Skip to main content
Erschienen in: International Journal of Public Health 1/2019

Open Access 03.12.2018 | Original Article

The neighbourhood social environment and alcohol use among urban and rural Scottish adolescents

verfasst von: Gina Martin, Joanna Inchley, Alan Marshall, Niamh Shortt, Candace Currie

Erschienen in: International Journal of Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Objectives

This research examined the relationship between neighbourhood social environmental characteristics and drinking outcomes among a sample of urban and rural adolescents.

Methods

From a sample of 1558 Scottish secondary schoolchildren, surveyed as part of the 2010 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, we modelled three drinking outcomes on a variety of neighbourhood conditions, including social cohesion, disorder, alcohol outlet density, deprivation, and urban/rurality. Nested and cross-classified multilevel logistic regressions were specified.

Results

An urban-to-rural gradient was found with non-urban adolescents exhibiting higher odds of having ever drank. Neighbourhood social cohesion related to having ever drank. Among drinkers, those living in accessible small towns had higher odds of weekly drinking and drunkenness compared to urban areas. Higher odds of drunkenness were also found in remote rural areas. Those residing in the least deprived areas had lower odds of weekly drinking.

Conclusions

In Scotland, inequalities exist in adolescent alcohol use by urban/rurality and neighbourhood social conditions. Findings support regional targeting of public health efforts to address inequalities. Future work is needed to develop and evaluate intervention and prevention approaches for neighbourhoods at risk.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00038-018-1181-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Adolescent alcohol use is an identified public health concern. Scotland’s 15-year-olds rank fifth of 41 countries in Europe for having been drunk at least twice (Inchley et al. 2016). Further, it has been estimated that 15 individuals per day aged 17 or under are admitted to Scottish hospitals intoxicated (Christie 2008; NHS Quality Improvement Scotland 2008); this equates to approximately 1707 hospital admissions per 100,000 of the population aged 13–17, annually. These patterns are of concern given the wide array of harms associated with alcohol use among adolescents.
Because initiation into alcohol use often occurs in adolescence, this life stage has been established as a crucial period to reduce drinking (Kuntsche et al. 2005). Consequently, it is important to understand the risk and protective factors associated with alcohol use in order to develop targeted public health policies (Bryden et al. 2013). Evidence suggests adolescent alcohol use varies across neighbourhoods (Fagan et al. 2015; Jackson et al. 2016). However, which specific neighbourhood characteristics underlie this variation is not fully understood (Fagan et al. 2015).
Many features of the neighbourhood have been theorised to be associated with adolescent alcohol use (Fagan et al. 2015). Studies examining neighbourhood socio-economic factors have found mixed results (Bryden et al. 2013; Jackson et al. 2014), thus implying that more research is required to examine neighbourhood social factors. Neighbourhood social conditions, such as cohesion and collective efficacy, have drawn more recent attention and are often posited to underlie the relationship between neighbourhood economic conditions and alcohol use (Fagan et al. 2013; Jackson et al. 2014). Theories of the social environment and substance use suggest that the positive bonds in society deter adolescents from substance use (Wray-Lake et al. 2012),while neighbourhoods with greater disorder may encourage alcohol use as a way of coping with environmental stress (Hill and Angel 2005). However, reviews of neighbourhood social factors and drinking behaviour among adolescents indicate varied findings (Bryden et al. 2013; Jackson et al. 2014). This may, in part, reflect equivocal measurements of the social environment (Martin et al. 2017a) and/or different drinking outcomes included in these studies.
Research examining neighbourhood characteristics and adolescent drinking typically focuses on urban environments (Bryden et al. 2013). However, adolescents’ urban/rural status has been found to associate with their alcohol use and has been theorised to contribute to geographic variation in drinking behaviours (Slutske et al. 2016). Contemporary research has shown that adolescents residing in rural areas tend to drink alcohol at higher rates than those in urban areas (Dixon and Chartier 2016; Donath et al. 2011). The mechanisms behind this are not well understood but may be due to physical and/or cultural differences that exist between these communities (Donath et al. 2012).
There has also been much interest in the associations between commercial alcohol availability and adolescent alcohol use. Bryden et al. (2012) report that the evidence is inconclusive regarding these relationships. Increased availability may make alcohol purchasing easier through greater physical access and reduced prices, due to market competition (Shortt et al. 2018; Treno et al. 2013). However, as it is often illegal to sell alcohol to someone under a certain age, 18 in Scotland, the presence of outlets does not necessarily mean alcohol is easily available. More likely, a higher density of alcohol outlets may influence adolescent alcohol use via neighbourhood social norms and the normalisation of alcohol consumption (Kuntsche et al. 2008; Shortt et al. 2018). Maimon and Browning (2012) found that those residing in areas with higher alcohol outlet density and lower collective efficacy had higher predicted probability of alcohol use. It is important to consider alcohol availability as an important covariate in order to avoid biased conclusions about the influence of the social characteristics of the neighbourhood on alcohol use (Mohnen et al. 2011). This is particularly relevant given that more alcohol outlets tend to be present in areas of both higher deprivation and lower social capital (Shortt et al. 2015; Theall et al. 2009).
Results from neighbourhood studies that only assess neighbourhood variation may be misleading if variation from other contexts, such as school, are ignored (De Clercq et al. 2014; Dunn et al. 2015). Studies that examined adolescent smoking, using cross-classified multilevel models to account for the influence of non-nested contexts (where individuals are nested in schools and neighbourhoods, but schools are not necessarily nested within neighbourhoods or vice versa), found that neighbourhood effects are overestimated when ignoring school-level variation (De Clercq et al. 2014; Dunn et al. 2015).
This paper aims to address the following questions:
1.
To what extent does adolescent alcohol use vary by neighbourhood?
 
