structured questionnaire after being presented with one of two randomly distributed vignettes varying in the age of the person with AD – 80 or 50 years of age. Cognitive, emotional and behavioral attributions of stigma were assessed using an adapted version of the Attribution Questionnaire. Other variables examined included background information, experiences and concerns about developing AD. T-tests and Ordinary Least Square (OLS) hierarchical regressions were used to analyze results.

Results

Similar to previous studies, students’ levels of dementia stigma were low to moderate. Negative attributions were consistently and significantly higher (β = .17 to .33, p < .01), and positive attributions were significantly lower (β = −.26, p < .01) when the target person was younger rather than older.

Conclusion

The differences in stigmatic beliefs towards a younger and older person with AD point to the theoretical and practical importance of clearly stating the age of the target person in stigma studies as well as in programs aimed at reducing public stigma towards persons with AD.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AD
Alzheimer’s disease
LOD
Late-onset dementia
OLS
Ordinary Least Squares
VIF
Variance inflation factor
YOD
Young-onset dementia

Background

Driven by the increasing rates of dementia worldwide [1], public health and research attention is currently dominated by efforts to decrease stigmatic beliefs towards persons with dementia, in general, and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), in particular. This is reflected in all National Dementia Strategy programs implemented in a variety of countries [2], as well as in the increasing amount of studies assessing stigma among the general public (see reviews by Herrmann et al. 2018 [3]; Nguyen & Li, 2018 [4];Werner, 2014 [5]). Despite the importance of the studies assessing public stigma towards persons with AD, it should be noted that they have predominantly concentrated on stigma directed towards persons with late-onset dementia (LOD). While LOD, defined as onset dementia at the age of 65 and over, is the most common type of dementia, assessing stigmatic beliefs towards persons with onset dementia under the age of 65 – called young-onset dementia (YOD) – is very important for several reasons. First, although sharing the same neuropathological characteristics, both types of dementia differ in several features such as the rate of progression of the disease, genetic characteristic, and the presentation of behavioral problems [6]. Second, an extensive body of research has demonstrated that sigmatic experiences are common in the lives of persons with YOD, as well as among those surrounding them, such as family and professional caregivers [710]. Finally, since the internalization of stigma is closely associated to the stigmatic beliefs held by the public [1113], evaluating public stigma towards younger persons with the disease – i.e., with young-onset dementia - is of the utmost importance.
Thus, the aim of the present study was to address this gap by comparing college students’ stigmatic beliefs towards an older and a younger person with AD, using an attributional model of stigma, which assumes that stigma includes three types of attributions: cognitive attributions or stereotypes about the person with the disease; emotional attributions, including negative and positive emotions; and behavioral attributions, including behavioral discrimination as well as willingness to help. Wile this conceptualization was developed originally for mental illness stigma [14], a recent study demonstrated that public stigma in the area of dementia is characterized by a similar cognitive, emotional, and behavioral process [15].

Methods

Study design and participants

This study used a cross-sectional design with a convenience sample of college students.
Using Green formula [16] the minimum sample required was 322. By assuming 30% dropout rate, a total of 494 college students were asked to participate in the study. Of these, 119 were excluded: nine because of language problems and 110 because they did not complete the questionnaire in its entirety. Thus, data from 375 students were available, rendering a response rate of 75.9%. The majority of the participants were female (58.9%), Jewish (64.3%), born in Israel (58.4%), not married (86.4%). Regarding their study characteristics, the majority were undergraduate students (85.6%), half (51.5%) studied social sciences, 31.5% exact sciences, and the rest humanities and health studies. Their mean age was 25.5 years (SD = 4.8, range 18–51).

Measures

The following instruments were used:

Dependent variables - public stigma towards a person with AD

An adapted version of the Attribution Questionnaire 27 [AQ-27; 14] was used. The adaptation entailed replacing mental illness with AD, and the inclusion of two items assessing lack of aesthetics: To what extent do you think Sara is filthy/smelly?). The inclusion of items assessing aesthetic was based on previous findings stressing the importance of these cognitive attributions for dementia stigma [13, 15]. All items were rated on a 9-point, Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 = not at all to 9 = very much, and tapping three dimensions of stigma – cognitive attributions (dangerousness, responsibility, and lack of aesthetics), emotional reactions (negative and positive), and discriminatory behavior (segregation, treatment coercion, and helping behaviors). The adapted version of the instrument was validated in a previous study, and good to very good internal reliability (Cronbach alphas ranging from .70 for responsibility to .86 for negative emotions, were found [15].