2.
Are there associations between neighbourhood characteristics and adolescent alcohol use?
 

Method

Participants

Survey data were collected as part of the 2010 Scottish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) survey, a World Health Organisation cross-national study (Currie et al. 2009). The 2010 Scottish survey of pupils in the fourth year of secondary school (S4: approximately 15 years old) included a boosted sample of rural schools allowing for comparisons at various levels of urban/rurality (Levin et al. 2014). Ethical approval was granted by the University of Edinburgh’s School of Education Ethics Committee. Prior informed consent was also obtained at local authority, school, parent, and pupil levels.
Pupils reported their residential postcode. In Scotland, postcodes represent a small geographical area making it possible to geocode (assign latitudes and longitudes) to each adolescent. Scottish data zones (DZ) and intermediate data zones (IDZs) are higher levels of geography which contain multiple postcodes. DZs (of which there are 6505) have on average 750 residents. IDZs are built up from data zones, representing 1235 regions in Scotland, containing on average 4000 residents. IDZs were developed based on administrative data and local knowledge (Flowerdew and Feng 2004). When linking in alcohol outlet densities (AOD), urban/rurality and neighbourhood deprivation, the finest geographic resolution available was used to achieve the most detailed estimate.
To increase the reliability of neighbourhood-level measures derived from aggregated individual-level responses (neighbourhood social cohesion and neighbourhood disorder), the sample was limited to 1561 students who reported their postcode and resided in an IDZ with 5 or more students (Martin et al. 2017b; Prins et al. 2014). Those included in the study were significantly (p < 0.05) more likely to be in the high-family-affluence tertile and to report their ethnicity as white, than those excluded, but were no more likely (p > 0.05) to be male, have ever drank, drink weekly, or have been drunk twice or more. An additional three students were removed from analysis based on inconsistent responses on the alcohol use questions.

Measures

Drinking behaviours

Three drinking behaviours were considered in these analyses. (1) Ever drank was classified as those who reported an age at which they had first drunk alcohol (“more than a small amount”) as opposed to “never”. (2) Weekly drinking was calculated by the following question: “at present how often do you drink anything alcoholic, such as beer, wine, or spirits? Try to include even those times when you only drank a small amount” Responses included frequency of consumption (every day, every week, every month, hardly ever, and never). Those who reported drinking any beverages daily or weekly were classified as weekly drinkers. (3) Drunkenness was assessed with the following question: “have you ever had so much alcohol that you were really drunk?” Responses were: never, once, two–three times, four–ten times, more than ten times. This was dichotomised into less than twice or twice or more.

Demographics and family characteristics

Sex was included based on self-report to the question: are you a boy or a girl?
Although all students were approximately the same age (15), even small age differences may impact behaviour, given the vast number of biological and social changes that occur during this time period. Age was based on year and month of birth.
Adolescent family structure was classified as living in a family with (1) both parents, (2) a single-parent household, or (3) with a step-parent family or other family composition (Levin and Currie 2010).
Family affluence was measured on a composite scale (Currie et al. 2008) using responses to four questions regarding family vehicle and computer ownership, having one’s own bedroom, and family holidays. The items were combined using categorical principal components analysis to create tertiles of low, medium, and high family affluence, for the total sample using CATPCA in SPSS, as recommended by Batista-Foguet et al. (2004) (see Levin et al. 2014). Family affluence has been found to be associated with adolescent drinking outcomes (Obradors-Rial et al. 2018).
Respondents reported their ethnic background(s). This was dichotomised into (1) white only, or (2) other, due to there being a small number of individuals within the sample, who identified as non-white.

Residential characteristics

Data were obtained from the Centre for Research on Environment, Society and Health at the University of Edinburgh who geocoded all alcohol outlets that held a licence to sell alcohol in 2012 based on postcodes. These data were used to estimate a measure of AOD using Kernel Density Estimation (KDE). This process divided Scotland into 100 × 100 m grid cells and assessed the number and proximity of outlets at a radius of the centre of each cell. Outlets nearer the centre were given greater weight than those further away; therefore, the value represents a proximity-weighted estimate of the density of each outlet type. See Supplemental Material for more details on KDE. Data were classified as on-trade (e.g. bars or restaurants) and off-trade (e.g. shops) (Shortt et al. 2018). As a first step, the models were run with an 800 m radius as this approximately equates to a 10-min walk (Shortt et al. 2016). Sensitivity analysis was also conducted using the 400 and 1000 m radius.
Neighbourhood socio-economic condition was determined by the income domain of the Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD) 2012. This measure was based on quintiles of all DZs in Scotland. The SIMD consists of seven domains, such as access to services and crime. Using the income domain as the most appropriate indicator of neighbourhood socio-economic circumstances follows the precedent of past studies (Levin et al. 2014; Shortt et al. 2015). The two most deprived categories were combined, as few of the sample (8%) resided in the most deprived quintile.
Respondents’ home postcodes were used to classify urban/rurality. Urban/rurality was classified into six categories: (a) large cities (population of 125,000 or more), (b) other urban (population ≥ 10,000 and < 125,000), (c) accessible towns (population between 3000 and 9999 and within a 30-min drive to a settlement ≥ 10,000), (d) remote towns (population between 3000 and 9999 and more than a 30-min drive to a settlement of ≥ 10,000, (e) accessible rural (population < 3000 and within a 30-min drive to a settlement of ≥ 10,000), and (f) remote rural (population < 3000 and more than a 30-min drive to a settlement ≥ 10,000).
Neighbourhood social cohesion was measured using three questions from the HBSC survey: in the area where you live (1) you can trust people around here, (2) people say “hello” and talk to each other in the streets, and (3) it is safe for younger children to play outside. Responses ranged from “agree a lot” to “disagree a lot”, on a five-point scale. The Cronbach’s alpha at the individual level (perceived social cohesion) was 0.745. Neighbourhood disorder was measured using the same procedure. Three questions were used in this measure: in the area where you live are there (1) groups of young people who cause trouble? (2) litter, broken glass or rubbish lying around?, and (3) run-down houses or buildings? Responses ranged from “none” to “lots”, on a three-point scale. The Cronbach’s alpha at the individual level (perceived disorder) was 0.754. Both measures have previously shown reliability (items were found to highly correlate) using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and showed measurement invariance between urban/rural classifications (Martin et al. 2017b).
Neighbourhood-level aggregation occurred using a three-level item response model accounting for item severity and the respondent’s sex (Martin et al. 2017b). The reliability at the neighbourhood level for social cohesion and disorder was 0.577 and 0.563, respectively. These measures have shown convergent validity (Martin et al. 2017b).

Statistical analysis

Analysis was conducted to examine whether the three adolescent drinking outcomes did indeed vary by neighbourhood (IDZs) (Research Question 1). This was done by fitting an empty two-level random intercept model with adolescents at level-1 and neighbourhoods at level-2, with no covariates (Robson and Pevalin 2015). These models assume a two-level structure where adolescents are only nested in neighbourhoods (ignoring schools). Second, a two-level model was run with schools at level-2 (ignoring neighbourhoods) (Dunn et al. 2015). Third, in a cross-classified model, individual adolescents were grouped simultaneously into two non-nested contexts (neighbourhood and school). A variance partition coefficient (VPC) was calculated to estimate the proportion of variance in drinking outcomes that are attributed to neighbourhoods and schools.
A second series of models was conducted to address Research Question 2. Only individuals with complete data on all covariates were included in multivariable models. Model 1 represents a two-level neighbourhood model which included individual socio-demographic factors. Model 2 also included alcohol outlet density and urban/rurality. Model 3 added neighbourhood deprivation. Models 4 and 5 added neighbourhood-level social cohesion and neighbourhood disorder, respectively. Model 6 included neighbourhood social cohesion and neighbourhood disorder together. Model 7 added individual perceptions. Model 8 included a cross-classified specification for school-level variation to ensure associations noted were indeed at the neighbourhood level. The sample was reduced to drinkers when examining weekly drinking and drunkenness.
Variance inflation factor values were below 3 for all independent variables indicating that multicollinearity was not a concern (O’brien 2007). All models were conducted using runmlwin (Leckie and Charlton 2013) via Stata v13 and MLwiN v3.01 with Bayesian estimation procedures as implemented by Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods. Because no previous knowledge was assumed, the MLwiN default diffuse prior distributions were used for all estimates. Initial values were derived from an iterative generalised least squares algorithm, and Metropolis–Hastings sampling was used (Browne 2017; Leckie and Charlton 2013). Odds ratios are reported with 95% credible intervals and p values. Bayesian deviance information criterion (DIC) was used to test for improvement of model fit, with lower values indicating better fit; generally, a difference of 5 is considered a substantial improvement (Khana et al. 2018; Spiegelhalter et al. 2002). This method was best suited to these analyses as it is appropriate for low numbers of respondents in higher levels of a multilevel model and because maximum likelihood methods are found to be inefficient for cross-classified models (De Clercq et al. 2014; Leckie and Charlton 2013).

Results

Participant characteristics

Table 1 outlines the characteristics of the sample. The majority of adolescents had drunk alcohol (83%), while almost half of the respondents (45%) had been drunk twice or more. Twenty-seven per cent of the respondents were weekly drinkers.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics of the study sample from the Scottish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children survey, 2010 (n = 1558)
Characteristics
Valid n
Mean (SD)/n (%)
Minimum
Maximum
Demographics and family characteristics
    
Age
1554
15.55 (0.33)
13.25
16.67
Male
1558
772 (50%)
  
White
1558
1515 (97%)
  
Family affluence
1558
   
 Low
 
496 (32%)
  
 Medium
 
479 (31%)
  
 High
 
583 (37%)
  
Family structure
1530
   
 Both parents
 
1080 (71%)
  
 Single parent
 
274 (18%)
  
 Stepfamily/other
 
176 (11%)
  
Individual neighbourhood perceptions
    
Perceived neighbourhood disordera
1516
4.92 (1.53)
3
9
Perceived social cohesiona
1522
11.98 (2.59)
3
15
Residential characteristics
    
 Neighbourhood deprivation
1558
   
  1 (most deprived)
 
343 (22%)
  
  2
 
358 (23%)
  
  3
 
461 (30%)
  
  4 (least deprived)
 
396 (25%)
  
 Urban/rurality
1554
   
  Large urban
 
263 (17%)
  
  Other urban
 
267 (17%)
  
  Accessible small town
 
193 (12%)
  
  Accessible rural
 
241 (15%)
  
  Remote small town
 
198 (13%)
  
  Remote rural
 
392 (25%)
  
Off-trade alcohol outlets (800 m)
1557
1.59 (1.87)
0
14.25
On-trade alcohol outlets (800 m)
1557
2.91 (4.17)
0
38.31
Neighbourhood-level disorderb
1488
− 0.01 (0.14)
− 0.27
0.37
Neighbourhood-level social cohesionc
1506
0.04 (0.25)
− 0.61
0.64
Alcohol use
    
Have ever drunk
1550
1281 (83%)
  
Drink weekly
1553
414 (27%)
  
Drunk twice or more
1545
689 (45%)
  
SD Standard deviation
aIf less than half of the items were missing, mean person imputation was used. This occurred in < 1% of cases
bAt the neighbourhood level, mean = 0 for 191 neighbourhoods
cAt the neighbourhood level, mean = 0 for 194 neighbourhoods

Empty models

For ever drank, weekly drinking, and drunkenness, neighbourhood accounts for 9.7%, 5.7%, and 3.6% of the variation, respectively, when ignoring school-level variation. This was reduced to 7.6%, 5.0%, and 1.0%, respectively, when accounting for school-level variation. For having ever drank and weekly drinking, the DIC was lowest in the cross-classified model compared to the two-level models, suggesting best fit when including both levels. For drunkenness, the DIC was only slightly lower in the cross-classified model, compared to the school-only model (see Supplementary Material).

Multivariable models

Full results of Models 1–8 for the three drinking outcomes can be found in the Supplementary Material. Models 6–7 are presented in Tables 2 and 3.
Table 2
Having ever drunk regressed on neighbourhood and individual measures from the Scottish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children survey, 2010 (95% credible intervals) (n = 1457; intermediate data zones n = 190; schools n = 152)
Variable
Model 6
Model 7
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
Sex (male)
1.01 (0.74, 1.34)
0.971
1.02 (0.75, 1.35)
0.959
Age
2.04 (1.30, 2.92)
0.001
1.83 (1.37, 2.49)
< 0.001
Family structure (reference: both parents)
    
 Single parent
1.37 (0.89, 2.04)
0.173
1.31 (0.84,1.96)
0.256
 Stepfamily/other
2.05 (1.16, 3.49)
0.016
2.00 (1.13, 3.40)
0.021
Family affluence (reference: low)
    
 Medium
1.50 (1.00, 2.15)
0.048
1.50 (1.00, 2.18)
0.051
 High
1.51 (1.02, 2.15)
0.036
1.51 (1.02, 2.16)
0.041
 Ethnicity (white)
3.06 (1.31, 5.95)
0.007
2.74 (1.17, 5.43)
0.018
 On-trade licence density
0.97 (0.92, 1.03)
0.307
0.97(0.93, 1.03)
0.322
 Off-trade licence density
1.02 (0.90, 1.16)
0.742
1.01 (0.88, 1.15)
0.873
Urban/rurality (reference: large cities)
    