Independent variables

These included socio-demographic characteristics, and health beliefs regarding AD.
Socio-demographic characteristics included age, gender (male and female), majority (Jewish) or minority (non-Jewish) group, and area of study (health and other).
Experiences and concerns of developing AD: These included familiarity with the disease, and worry about developing it.
Familiarity was assessed by asking participants if they knew someone with Alzheimer’s disease among their relatives or acquaintances.
Worry about developing Alzheimer’s disease was assessed by a single question: “How much do you worry that you will develop Alzheimer’s disease?” Answers were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 = not at all worried to 5 = very worried.

Procedure

Participants were recruited opportunistically from various colleges in the Northern part of Israel. They were asked to answer a computerized structured questionnaire after being presented with a vignette developed and used in a previous study [17], but describing a female instead of male person with AD (Sara), aged 80 years old (LOD) or 50 years old (YOD). Besides of the age of the person, both vignettes were identical. Each version was assigned randomly, with 159 participants being exposed to the YOD vignette and 211 to the LOD vignette. No statistically significant differences were found in the socio-demographic or academic characteristics of the participants in each group or in their health beliefs regarding AD. It took approximately 15 min to complete the questionnaire.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics (percentages, means, and standard deviations) were used to describe the sample and the main variables. T-tests were used to assess differences in attributions of stigma according to the age of the person with AD described in the vignette. Finally, Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) hierarchical regressions were conducted in order to examine the effect of the age of the person with AD on stigmatic attributions in each one of the stigma dimensions. In the first step of the regressions, we included the age of the person with AD as presented in the vignettes. In the second step, sociodemographic factors were entered, followed by health beliefs about Alzheimer’s disease, in the third step. We tested for multicollinearity, and the results indicated that it was not a concern in our model. Variance inflation factor (VIF) did not exceed 2.3.

Ethical considerations

The study’s protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Haifa. All potential participants were given and read an informed consent form describing the importance of the study, its anonymity, and the possibility to refuse to participate without any consequences. The students were required to sign an informed consent form before starting to answer the questionnaire.

Results

Overall, as can be observed in Fig. 1, regardless of which vignette was presented and with the exception of coercion, college students’ stigmatic attributions were below the neutral score of 4.5, reflecting low to moderate levels of stigma towards a person with AD. However, participants exposed to the vignette presenting a person with YOD reported significantly higher levels of cognitive attributions, negative emotions, and attributions of segregation and coercion, than those exposed to the LOD vignette. Positive emotions and willingness to help were significantly lower in the group exposed to the YOD, compared to those exposed to the LOD vignette. Moreover, results of the hierarchical regressions (Table 1) showed that the age of the person with AD had the strongest influence on explaining dangerousness (R2 = .10), negative reactions (R2 = .10), positive reactions (R2 = .06), treatment coercion (R2 = .04), and helping behaviors (R2 = .06). While age, gender, and area of study were not significantly associated with any of the stigma variables, participants pertaining to a minority (non-Jewish) group reported higher levels of stigmatic beliefs compared to those pertaining to a majority (Jewish) group. More specifically, this variable was the most important determinant of lack of aesthetics (it increased the explained variance by 4% when added in Step 2), as well as of responsibility (it increased the explained variance by 10% when added in Step 2), and institutionalization (it increased the explained variance by 7% when added in Step 2). Experiences and concerns about developing AD had a very small influence on explaining stigma attributions for all dimensions.
Table 1
Hierarchical regressions assessing the effect of the age of the person described in the vignette (n = 375)
 
Cognitive dimension
Emotional reactions
Behavioral attributions
Dangerousness
Lack of Aesthetics
Responsibility
Negative Reactions
Positive Reactions
Institutionalization
Treatment Coercion
Helping
Step 1
LOD/YOD
.33***
.17**
.20**
.33***
−.26***
.21***
.19***
−.26***
Adjusted R2
.10
.02
.03
.10
.06
.04
.04
.06
Step 2
Age
.01
.05
.03
.01
.03
−.07
.03
−.03
Gender
−.01
.03
−.05
−.03
.10
−.07
.01
.10
Jewish/Non-Jewish
.21***
.19*
.29***
.20**
.01
.27***
.18**
−.02
Area of study
−.09
−.07
−.17**
−.13*
.10
.02
.09
.05
Adjusted R2
.14
.06
.13
.15
.06
.11
.06
.06
Step 3
Familiarity with AD
.02
−.14*
.11*
−.01
−.07
.02
−.17**
−.01
Concern about AD
.04
.04
.02
.06
.09
.10
.20***
.14*
Total Adjusted R2
.14
.07
.15
.15
.07
.12
.10
.08
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .00