 Other urban
1.47 (0.85, 2.40)
0.181
1.48 (0.84, 2.44)
0.188
 Accessible small towns
2.02 (1.03, 3.58)
0.038
2.02 (1.04, 3.62)
0.042
 Accessible rural
2.46 (1.29, 4.28)
0.005
2.50 (1.31, 4.40)
0.005
 Remote small towns
3.70 (1.80, 6.94)
< 0.001
3.83 (1.83, 7.21)
< 0.001
 Remote rural
3.64 (1.91, 6.37)
< 0.001
3.61 (1.87, 6.43)
< 0.001
Neighbourhood deprivation (reference: 1 most deprived)
    
 2
1.21 (0.70, 1.95)
0.557
1.26 (0.73, 2.04)
0.450
 3
0.94 (0.54, 1.52)
0.728
1.01 (0.58, 1.63)
0.910
 4 (least deprived)
1.01 (0.57, 1.66)
0.907
1.05 (0.59, 1.74)
0.964
 Neighbourhood social cohesion
0.33 (0.12, 0.75)
0.011
0.33 (0.10, 0.80)
0.017
 Neighbourhood disorder
1.25 (0.22, 4.10)
0.941
0.45 (0.06, 1.62)
0.169
 Perceived social cohesion
 
0.99 (0.93, 1.06)
0.806
 Perceived disorder
 
1.24 (1.10, 1.40)
0.001
 Neighbourhood variance
0.30 (0.03, 0.66)
 
0.31 (0.04, 0.68)
 
 DIC
1302.86
 
1291.59
 
Burn-in 5000, chain 200,000, OR odds ratio, DIC deviance information criterion, respondents missing on any predictor or outcome were not included in the models, bold = p < 0.05
Table 3
Weekly drinking and drunkenness regressed on neighbourhood and individual measures from the Scottish Health Behaviour in School-aged Children survey, 2010, among those who have drank (95% credible intervals) (intermediate data zones n = 190; schools n = 152)
Variable
Weekly drinking (n = 1205)
Drunkenness (n = 1198)
Model 6
Model 7
Model 6
Model 7
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
OR (95% credible intervals)
p value
Sex (male)
1.43 (1.10, 1.84)
0.007
1.44 (1.10, 1.86)
0.008
0.98 (0.77, 1.24)
0.835
0.99 (0.77, 1.25)
0.879
Age
1.38 (0.80, 2.39)
0.339
1.10 (0.75, 1.66)
0.704
1.25 (0.87, 1.75)
0.266
1.18 (0.79, 1.58)
0.346
Family structure (reference: both parents)
        
 Single parent
1.52 (1.06, 2.10)
0.021
1.47 (1.02, 2.04)
0.038
1.38 (0.98, 1.89)
0.070
1.36 (0.96, 1.87)
0.082
 Stepfamily/other
1.16 (0.76, 1.68)
0.537
1.13 (0.74, 1.65)
0.619
2.04 (1.36, 2.97)
0.001
2.02 (1.36, 2.97)
0.001
Family affluence (reference: low)
        
 Medium
1.21 (0.85, 1.67)
0.305
1.25 (0.87, 1.73)
0.236
0.93 (0.67, 1.26)
0.606
0.94 (0.68, 1.26)
0.625
 High
1.23 (0.88, 1.68)
0.241
1.26 (0.89, 1.72)
0.209
1.28 (0.93, 1.72)
0.132
1.28 (0.93, 1.71)
0.135
 Ethnicity (white)
0.71 (0.28, 1.50)
0.713
0.66 (0.25, 1.42)
0.246
0.68 (0.26, 1.47)
0.282
0.66 (0.25, 1.41)
0.241
 On-trade licence density
1.00 (0.96, 1.05)
0.938
1.00 (0.96, 1.05)
0.866
1.03 (0.99, 1.08)
0.180
1.03 (0.99, 1.08)
0.174
 Off-trade licence density
1.01 (0.90, 1.13)
0.934
0.99 (0.88, 1.11)
0.876
1.00 (0.90, 1.11)
0.947
0.99 (0.89, 1.10)
0.850
Urban/rurality (reference: large cities)
        
 Other urban
1.23 (0.73, 1.96)
0.490
1.25 (0.72, 2.01)
0.476
1.09 (0.69, 1.66)
0.771
1.09 (0.69, 1.66)
0.779
 Accessible small towns
2.04 (1.13, 3.42)
0.017
2.08 (1.14, 3.54)
0.016
2.24 (1.31, 3.61)
0.003
2.24 (1.31, 3.58)
0.003
 Accessible rural
1.23 (0.68, 2.06)
0.557
1.28 (0.70, 2.19)
0.474
1.34 (0.81, 2.11)
0.280
1.35 (0.81, 2.13)
0.277
 Remote small towns
1.26 (0.69, 2.13)
0.501
1.32 (0.72, 2.25)
0.418
1.41 (0.83, 2.24)
0.221
1.42 (0.84, 2.26)
0.208
 Remote rural
1.41 (0.78, 2.35)
0.277
1.39 (0.77, 2.34)
0.302
2.04 (1.23, 3.19)
0.005
2.01 (1.21, 3.15)
0.006
Neighbourhood deprivation (reference: 1 most deprived)
        
 2
0.89 (0.59,1.31)
0.518
0.91 (0.60, 1.35)
0.593
0.73 (0.49, 1.06)
0.096
0.75 (0.49, 1.07)
0.115
 3
0.76 (0.49, 1.14)
0.173
0.80 (0.51, 1.21)
0.271
0.73 (0.49, 1.06)
0.099
0.74 (0.48, 1.09)
0.126
 4 (least deprived)
0.63 (0.39, 0.97)
0.034
0.64 (0.40, 1.00)
0.048
0.70 (0.48, 1.04)
0.072
0.70 (0.45, 1.04)
0.080
 Neighbourhood social cohesion
1.07 (0.48, 2.10)
0.989
1.38 (0.56, 2.88)
0.579
0.88 (0.42, 1.62)
0.580
0.88 (0.39, 1.69)
0.590
 Neighbourhood disorder
2.31 (0.58, 6.44)
0.291
1.23 (0.25, 3.74)
0.965
3.50 (1.00, 9.10)
0.051
2.36 (0.60, 6.54)
0.272
 Perceived social cohesion
 
0.95 (0.89, 1.01)
0.082
  
1.00 (0.94, 1.06)
0.958
 Perceived disorder
 
1.14 (1.03, 1.27)
0.011
  
1.09 (0.99, 1.19)
0.090
 Neighbourhood variance
0.12 (0.00, 0.40)
 
0.17 (0.00, 0.46)
 
0.06 (0.00, 0.22)
 
0.05 (0.00, 0.22)
 
 DIC
1513.15
 
1499.86
 
1631.51
 
1631.68
 
Burn-in 5000, Chain 200,000, OR odds ratio, DIC deviance information criterion, respondents missing on any predictor or outcome were not included in the models, bold = p < 0.05
Urban/rurality showed a clear gradient in alcohol use (Table 2—Model 7); those in remote and rural regions had higher odds of having ever drunk than those in large cities, while those in smaller urban areas were not significantly different in terms of ever drinking (p > 0.05). A significant association was present for neighbourhood social cohesion on having ever drunk alcohol (odds ratio = 0.33, p = 0.017), in fully adjusted models. Including this measure also improved model fit compared to the null model (DIC = 1301.69 vs. 1304.15) (see Supplementary Material). In fully adjusted models (Model 7), no significant associations were found for AODs or neighbourhood disorder with having ever drank (p > 0.05); however, individual perceived disorder was associated with having ever drank, (odds ratio = 1.24, p = 0.001).
Among those who had ever drank, those residing in the least deprived areas had reduced odds of weekly drinking compared to those in the most deprived areas (odds ratio = 0.64, p = 0.048), in fully adjusted models (Table 3—Model 7). Additionally, those in accessible small towns had higher odds of weekly drinking than those in large urban areas (odds ratio = 2.08, p = 0.016). No significant associations were found for AODs, neighbourhood disorder, or neighbourhood social cohesion (p > 0.05). Individual perceived disorder was associated with weekly drinking (odds ratio = 1.14, p = 0.011).
Turning now to drunkenness, among those who had ever drank, those in accessible small towns (odds ratio = 2.24, p = 0.003) and remote rural areas (odds ratio = 2.01, p = 0.006) had higher odds of drunkenness than those in large urban areas (Table 3—Model 7), in the fully adjusted models. Those residing in areas of lower deprivation had significantly reduced odds of drunkenness; however, this relationship became non-significant when accounting for neighbourhood disorder (see Supplementary Material). Neighbourhood-level disorder was associated with increased odds of drunkenness (see Supplementary Material); however, this relationship was no longer significant when accounting for neighbourhood social cohesion and individual neighbourhood perceptions (Models 6–7).
For all outcomes, the associations in Model 7 were still observed after accounting for school-level variation (see Supplementary Material). Sensitivity analysis using different distance bands to measure AODs did not influence main findings from the models.
Because the data are spatially distributed, a global Moran’s I was calculated on the IDZ residuals from Model 8 to detect whether spatial autocorrelation was present, which would violate the assumption of independence of error-terms. The Moran’s I statistic was not significant (p > 0.05) (see Supplementary Material), indicating no spatial clustering in the model residuals (Anselin and Griffith 1988).

Discussion

This study used multilevel analysis to examine associations of neighbourhood characteristics with adolescent drinking behaviours. The results are strengthened by the inclusion of the school-level; thus, testing whether the findings are, in fact, overestimated due to the omission of the school-level variance. Results show that having ever consumed alcohol and weekly alcohol use varied by neighbourhood and are in line with a study of US adolescents that found significant variance of alcohol misuse at the neighbourhood level but not the school level (Ennett et al. 2008). However, school explained a greater amount of variance in drunkenness. This may be due to binge drinking being more influenced by shared peer culture experienced at school (Kuntsche and Jordan 2006).
The more remote and rural the area an adolescent resided in, the higher the odds of having ever drank. Other studies in Scotland have found an urban/rural difference (using a dichotomous measure) in whether adolescents had ever drunk alcohol (The Scottish Government 2016). Our current research found that among drinkers, those living in accessible small towns had higher odds of weekly drinking and drunkenness and those in remote rural areas had higher odds of drunkenness. This supports the principle that more detailed classifications of urban/rural are necessary, as suggested by Dixon and Chartier (2016). Additionally, the results reflect previous research on adolescent illicit substance and tobacco use, which maintain that adolescent substance use in Scotland is not concentrated in urban areas (Forsyth and Barnard 1999; Levin et al. 2014). The associations related to urban/rurality remained unexplained after controlling for neighbourhood social conditions and AOD, indicating that there may be other reasons for these inequalities. It may be that in rural areas and accessible small towns, adolescent alcohol use may be normalised and used as a form of “cultural capital” (Kloep et al. 2001).
It is important to note that the sample was made up of 15-year-olds; therefore, findings of an urban/rural gradient in having ever drunk represent a more delayed initiation to drinking but do not necessarily translate to lifetime abstention throughout adulthood. Conversely, many studies have found that, among adults, those in urban areas have higher rates of alcohol use compared to those in rural areas (Dixon and Chartier 2016; Slutske et al. 2016). Comprehension of different drinking trajectories across the life course, in terms of urban/rurality, is needed to explain this pattern.
Those living in an area of low deprivation had lower odds of weekly drinking, but not having ever drunk, or drunkenness (in fully adjusted models). Based on these findings, a potential explanation for the mixed results found in previous studies of neighbourhood socio-economics and adolescent alcohol use could be due to differing alcohol outcomes. Our results are in accordance with other research that found a relationship with neighbourhood deprivation and regular drinking among adolescents in Scotland (Petrou and Kupek 2018). The current study strengthens that evidence in that it adjusts for other neighbourhood conditions and family factors and confirms that this relationship holds.
Neighbourhood social cohesion was negatively associated with having ever drunk by S4; however, among drinkers, there was no association with alcohol use drinking behaviours. This is counter to findings from an urban US study that found neighbourhood collective efficacy did not influence adolescent alcohol use (Fagan et al. 2015). This may be due to their measures of the social environment originating from adults rather than adolescents. Conversely, Jackson et al. (2016) found collective efficacy, as measured by adolescents, was associated with adolescent drinking outcomes in an urban sample. Our findings support theories which argue that positive social connections discourage adolescent alcohol use; however, the association is limited to alcohol initiation. More research is needed to determine if creating more cohesive communities could reduce the likelihood of adolescents commencing alcohol use.
Unlike previous studies of Scottish adult populations (Shortt et al. 2018), we did not find an association between AOD and adolescent drinking outcomes. This may be because 15-year-olds are unlikely to purchase alcohol directly from retailers due to Scotland’s age restrictions and regulations (The Scottish Government 2016). It is noteworthy that the measure of on-trade outlets did not distinguish between establishment types. These may have differing impacts for adolescents as, unlike adults, they are restricted in terms of alcohol access in these venues. Some establishments would primarily be drinking establishments and may influence social norms in the neighbourhood, while other establishments may serve as a source of entertainment with alcohol consumption not being the primary activity. Moreover, the impact of AOD may only be observed over time after repeat exposure; longitudinal studies are needed to examine this possibility.
This study has several strengths, including having a boosted sample of non-urban youth, accounting for a variety of theoretically important neighbourhood conditions, and adjusting for school-level variation. Some limitations are worth consideration. First, this study is cross-sectional, so causation cannot be inferred. Additionally, IDZs were used to represent neighbourhoods. However, this is an administrative unit and may not correspond to the respondents’ understandings of their neighbourhood boundaries. Moreover, the neighbourhood-level social cohesion and disorder measures are derived from the same adolescents who reported their drinking behaviours; therefore, this study is at risk of same-source bias (Jackson et al. 2016). Further, we were unable to examine family structures that did not include a biological parent due to small numbers of students reporting these family compositions. Future studies designed to explicitly examine alcohol consumption among young people in alternative family situations are required. Finally, the focus of this study was on neighbourhood characteristics. Future studies may examine school characteristics. This is of particular interest for drunkenness given the greater proportion of variance accounted for by school compared to neighbourhood.
Despite these limitations, the results have important implications for public health strategies. Efforts that are targeted to rural areas, small towns, and neighbourhoods with low social cohesion are needed, given higher rates of adolescent alcohol use. Additionally, services and interventions should be directed at regions of high deprivation in Scotland, due to the higher rates of regular alcohol use. Future work is needed to develop and evaluate intervention and prevention approaches targeted to neighbourhoods at greatest risk.