Discussion

This study investigated whether differences exist among college students’ stigmatic attributions towards a person with YOD or LOD. Overall, similar to other studies conducted with the general public [15, 1823], stigmatic attributions towards a person with AD were low to moderate in our sample of relatively young college students. This calls for special attention, particularly since it was established that stigmatic processes, in general, begin at a young age [24], and specifically in regards to dementia, stigmatic attributions increase with age [22, 25, 26] .
Most importantly, our study expands these findings by demonstrating that stigmatic attributions elicited by a person with AD vary according to the age of the target person. Indeed, college students consistently reported higher stigmatic attributions when presented with a younger person with dementia than with an older one. Several explanations might be provided for this finding: some relate to the concept of stigma in the area of AD and some to general attitudes towards older persons.
Public stigma is defined as laypersons’ perceptions of a person or a group as having a cue or mark which makes them different or abnormal [27]. Given the characteristics of AD and its clear association with older age [28], we can hypothesize that the higher levels of stigmatic beliefs reported toward the younger person with AD were a result of her being perceived as “different” or “abnormal”. This explanation is supported by studies examining persons with YOD and their caregivers, which described stigma as a reoccurring theme because of the uncommon situation of encountering a relatively young person with the disease [29]. Moreover, it has been stated that laypersons’ stigmatic beliefs are associated with perceptions of threat associated with the stigmatized group [30]. In view of the relatively young age of our sample, we can assume that the feelings of threat will be higher for those exposed to the vignette describing a person closer to their age.
Alternatively, we can assume that the differences in stigmatic beliefs associated with the age of the person with AD do not stem, as suggested above, from higher stigmatic beliefs directed towards the younger person, but from lower stigmatic beliefs towards the older person with AD caused by paternalistic stereotypes. While paternalistic stereotypes, defined as attributions attached to a person as needing care and help, were previously studied mainly in regard to persons with mental illness [31], studies showed that older healthy targets also elicit more positive emotions than younger targets, even if they are perceived as less competent [32, 33].
Finally, the age of the person described in the vignette remained a significant predictor of all stigmatic attributions, even after including other variables. Moreover, it emerged as the most important determinant for two negative attributions (dangerousness, and negative reactions), and the second most important determinant for the rest of the attributions, apart from ethnicity. These findings call for future studies in the area of dementia stigma to explicitly state the age of the person with the disease.
In addition to the contribution of the age of the person with AD to the explanation of stigmatic attributions, our findings showed that pertaining to a minority (non-Jewish) group was consistently associated with increased stigma. A similar trend was found in an Israeli sample of high school students [34], stressing the importance of cultural factors to the formation of stigmatic beliefs. Finally, the lack of significant relationships with health beliefs is noteworthy, as this is a central factor in attribution models of stigma [14]. While this might be a result of the relatively young age of our sample, future studies should try to further examine these associations.
Several limitations should be noted before we discuss the implications of our results. First, the study employed a non-random sample. Participation in the study was voluntary and no information was available about the characteristics of those who did not participate. Finally, the cross-sectional design of our study does not allow for a causal interpretation of the results.
Our findings should also be considered in the context of our measurement methods. The use of self-reported measures might be associated with social desirability bias and item non-response. However, only 11 to 14% of the participants did not answer one of the attribution items. Moreover, we hope that the anonymity of the questionnaires encouraged students to be honest in their responses. Another limitation might be the use of vignettes, which do not necessarily reflect real-life situations [35]. However, vignettes are commonly used to assess stigmatic beliefs in a variety of conditions [36, 37], and they have been especially useful for testing hypotheses [17, 20, 3840]. Finally, it should be noted that our vignettes did not use the terms “young/late onset dementia”, but rather varied the age of the person described in them. Future studies might want to assess the effect of using these labels on stigmatic beliefs towards a person with AD.