Acknowledgements

The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study is an international survey conducted in collaboration with the WHO Regional Office for Europe. The authors would like to acknowledge the HBSC international research network that developed the study’s research protocol. For details, see http://​www.​hbsc.​org. Additionally, we thank the Regional and Island Authorities for granting permission for their schools to participate in the survey, all the young people who generously completed questionnaires, and the schools and teachers who kindly agreed to administer the survey. We thank Dr. Michael Livingston who provided feedback on an earlier draft of this work and the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. We also are grateful to Gill Rhodes for her time in proof-reading the manuscript and Catherine Tisch for her assistance with the outlet density measures.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest to declare.

Ethical approval

All procedures were in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments. Approval was gained from the following ethics review boards: University of Edinburgh’s School of Education Ethics Committee and University of St Andrews Teaching and Research Ethics Committee. Student participation was voluntary, and consent was obtained at local authority, school, parent, and pupil levels. Because of concerns regarding participant confidentiality, due to the geographic characteristics being linked, the data used in this analysis are unavailable publicly. A public data file (which is a subset of the data) is available through http://​www.​uib.​no/​en/​hbscdata.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Anselin L, Griffith D (1988) Do spatial effects really matter in regression analysis? Pap Reg Sci Ass 65:11–34CrossRef Anselin L, Griffith D (1988) Do spatial effects really matter in regression analysis? Pap Reg Sci Ass 65:11–34CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Batista-Foguet J, Fortiana J, Currie C et al (2004) Socio-economic indexes in surveys for comparisons between countries. Soc Indic Res 67:315–332CrossRef Batista-Foguet J, Fortiana J, Currie C et al (2004) Socio-economic indexes in surveys for comparisons between countries. Soc Indic Res 67:315–332CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Browne WJ (2017) MCMC estimation in MLwiN v.3.01. Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol, Bristol Browne WJ (2017) MCMC estimation in MLwiN v.3.01. Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol, Bristol
Zurück zum Zitat Bryden A, Roberts B, McKee M et al (2012) A systematic review of the influence on alcohol use of community level availability and marketing of alcohol. Health Place 18:349–357CrossRefPubMed Bryden A, Roberts B, McKee M et al (2012) A systematic review of the influence on alcohol use of community level availability and marketing of alcohol. Health Place 18:349–357CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bryden A, Roberts B, Petticrew M et al (2013) A systematic review of the influence of community level social factors on alcohol use. Health Place 21:70–85CrossRefPubMed Bryden A, Roberts B, Petticrew M et al (2013) A systematic review of the influence of community level social factors on alcohol use. Health Place 21:70–85CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Christie B (2008) Fifteen children a day attend Scottish emergency departments as result of drinking alcohol. Br Med J 336:1036CrossRef Christie B (2008) Fifteen children a day attend Scottish emergency departments as result of drinking alcohol. Br Med J 336:1036CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W et al (2008) Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the Health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Soc Sci Med 66:1429–1436CrossRefPubMed Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W et al (2008) Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the Health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Soc Sci Med 66:1429–1436CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Currie C, Gabhainn SN, Godeau E, International HBSC Network Coordinating Committee et al (2009) The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children: WHO Collaborative Cross-National (HBSC) study: origins, concept, history and development 1982–2008. Int J Public Health 54:131–139CrossRefPubMed Currie C, Gabhainn SN, Godeau E, International HBSC Network Coordinating Committee et al (2009) The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children: WHO Collaborative Cross-National (HBSC) study: origins, concept, history and development 1982–2008. Int J Public Health 54:131–139CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat De Clercq B, Pfoertner T-K, Elgar FJ et al (2014) Social capital and adolescent smoking in schools and communities: a cross-classified multilevel analysis. Soc Sci Med 119:81–87CrossRefPubMed De Clercq B, Pfoertner T-K, Elgar FJ et al (2014) Social capital and adolescent smoking in schools and communities: a cross-classified multilevel analysis. Soc Sci Med 119:81–87CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dixon MA, Chartier KG (2016) Alcohol use patterns among urban and rural residents: demographic and social influences. Alcohol Res 38:69–77PubMedPubMedCentral Dixon MA, Chartier KG (2016) Alcohol use patterns among urban and rural residents: demographic and social influences. Alcohol Res 38:69–77PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Donath C, Gräßel E, Baier D et al (2011) Alcohol consumption and binge drinking in adolescents: comparison of different migration backgrounds and rural vs. urban residence-a representative study. BMC Public Health 11:84CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Donath C, Gräßel E, Baier D et al (2011) Alcohol consumption and binge drinking in adolescents: comparison of different migration backgrounds and rural vs. urban residence-a representative study. BMC Public Health 11:84CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Donath C, Gräßel E, Baier D et al (2012) Predictors of binge drinking in adolescents: ultimate and distal factors-a representative study. BMC Public Health 12:263CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Donath C, Gräßel E, Baier D et al (2012) Predictors of binge drinking in adolescents: ultimate and distal factors-a representative study. BMC Public Health 12:263CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn EC, Richmond TK, Milliren CE et al (2015) Using cross-classified multilevel models to disentangle school and neighborhood effects: an example focusing on smoking behaviors among adolescents in the United States. Health Place 31:224–232CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Dunn EC, Richmond TK, Milliren CE et al (2015) Using cross-classified multilevel models to disentangle school and neighborhood effects: an example focusing on smoking behaviors among adolescents in the United States. Health Place 31:224–232CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Fagan AA, Wright EM, Pinchevsky GM (2013) Racial/ethnic differences in the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent substance use. J Drug Issues 43:69–84CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fagan AA, Wright EM, Pinchevsky GM (2013) Racial/ethnic differences in the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent substance use. J Drug Issues 43:69–84CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Fagan AA, Wright EM, Pinchevsky GM (2015) A multi-level analysis of the impact of neighborhood structural and social factors on adolescent substance use. Drug Alcohol Depend 153:180–186CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fagan AA, Wright EM, Pinchevsky GM (2015) A multi-level analysis of the impact of neighborhood structural and social factors on adolescent substance use. Drug Alcohol Depend 153:180–186CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Flowerdew R, Feng Z (2004) Report to the Scottish executive on: Scottish neighbourhood statistics intermediate geography. Geowise Ltd, Edinburgh Flowerdew R, Feng Z (2004) Report to the Scottish executive on: Scottish neighbourhood statistics intermediate geography. Geowise Ltd, Edinburgh
Zurück zum Zitat Forsyth AJ, Barnard M (1999) Contrasting levels of adolescent drug use between adjacent urban and rural communities in Scotland. Addiction 94:1707–1718CrossRefPubMed Forsyth AJ, Barnard M (1999) Contrasting levels of adolescent drug use between adjacent urban and rural communities in Scotland. Addiction 94:1707–1718CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hill TD, Angel RJ (2005) Neighborhood disorder, psychological distress, and heavy drinking. Soc Sci Med 61:965–975CrossRefPubMed Hill TD, Angel RJ (2005) Neighborhood disorder, psychological distress, and heavy drinking. Soc Sci Med 61:965–975CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Inchley J, Currie D, Young T, et al. (2016) Growing up unequal: gender and socioeconomic differences in young people’s health and well-being. In: Health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) study: international report from the 2013/2014 Survey 2016 Inchley J, Currie D, Young T, et al. (2016) Growing up unequal: gender and socioeconomic differences in young people’s health and well-being. In: Health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) study: international report from the 2013/2014 Survey 2016
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson N, Denny S, Ameratunga S (2014) Social and socio-demographic neighborhood effects on adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of multi-level studies. Soc Sci Med 115:10–20CrossRefPubMed Jackson N, Denny S, Ameratunga S (2014) Social and socio-demographic neighborhood effects on adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of multi-level studies. Soc Sci Med 115:10–20CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson N, Denny S, Sheridan J et al (2016) The role of neighborhood disadvantage, physical disorder, and collective efficacy in adolescent alcohol use: a multilevel path analysis. Health Place 41:24–33CrossRefPubMed Jackson N, Denny S, Sheridan J et al (2016) The role of neighborhood disadvantage, physical disorder, and collective efficacy in adolescent alcohol use: a multilevel path analysis. Health Place 41:24–33CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Khana D, Rossen LM, Hedegaard H et al (2018) A Bayesian spatial and temporal modeling approach to mapping geographic variation in mortality rates for subnational areas with R-INLA. J Data Sci 16:147–182PubMedPubMedCentral Khana D, Rossen LM, Hedegaard H et al (2018) A Bayesian spatial and temporal modeling approach to mapping geographic variation in mortality rates for subnational areas with R-INLA. J Data Sci 16:147–182PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Kloep M, Hendry LB, Ingebrigtsen J et al (2001) Young people in ‘drinking’ societies? Norwegian, Scottish and Swedish adolescents’ perceptions of alcohol use. Health Educ Res 16:279–291CrossRefPubMed Kloep M, Hendry LB, Ingebrigtsen J et al (2001) Young people in ‘drinking’ societies? Norwegian, Scottish and Swedish adolescents’ perceptions of alcohol use. Health Educ Res 16:279–291CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Jordan MD (2006) Adolescent alcohol and cannabis use in relation to peer and school factors. Drug Alcohol Depend 84:167–174CrossRefPubMed Kuntsche E, Jordan MD (2006) Adolescent alcohol and cannabis use in relation to peer and school factors. Drug Alcohol Depend 84:167–174CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G et al (2005) Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev 25:841–861CrossRefPubMed Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G et al (2005) Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev 25:841–861CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Kuendig H, Gmel G (2008) Alcohol outlet density, perceived availability and adolescent alcohol use: a multilevel structural equation model. J Epidemiol Commun Health 62:811–816CrossRef Kuntsche E, Kuendig H, Gmel G (2008) Alcohol outlet density, perceived availability and adolescent alcohol use: a multilevel structural equation model. J Epidemiol Commun Health 62:811–816CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Leckie G, Charlton C (2013) Runmlwin-a program to run the MLwiN multilevel modelling software from within stata. J Stat Softw 52:1–40 Leckie G, Charlton C (2013) Runmlwin-a program to run the MLwiN multilevel modelling software from within stata. J Stat Softw 52:1–40
Zurück zum Zitat Levin KA, Currie C (2010) Family structure, mother-child communication, father-child communication, and adolescent life satisfaction: a cross-sectional multilevel analysis. Health Educ 110:152–168CrossRef Levin KA, Currie C (2010) Family structure, mother-child communication, father-child communication, and adolescent life satisfaction: a cross-sectional multilevel analysis. Health Educ 110:152–168CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Levin K, Dundas R, Miller M et al (2014) Socioeconomic and geographic inequalities in adolescent smoking: a multilevel cross-sectional study of 15 year olds in Scotland. Soc Sci Med 107:162–170CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Levin K, Dundas R, Miller M et al (2014) Socioeconomic and geographic inequalities in adolescent smoking: a multilevel cross-sectional study of 15 year olds in Scotland. Soc Sci Med 107:162–170CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Maimon D, Browning CR (2012) Underage drinking, alcohol sales and collective efficacy: informal control and opportunity in the study of alcohol use. Soc Sci Res 41:977–990CrossRefPubMed Maimon D, Browning CR (2012) Underage drinking, alcohol sales and collective efficacy: informal control and opportunity in the study of alcohol use. Soc Sci Res 41:977–990CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Martin G, Gavine A, Inchley J et al (2017a) Conceptualizing, measuring and evaluating constructs of the adolescent neighbourhood social environment: a systematic review. SSM Popul Health 3:335–351CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Martin G, Gavine A, Inchley J et al (2017a) Conceptualizing, measuring and evaluating constructs of the adolescent neighbourhood social environment: a systematic review. SSM Popul Health 3:335–351CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Martin G, Inchley J, Humphris G et al (2017b) Assessing the psychometric and ecometric properties of neighborhood scales using adolescent survey data from urban and rural Scotland. Popul Health Metr 15:11CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Martin G, Inchley J, Humphris G et al (2017b) Assessing the psychometric and ecometric properties of neighborhood scales using adolescent survey data from urban and rural Scotland. Popul Health Metr 15:11CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Mohnen SM, Groenewegen PP, Völker B et al (2011) Neighborhood social capital and individual health. Soc Sci Med 72:660–667CrossRefPubMed Mohnen SM, Groenewegen PP, Völker B et al (2011) Neighborhood social capital and individual health. Soc Sci Med 72:660–667CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat NHS Quality Improvement Scotland (2008) Understanding alcohol misuse in Scotland harmful drinking: final report NHS Quality Improvement Scotland (2008) Understanding alcohol misuse in Scotland harmful drinking: final report