Conclusions

Notwithstanding these limitations, this study has important implications. First, it supports recent findings [10] showing that the use of an expanded model of Attribution Theory provides an adequate conceptual framework for understanding dementia stigma. Second, the differences found in the stigmatic beliefs attributed to a younger or an older person with AD point to the theoretical and practical importance of clearly stating the age of the target person in stigma studies and in anti-stigma campaigns in the area of AD.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
The study’s protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Haifa. All potential participants were given and read an informed consent form describing the importance of the study, its anonymity, and the possibility to refuse to participate without any consequences. The students were required to sign an informed consent form before starting to answer the questionnaire.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Stephan B, Birdi R, Yee Hing Tang E, Cosco TD, Donini LM, et al. Secular trends in dementia prevalence and incidence worldwide: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66(2):653–80.CrossRef Stephan B, Birdi R, Yee Hing Tang E, Cosco TD, Donini LM, et al. Secular trends in dementia prevalence and incidence worldwide: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66(2):653–80.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Chow S, Chow R, Wan A, Lam HR, Taylor K, Bonin K, et al. National dementia strategies: what should Canada learn? Can Geriatr J. 2018;21(2):173–209.CrossRef Chow S, Chow R, Wan A, Lam HR, Taylor K, Bonin K, et al. National dementia strategies: what should Canada learn? Can Geriatr J. 2018;21(2):173–209.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Herrmann LK, Welter E, Leverenz J, Lerner AJ, Udelson N, Kanetsky C, et al. A systematic review of dementia-related stigma research: can we move the stigma dial? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018;26(3):361–31.CrossRef Herrmann LK, Welter E, Leverenz J, Lerner AJ, Udelson N, Kanetsky C, et al. A systematic review of dementia-related stigma research: can we move the stigma dial? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018;26(3):361–31.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Nguyen T, Li X. Understanding public-stigma and self-stigma in the context of dementia: a systematic review of the global literature. Dementia. 2020;19(2):148-81.CrossRef Nguyen T, Li X. Understanding public-stigma and self-stigma in the context of dementia: a systematic review of the global literature. Dementia. 2020;19(2):148-81.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P. Stigma and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review of evidence, theory, and methods. In: Corrigan P, editor. The stigma of disease and disability: understanding causes and overcoming injustices. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2014. p. 223–44.CrossRef Werner P. Stigma and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review of evidence, theory, and methods. In: Corrigan P, editor. The stigma of disease and disability: understanding causes and overcoming injustices. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2014. p. 223–44.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Panegyres PK, Chen HY. Differences between early and late onset Alzheimer's disease. Am J Neurodeger Dis. 2013;2(4):300–6. Panegyres PK, Chen HY. Differences between early and late onset Alzheimer's disease. Am J Neurodeger Dis. 2013;2(4):300–6.
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ducharme F, Kergoat MJ, Antoine P, Pasquier F, Coulombe R. The unique experience of spouses in early-onset dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2013;28(6):634–41.CrossRef Ducharme F, Kergoat MJ, Antoine P, Pasquier F, Coulombe R. The unique experience of spouses in early-onset dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2013;28(6):634–41.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Holdsworth K, McCabe M. The impact of younger-onset dementia on relationships, intimacy, and sexuality in midlife couples: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2018;30(1):15–29.CrossRef Holdsworth K, McCabe M. The impact of younger-onset dementia on relationships, intimacy, and sexuality in midlife couples: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2018;30(1):15–29.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Spreadbury JH, Kipps C. Measuring younger onset dementia: what the qualitative literature reveals about the ‘lived experience’ for patients and caregivers. Dementia. 2019;18(2):579–98.CrossRef Spreadbury JH, Kipps C. Measuring younger onset dementia: what the qualitative literature reveals about the ‘lived experience’ for patients and caregivers. Dementia. 2019;18(2):579–98.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Corrigan PW, Miller FE. Shame, blame, and contamination: a review of the impact of mental illness stigma on family members. J Ment Health. 2004;13(6):537–48.CrossRef Corrigan PW, Miller FE. Shame, blame, and contamination: a review of the impact of mental illness stigma on family members. J Ment Health. 2004;13(6):537–48.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Mak WW, Cheung RY. Affiliate stigma among caregivers of people with intellectual disability or mental illness. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2008;21(6):532–45.CrossRef Mak WW, Cheung RY. Affiliate stigma among caregivers of people with intellectual disability or mental illness. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2008;21(6):532–45.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, Rowan D, Kubiak MA. An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(2):162–79.CrossRef Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, Rowan D, Kubiak MA. An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(2):162–79.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P, Kalaitzaki AE, Spitzer N, Raviv Turgeman L, Koukouli S, Tzriraki C. Stigmatic beliefs towards persons with dementia: comparing Israeli and Greek college students. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(10):1393–401.CrossRef Werner P, Kalaitzaki AE, Spitzer N, Raviv Turgeman L, Koukouli S, Tzriraki C. Stigmatic beliefs towards persons with dementia: comparing Israeli and Greek college students. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(10):1393–401.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Green SB. How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis. Mutivariate Behav Res. 1991;26(3):499–510.CrossRef Green SB. How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis. Mutivariate Behav Res. 1991;26(3):499–510.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P, Davidson M. Emotional reactions of lay persons to someone with Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19(4):391–7.CrossRef Werner P, Davidson M. Emotional reactions of lay persons to someone with Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19(4):391–7.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Adebiyi AO, Fagbola MA, Olakehinde O, Ogunniyi A. Enacted and implied stigma for dementia in a community in south-West Nigeria. Psychogeriatrics. 2016;16(4):268–73.CrossRef Adebiyi AO, Fagbola MA, Olakehinde O, Ogunniyi A. Enacted and implied stigma for dementia in a community in south-West Nigeria. Psychogeriatrics. 2016;16(4):268–73.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Schomerus G. Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: changes over two decades. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):146–51.CrossRef Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Schomerus G. Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: changes over two decades. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):146–51.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Blay SL, Peluso ÉTP. Public stigma: the community's tolerance of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(2):163–71.CrossRef Blay SL, Peluso ÉTP. Public stigma: the community's tolerance of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(2):163–71.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson R, Harkins K, Cary M, Sankar P, Karlawish J. The relative contributions of disease label and disease prognosis to Alzheimer's stigma: a vignette-based experiment. Soc Sci Med. 2015;143:117–27.CrossRef Johnson R, Harkins K, Cary M, Sankar P, Karlawish J. The relative contributions of disease label and disease prognosis to Alzheimer's stigma: a vignette-based experiment. Soc Sci Med. 2015;143:117–27.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Piver LC, Nubukpo P, Faure A, Dumoitier N, Couratier P, Clément JP. Describing perceived stigma against Alzheimer's disease in a general population in France: the STIG-MA survey. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28(9):933–8.CrossRef Piver LC, Nubukpo P, Faure A, Dumoitier N, Couratier P, Clément JP. Describing perceived stigma against Alzheimer's disease in a general population in France: the STIG-MA survey. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28(9):933–8.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Woo BK. Knowledge of dementia among Chinese American immigrants. Asian J Psychiatr. 2013;6(4):351–2.CrossRef Woo BK. Knowledge of dementia among Chinese American immigrants. Asian J Psychiatr. 2013;6(4):351–2.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulvey KL, Hitti A, Killen M. The development of stereotyping and exclusion. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2010;1(4):597–606.CrossRef Mulvey KL, Hitti A, Killen M. The development of stereotyping and exclusion. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2010;1(4):597–606.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Stites SD, Johnson R, Harkins K, Sankar P, Xie D, Karlawish J. Identifiable characteristics and potentially malleable beliefs predict stigmatizing attributions toward persons with Alzheimer’s disease dementia: results of a survey of the US general public. Health Commun. 2018;33(3):264–73.CrossRef Stites SD, Johnson R, Harkins K, Sankar P, Xie D, Karlawish J. Identifiable characteristics and potentially malleable beliefs predict stigmatizing attributions toward persons with Alzheimer’s disease dementia: results of a survey of the US general public. Health Commun. 2018;33(3):264–73.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P. Lay perceptions about mental health: where is age and where is Alzheimer's disease? Int Psychogeriatr. 2005;17(3):371–82.CrossRef Werner P. Lay perceptions about mental health: where is age and where is Alzheimer's disease? Int Psychogeriatr. 2005;17(3):371–82.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Corrigan PW. Mental health stigma as social attribution: implications for research methods and attitude change. Clin Psychol-Sci Pr. 2000;7:48–67.CrossRef Corrigan PW. Mental health stigma as social attribution: implications for research methods and attitude change. Clin Psychol-Sci Pr. 2000;7:48–67.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Friedman DB, Becofsky K, Anderson LA, Bryant LL, Hunter RH, Ivey SL, et al. Public perceptions about risk and protective factors for cognitive health and impairment: a review of the literature. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27(8):1263–75.CrossRef Friedman DB, Becofsky K, Anderson LA, Bryant LL, Hunter RH, Ivey SL, et al. Public perceptions about risk and protective factors for cognitive health and impairment: a review of the literature. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27(8):1263–75.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Millenaar JK, Bakker C, Koopmans RT, Verhey FR, Kurz A, Vugt ME. The care needs and experiences with the use of services of people with young-onset dementia and their caregivers: a systematic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31(12):1261–76.CrossRef Millenaar JK, Bakker C, Koopmans RT, Verhey FR, Kurz A, Vugt ME. The care needs and experiences with the use of services of people with young-onset dementia and their caregivers: a systematic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31(12):1261–76.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin JK, Pescosolido BA, Tuch SA. Of fear and loathing: the role of disturbing behavior' labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41(2):208–23.CrossRef Martin JK, Pescosolido BA, Tuch SA. Of fear and loathing: the role of disturbing behavior' labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41(2):208–23.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Charles JL, Bentley KJ. Stigma as an organizing framework for understanding the early history of community mental health and psychiatric social work. Soc Work Ment Health. 2016;14(2):149–73.CrossRef Charles JL, Bentley KJ. Stigma as an organizing framework for understanding the early history of community mental health and psychiatric social work. Soc Work Ment Health. 2016;14(2):149–73.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST. Doddering but dear: process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. London: The MIT Press; 2002. p. 3–26. Cuddy AJ, Fiske ST. Doddering but dear: process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. London: The MIT Press; 2002. p. 3–26.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuddy AJ, Norton MI, Fiske ST. This old stereotype: the pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Aust J Soc Issues. 2005;61(2):267–85.CrossRef Cuddy AJ, Norton MI, Fiske ST. This old stereotype: the pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Aust J Soc Issues. 2005;61(2):267–85.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P, AboJabel H, Reuveni Y, Prilutzki D. Stigmatic beliefs toward a person with Alzheimer's disease among high school students: does majority-minority status make a difference? Educ Gerontol. 2017;43(12):609–18. Werner P, AboJabel H, Reuveni Y, Prilutzki D. Stigmatic beliefs toward a person with Alzheimer's disease among high school students: does majority-minority status make a difference? Educ Gerontol. 2017;43(12):609–18.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruggs EN, King EB, Hebbl M, Fitzsimmons M. Assessment of weight stigma. Obes Facts. 2010;3(1):60–9.CrossRef Ruggs EN, King EB, Hebbl M, Fitzsimmons M. Assessment of weight stigma. Obes Facts. 2010;3(1):60–9.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Link BG, Yang LH, Phelan JC, Collins PY. Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophr Bull. 2014;30(3):511–41.CrossRef Link BG, Yang LH, Phelan JC, Collins PY. Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophr Bull. 2014;30(3):511–41.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity. 2009;17(5):941–64.CrossRef Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity. 2009;17(5):941–64.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bourkel E, Ferring D, Weber G. Perceived rights of and social distance to people with Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontopsychol Geriatric Psychiatry. 2012;25(1):25–32. Bourkel E, Ferring D, Weber G. Perceived rights of and social distance to people with Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontopsychol Geriatric Psychiatry. 2012;25(1):25–32.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Wadley VG, Haley WE. Diagnostic attributions versus labeling impact of Alzheimer's disease and major depression diagnoses on emotions, beliefs, and helping intentions of family members. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56(4):244–22.CrossRef Wadley VG, Haley WE. Diagnostic attributions versus labeling impact of Alzheimer's disease and major depression diagnoses on emotions, beliefs, and helping intentions of family members. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56(4):244–22.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner P. Lay perceptions regarding the competence of persons with Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21(7):674–80.CrossRef Werner P. Lay perceptions regarding the competence of persons with Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21(7):674–80.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The influence of the age of dementia onset on college students’ stigmatic attributions towards a person with dementia
verfasst von
Perla Werner
Lilach Raviv-Turgeman
Patrick W. Corrigan
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2020
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Geriatrics / Ausgabe 1/2020
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2318
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-020-1505-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2020

BMC Geriatrics 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.