Zurück zum Zitat O’brien RM (2007) A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Qual Quant 41:673–690CrossRef O’brien RM (2007) A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Qual Quant 41:673–690CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Obradors-Rial N, Ariza C, Rajmil L et al (2018) Socioeconomic position and occupational social class and their association with risky alcohol consumption among adolescents. Int J Public Health 63:457–467CrossRefPubMed Obradors-Rial N, Ariza C, Rajmil L et al (2018) Socioeconomic position and occupational social class and their association with risky alcohol consumption among adolescents. Int J Public Health 63:457–467CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Petrou S, Kupek E (2018) Epidemiological trends and risk factors for tobacco, alcohol and drug use among adolescents in Scotland. J Public Health. 2002–2013 Petrou S, Kupek E (2018) Epidemiological trends and risk factors for tobacco, alcohol and drug use among adolescents in Scotland. J Public Health. 2002–2013
Zurück zum Zitat Prins R, Beenackers M, Boog M et al (2014) Neighbourhood Social Capital as a moderator between individual cognitions and sports behaviour among Dutch adolescents. Soc Sci Med 105:9–15CrossRefPubMed Prins R, Beenackers M, Boog M et al (2014) Neighbourhood Social Capital as a moderator between individual cognitions and sports behaviour among Dutch adolescents. Soc Sci Med 105:9–15CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Robson K, Pevalin D (2015) Multilevel modeling in plain language. Sage Publications Ltd, Thousand Oaks Robson K, Pevalin D (2015) Multilevel modeling in plain language. Sage Publications Ltd, Thousand Oaks
Zurück zum Zitat Shortt NK, Tisch C, Pearce J et al (2015) A cross-sectional analysis of the relationship between tobacco and alcohol outlet density and neighbourhood deprivation. BMC Public Health 15:1014CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Shortt NK, Tisch C, Pearce J et al (2015) A cross-sectional analysis of the relationship between tobacco and alcohol outlet density and neighbourhood deprivation. BMC Public Health 15:1014CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Shortt N, Tisch C, Pearce J et al (2016) The density of tobacco retailers in home and school environments and relationship with adolescent smoking behaviours in Scotland. Tob Control 25:75–82PubMed Shortt N, Tisch C, Pearce J et al (2016) The density of tobacco retailers in home and school environments and relationship with adolescent smoking behaviours in Scotland. Tob Control 25:75–82PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Shortt NK, Rind E, Pearce J et al (2018) Alcohol risk environments, vulnerability, and social inequalities in alcohol consumption. Ann Am Assoc Geogr 108:1210–1227 Shortt NK, Rind E, Pearce J et al (2018) Alcohol risk environments, vulnerability, and social inequalities in alcohol consumption. Ann Am Assoc Geogr 108:1210–1227
Zurück zum Zitat Slutske WS, Deutsch AR, Piasecki TM (2016) Neighborhood contextual factors, alcohol use, and alcohol problems in the United States: evidence from a nationally representative study of young adults. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 40:1010–1019CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Slutske WS, Deutsch AR, Piasecki TM (2016) Neighborhood contextual factors, alcohol use, and alcohol problems in the United States: evidence from a nationally representative study of young adults. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 40:1010–1019CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Spiegelhalter DJ, Best NG, Carlin BP et al (2002) Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. J R Stat Soc B Stat Methodol 64:583–639CrossRef Spiegelhalter DJ, Best NG, Carlin BP et al (2002) Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. J R Stat Soc B Stat Methodol 64:583–639CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat The Scottish Government (2016) Scottish Schools Adolescent lifestyle and substance use survey (SALSUS) 2015: Alcohol summary report The Scottish Government (2016) Scottish Schools Adolescent lifestyle and substance use survey (SALSUS) 2015: Alcohol summary report
Zurück zum Zitat Theall KP, Scribner R, Cohen D et al (2009) Social capital and the neighborhood alcohol environment. Health Place 15:323–332CrossRefPubMed Theall KP, Scribner R, Cohen D et al (2009) Social capital and the neighborhood alcohol environment. Health Place 15:323–332CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Treno AJ, Ponicki WR, Stockwell T et al (2013) Alcohol outlet densities and alcohol price: the British Columbia experiment in the partial privatization of alcohol sales off-premise. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 37:854–859CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Treno AJ, Ponicki WR, Stockwell T et al (2013) Alcohol outlet densities and alcohol price: the British Columbia experiment in the partial privatization of alcohol sales off-premise. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 37:854–859CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Wray-Lake L, Maggs JL, Johnston LD et al (2012) Associations between community attachments and adolescent substance use in nationally representative samples. J Adoles Health 51:325–331CrossRef Wray-Lake L, Maggs JL, Johnston LD et al (2012) Associations between community attachments and adolescent substance use in nationally representative samples. J Adoles Health 51:325–331CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The neighbourhood social environment and alcohol use among urban and rural Scottish adolescents
verfasst von
Gina Martin
Joanna Inchley
Alan Marshall
Niamh Shortt
Candace Currie
Publikationsdatum
03.12.2018
Verlag
Springer International Publishing
Erschienen in
International Journal of Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2019
Print ISSN: 1661-8556
Elektronische ISSN: 1661-8564
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-018-1181-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

International Journal of Public Health 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe