Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research article

The student resilience survey: psychometric validation and associations with mental health

verfasst von: Suzet Tanya Lereya, Neil Humphrey, Praveetha Patalay, Miranda Wolpert, Jan R. Böhnke, Amy Macdougall, Jessica Deighton

Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Policies, designed to promote resilience, and research, to understand the determinants and correlates of resilience, require reliable and valid measures to ensure data quality. The student resilience survey (SRS) covers a range of external supports and internal characteristics which can potentially be viewed as protective factors and can be crucial in exploring the mechanisms between protective factors and risk factors, and to design intervention and prevention strategies. This study examines the validity of the SRS.

Methods

7663 children (aged 11–15 years) from 12 local areas across England completed the SRS, and questionnaires regarding mental and physical health. Psychometric properties of 10 subscales of the SRS (family connection, school connection, community connection, participation in home and school life, participation in community life, peer support, self-esteem, empathy, problem solving, and goals and aspirations) were investigated by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), differential item functioning (DIF), differential test functioning (DTF), Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω. The associations between the SRS scales, mental and physical health outcomes were examined.

Results

The results supported the construct validity of the 10 factors of the scale and provided evidence for acceptable reliability of all the subscales. Our DIF analysis indicated differences between boys and girls, between primary and secondary school children, between children with or without special educational needs (SEN) and between children with or without English as an additional language (EAL) in terms of how they answered the peer support subscale of the SRS. Analyses did not indicate any DIF based on free school meals (FSM) eligibility. All subscales, except the peer support subscale, showed small DTF whereas the peer support subscale showed moderate DTF. Correlations showed that all the student resilience subscales were negatively associated with mental health difficulties, global subjective distress and impact on health. Random effects linear regression models showed that family connection, self-esteem, problem solving and peer support were negatively associated with all the mental health outcomes.

Conclusions

The findings suggest that the SRS is a valid measure assessing these relevant protective factors, thereby serving as a valuable tool in resilience and mental health research.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s13034-016-0132-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
CFA
confirmatory factor analysis
CORS
child outcome rating scale
DIF
differential item functioning
DTF
differential test functioning
EAL
English as an additional language
FSM
free school meals
M&MS
me and my feelings questionnaire
NPD
national pupil database
SEN
special educational needs
SRS
student resilience survey

Background

Over the past two decades, there has been a substantial increase in resilience research [1, 2], following dissatisfaction with ‘deficit’ models of illness and psychopathology [3]. Resilience is defined as the maintenance of positive adjustment in the context of exposure to significant adversity [4]. Key protective factors that confer resilience include positive individual characteristics, functional family relationships and a supportive environment outside the family [5, 6]. Individual characteristics such as self-control, empathy, intelligence, self-esteem and problem-solving skills have been identified as beneficial whether someone is facing low or high adversity [e.g. 7]. Similarly, warm relationships within the family and well-structured home environments are important for positive development in all children even in the absence of exposure to stressful life events. However, having a supportive family has been shown to be particularly important for children trying to cope with stressful experiences [e.g. 8]. Lastly, supportive environments outside the family such as availability of social support, school connectedness, having good neighbours and positive role models have been identified as potential protective factors [e.g. 5].
While resilience is conceived of as an end product of buffering processes that do not eliminate risks and stress but allow the individual to deal with them effectively, protective factors have been viewed as moderators of risk and adversity that enhance positive (i.e. developmentally appropriate) outcomes [4, 9]. The measurement of a range of factors that promote positive outcomes is crucial to explore the mechanisms between protective factors and risk factors, and to design intervention and prevention strategies. The student resilience survey [SRS; 10] covers a range of external supports and internal characteristics which can potentially be viewed as protective factors. It was constructed by combining elements from two surveys: the California Healthy Kids Survey [11] and the Perceptions of Peer Support Scale [12], assessing student perceptions of their individual characteristics, protective resources from family, peer, school and community.
The initial SRS development study by Sun and Stewart has supported the validity of the scale [10]. However, there were several limitations to the SRS validation. Firstly, the samples of children were drawn from only 20 primary schools in the state of Queensland, Australia. Sampling from a larger selection of schools, and a wider geographical area, is clearly needed for a more robust assessment of the psychometric properties of this measure. Secondly, although the initial validation study comprised a large sample size (n = 2794), this study will further provide confirmation by including reports from over 7000 children. Thirdly, SRS has only been validated using a scale-level approach. Scale-level analysis does not account for how individuals at different levels of the latent construct perform on the individual items of an instrument [13]. Item-level approaches allow examination of how individual subject responses on items of an instrument relate to an unobservable trait [13]. Differential item functioning [DIF; 14] allows for investigating item response probability based on different groups. DIF is present when individuals from different sociodemographic groupings (such as gender or ethnicity) have a different probability of answering an item [15]. Lastly, the initial validation did not investigate the association between SRS subscales and mental health outcomes. It is expected that most of the SRS subscale scores will be negatively correlated with emotional and behavioural problems [e.g. 16, 17] and attainment of good health [18].
The SRS can be an important tool in assessing the impact of protective factors when investigating the relationship between risk and psychological outcome and development. The purposes of the present study were threefold. First, we aimed to replicate the psychometric characteristics of the SRS found in its initial investigation, this time with an English sample [10]. Second, we aimed to investigate the measurement invariance in regard to several subgroups. Third, we aimed to assess the relationships between the SRS domains and children’s mental health outcomes.

Methods

Sample

Data were collected in 2015 from children who were part of a large project that focused on the promotion of resilience and emotional wellbeing (‘HeadStart’, funded by the Big Lottery Fund) in 12 local areas across 90 schools, England. The analyses reported are based on surveys completed by 7663 pupils (42.3% male); 1967 pupils were in primary school (year 6, mean age = 11.38, SD = 0.29) and 5696 pupils were in secondary school (years 7, 8 and 9, mean age = 13.31, SD = 0.86). For the item-level DIF analysis all items needed to be complete, hence only pupils who completed all items were included (sample size ranged from 6047 to 6123).
The sample was not drawn to be representative of all school children in England; it was based on local areas that were part of the HeadStart programme and each of the 12 local areas selected the schools to participate [19]. Overall, 5496 (72.8%) of pupils were White British (compared to the national average of 76.2%), and 6176 (81.6%) pupils’ first language was English (compared to the national average of 82.5%). 1452 (19.1%) were eligible for free school meals (compared to the national average of 16.2%—including nursery schools), 131 (1.7%) had a statement of special educational needs1 (compared to the national average of 2.8%) and a further 1159 (15.1%) had any elevated special educational needs, albeit not great enough to meet the threshold for a full statement of SEN.

Measures

Student resilience survey (SRS)

The SRS is a 47-item measure comprising 12 subscales measuring students’ perceptions of their individual characteristics as well as protective factors embedded in the environment. Frequency of each item was rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = always).
As this was part of a larger project and it was important not to burden the pupils with a long survey, only 10 of the SRS subscales (out of 12) were selected to be included into the survey with other validated measures. The 10 chosen subscales were: family connection, school connection, community connection, participation in home and school life, participation in community life, peer support, self-esteem, empathy, problem solving, and goals and aspirations. As the main project was interested in identifying protective factors in a child’s life, the pro-social peers (two items: my friends try to do what is right; my friends do well in school) and communication and cooperation (three items: I help other people; I enjoy working with other students; I stand up for myself) subscales were not included. Moreover, the scale was adapted to English school children based on discussions with young advisors from Common Room (a young people’s advocacy and engagement group with a specific focus on disability, health and mental health). Four items were edited so that they were more general and suitable for school-aged children in England (i.e. instead of “are there students at your school who would ask you to play when you are all alone”, it has been changed to “are there students at your school who would ask you to join in when you are all alone”; instead of “are there students at your school who would help you if you hurt yourself in the playground”, it has been changed to “are there students at your school who would help you if you hurt yourself”; instead of “are there students at your school who would invite you to play at their home”, it has been changed to “are there students at your school who would invite you to their home”; instead of “are there students at your school who would share things like stickers, toys & games with you”, it has been changed to are there students at your school who would share things with you”). Lastly, one item from the peer support scale (tell you you’re good at things) was omitted.
Mental health difficulties were measured with the me and my feelings questionnaire (formerly known as Me and My School measure, M&MS). It is a 16-item measure comprising a 10-item emotional difficulties scale and a 6-item behavioural difficulties scale [20, 21]. Each item includes a short statement (e.g. I am lonely; I get angry) measured on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, and 2 = always) (emotional problems sum score mean = 5.17, SD = 3.87; behavioural problems sum score mean = 3.05, SD = 2.52). Cronbach’s αs in the current sample were 0.84 for emotional problems (n = 7187) and 0.80 for behavioural problems (n = 7243).
Global subjective distress was measured with child outcome rating scale (CORS). CORS consists of four items: how am I doing; how are things in my family; how am I doing at school; and how is everything going. The rating scale is a 10 cm line with a happy face at one end and a sad face at the other; children are asked to put a mark on the line to indicate the place that best describes how they feel. The score for each item is automatically recorded and the overall score can range from 0 to 40 (sum score mean = 9.59, SD = 7.7); higher scores indicate more global subjective distress [22]. Cronbach’s α in the current sample was 0.81 (n = 7448).
Impact of health on daily life was measured with the EQ 5D-Y [23]. It has five dimensions: mobility (‘walking about’), self-care (‘looking after myself’), usual activities (‘doing usual activities’), pain and discomfort (‘having pain or discomfort’) and anxiety and depression (‘feeling worried, sad or unhappy’). All items refer to the health state ‘today’. Each item has three levels of problems reported (1 = no problems, 2 = some problems and 3 = a lot of problems) (sum score mean = 6.20, SD = 1.46). Cronbach’s α in the current sample was 0.65 (n = 7038).
Health today was also measured using the EQ 5D-Y. It included a visual analogue scale where the children rated their overall health status on a scale from 0 to 100 with 0 representing the worst and 100 representing the best health state they can imagine (on that day). In the current study, it was recoded so that higher scores indicated worse health (sum score mean = 20.64, SD = 19.8).
Special educational needs (SEN), eligibility for free school meals (FSM), and English as an additional language (EAL) were derived from the national pupil database (NPD). SEN were based on the school’s assignment of a child to a level of special educational needs. Children with SEN, whether with or without statement, were considered as having special educational needs. FSM is frequently used as an indicator of low family income since only families on income support are entitled to claim free school meals. Lastly, EAL was coded as present if a child’s first language was not English.

Procedure

Ethical approval was obtained from the University College London Research Ethics Committee. Children completed questionnaires using a secure online system during their usual school day with parent consent. Before pupils responded to the survey, teachers read an information sheet to them which highlighted confidentiality of their answers as well as their right to withdraw from the study. Children provided informed consent prior to completing the survey. The online system was designed to be easy to read and child friendly.

Analyses

The structure and psychometric properties of the SRS were investigated in several stages. Firstly, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted, using Mplus version 7.11 [24], to confirm whether constructs identified as subscales in previous research of this measure are evident in the current sample. This analysis was controlled for intra-class correlation due to clustering by schools [25]. Secondly, differential item functioning (DIF) was investigated across a range of demographic groupings using the Mantel–Haenszel procedure and the Liu–Agresti common log odds ratio as a measure of effect size [26] in DIFAS 5.0 [27]. Thirdly, DTF was conducted to examine the measurement invariance directly at the scale level across different subgroups in DIFAS 5.0. Fourthly, Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω were calculated, using SPSS version 21 and R, to assess the reliability of the subscales. Fifthly, to identify the association between protective factors and mental health outcomes, correlations were run between the SRS subscales and mental health outcomes using SPSS version 21. Lastly, to investigate whether internal or external factors had an impact on mental health outcomes, all subscales of the SRS were entered into regression models at the same time predicting each of the health outcomes. Both unadjusted and adjusted (adjusted for gender, school level—primary/secondary—SEN, EAL and FSM) random effects linear regression analyses (allowing for different school intercepts) were run using STATA version 12; unstandardized Bs, standard error and p-values are reported.

Results

Factor structure

Confirmatory factor analysis for ordinal data with weighted least squares with robust standard errors, mean, and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator [28] was carried out by testing a model with 10 correlated factors indicated by previous research (Table 1). Given the large sample size, Chi-square was not used to test model fit [29]. Other fit indices (CFI = 0.99; TLI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.01, SRMR within = 0.03; n = 7663) indicated a good model fit based on widely accepted criteria [30]. The correlation between 10 latent factors ranged between 0.26 and 0.77 (Table 2).
Table 1
CFA standardised loadings, measurement errors and intra-class correlations (by school)
Subscales
Items in questionnaire
Factor loading
Measurement error
Intraclass correlation
Family connection
At home, there is an adult who:
 Is interested in my school work
0.77
0.007
0.040
 Believes that I will be a success
0.85
0.006
0.034
 Wants me to do my best
0.86
0.009
0.057
 Listens to me when I have something to say
0.81
0.005
0.037
School connection
At school, there is an adult who:
 Really cares about me
0.79
0.024
0.176
 Tells me when I do a good job
0.86
0.011
0.140
 Listens to me when I have something to say
0.84
0.006
0.147
 Believes that I will be a success
0.85
0.010
0.114
Community connection
Away from school, there is an adult who:
 Really cares about me
0.90
0.003
0.041
 Tells me when I do a good job
0.92
0.002
0.038
 Believes that I will be a success
0.94
0.002
0.040
 I trust
0.85
0.004
0.044
Participation in home and school life
Home and school
 I do things at home that make a difference (i.e. make things better)
0.77
0.005
0.032
 I help my family make decisions
0.75
0.005
0.014
 At school, I decide things like class activities or rules
0.69
0.007
0.036
 I do things at my school that make a difference (i.e. make things better)
0.81
0.005
0.044
Participation in community life
Away from school
 I am a member of a club, sports team, church group, or other group
0.80
0.012
0.063
 I take lessons in music, art, sports, or have a hobby
0.88
0.011
0.057
Self-esteem
I can work out my problems
0.77
0.005
0.033
I can do most things if I try
0.83
0.005
0.061
There are many things that I do well
0.83
0.005
0.064
Empathy
I feel bad when someone gets their feelings hurt
0.80
0.007
0.043
I try to understand what other people feel
0.87
0.006
0.036
Problem solving
When I need help, I find someone to talk to
0.83
0.004
0.043
I know where to go for help when I have a problem
0.84
0.004
0.056
I try to work out problems by talking about them
0.81
0.004
0.040
Goals and aspirations
I have goals and plans for future
0.75
0.007
0.039
I think I will be successful when I grow up
0.90
0.006
0.065
Peer support
Are there students at your school who would:
 Choose you on their team at school
0.72
0.005
0.029
 Explain the rules of a game if you didn’t understand them
0.75
0.005
0.054
 Invite you to their home
0.75
0.004
0.041
 Share things with you
0.83
0.004
0.038
 Help you if you hurt yourself
0.84
0.005
0.050
 Miss you if you weren’t at school
0.79
0.004
0.040
 Make you feel better if something is bothering you
0.86
0.004
0.028
 Pick you for a partner
0.81
0.004
0.034
 Help you if other students are being mean to you
0.85
0.004
0.039
 Tell you you’re their friend
0.87
0.004
0.031
 Ask you to join in when you are all alone
0.86
0.003
0.039
 Tell you secrets
0.73
0.005
0.047
All factor loadings in CFA are significant at p < 0.001; CFI = 0.99; TLI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.01, SRMR within = 0.03; SRMR between = 0.62; n = 7663
Table 2
Factor correlations
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
1. Family connection
1
         
2. School connection
0.55
1
        
3. Community connection
0.74
0.56
1
       
4. Participation in home and school life
0.60
0.51
0.54
1
      
5. Participation in community life
0.30
0.26
0.31
0.42
1
     
6. Self-esteem
0.58
0.50
0.54
0.74
0.41
1
    
7. Empathy
0.45
0.41
0.42
0.58
0.27
0.53
1
   
8. Problem solving
0.56
0.56
0.53
0.70
0.29
0.70
0.60
1
  
9. Goals and aspirations
0.55
0.45
0.51
0.62
0.40
0.77
0.46
0.63
1
 
10. Peer support
0.53
0.43
0.50
0.55
0.32
0.59
0.52
0.61
0.48
1
All factor correlations are significant at p < 0.0001 level. CFI = 0.99; TLI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.01, SRMR within = 0.03; SRMR between = 0.62; n = 7663

Reliability

Since Cronbach’s α as a single measure for reliability is no longer regarded as optimal [31], McDonald’s ω was also used. Coefficient ω gives a better estimate of reliability than Cronbach’s α if the items of a scale are not tau equivalent [32, 33]. McDonald’s ωs were determined using the factor loadings of the multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (within-school factor models; only for the subscales with more than 2 items). The internal consistency for all the subscales was good. Cronbach’s α was 0.80 and McDonald’s ω was 0.89 (n = 7360) for the family connection subscale; α was 0.89 and ω was 0.91 (n = 7332) for the school connection subscale; α was 0.91 and ω was 0.94 (n = 7286) for the community connection subscale; α was 0.79 and ω was 0.84 (n = 7288) for the participation in home and school life subscale; α was 0.74 (n = 7304) for the participation in community life subscale; α was 0.80 and ω was 0.85 (n = 7358) for the self-esteem subscale; α was 0.77 (n = 7391) for the empathy subscale; α was 0.83 and ω was 0.87 (n = 7314) for the problem-solving subscale; α was 0.73 (n = 7324) for the goals and aspirations subscale; lastly α was 0.93 and ω was 0.96 (n = 7052) for the peer support subscale.

Differential item functioning (DIF) and differential test functioning (DTF)

In order to examine whether items behaved equivalently across a range of different subgroups of children, DIF analyses were undertaken for all subscales with more than two items. The non-parametric Mantel–Haenszel procedure was chosen to test for DIF since it is not based on the assumptions of a specific item response model [34]. Nevertheless, the subscales were checked to be sufficiently unidimensional based on a single factor multi-level CFA and which was acceptable according to standard criteria for all subscales and only mild violations for ‘participation in home and school life’ were found (see Additional file 1: Table S1 for details). Further, whether the CFA model’s thresholds were ordered along the latent continuum was inspected. Higher item categories corresponded to higher trait levels and only the space on the latent trait corresponding to category 2 was for some items comparatively small [35] (see Additional file 2: Table S2 for item thresholds).
In the DIF analysis, six grouping criteria were examined: gender, primary/secondary school level, whether the child had any elevated special educational need (SEN), whether English was the child’s second language (EAL), and whether the child was eligible for free school meals (FSM). Boys (42.3%, n = 2591), secondary school children (75.0%, n = 4592), children with SEN with or without statement (18.6%, n = 950), non-native English speakers (17.6%, n = 1064), and children receiving FSM (18.4%, n = 1116) were the focus of these investigations (and formed the focal group in DIF analyses). DIF analyses compare the item endorsement rates in the focal group compared to reference group (e.g. children with SEN with or without statement compared to all other children), conditioning on test scores. If children with the same overall score on a subscale (e.g. overall family connection subscale score) have different probabilities of endorsing an individual item then this item is said to show differential item functioning; in other words the item behaves differently across the two groups.
The Liu-Agresti common logs ratio [L-A-LOR; 26] was used to assess the size of potential DIF effects and to gauge their potential relevance. Positive L-A-LOR values indicate the item is more difficult to endorse for the focal group, while negative L-A-LOR values indicate that the item is more difficult to endorse for the reference group, given the same level of underlying trait. The magnitude of DIF was interpreted using a widely accepted classifying system [36]: the magnitude was negligible if L-A-LOR was less than 0.43, moderate if L-A-LOR was between 0.43 and 0.64, and large if L-A-LOR was greater than 0.64.
According to the DIF analysis (Table 3), boys were more likely to agree that students at school were more likely to pick them for a partner and girls were more likely to agree that students told them their secrets and missed them if they weren’t at school. Primary school children were more likely to agree that they do things at school which make a difference; that their peers explain to them the rules if they don’t understand, and that their peers help them if they hurt themselves. Secondary school children were more likely than primary school children to endorse that there are students at their school who would tell them their secrets. Children with SEN were more likely to agree that their family listens to them when they have something to say. Lastly, children without EAL were more likely to agree that peers invite them to their home. Analyses did not indicate any DIF based FSM eligibility.
Table 3
Differential item functioning and differential test functioning for the resilience scale
Subscales
Items in questionnaire
Gender (focal = boys)
n = 6123
School level (focal = secondary)
n = 6123
SEN (focal = yes)
n = 6071
EAL (focal = yes)
n = 6047
FSM (focal = yes)
n = 6071
DIF
DTF
DIF
DTF
DIF
DTF
DIF
DTF
DIF
DTF
L-A LOR (SE)
v2 (SE)
L-A LOR (SE)
v2 (SE)
L-A LOR (SE)
v2 (SE)
L-A LOR (SE)
v2 (SE)
L-A LOR (SE)
v2 (SE)
Family connection
At home, there is an adult who…
 Is interested in my school work
0.24 (0.07)
0.03 (0.03)
−0.15 (0.07)
0.03 (0.03)
0.15 (0.09)
0.09 (0.07)
−0.04 (0.09)
0.02 (0.02)
0.29 (0.08)
0.03 (0.03)
 Believes that I will be a success
−0.10 (0.07)
0.15 (0.08)
0.27 (0.10)
0.25 (0.09)
-0.14 (0.10)
 Wants me to do my best
0.17 (0.11)
0.34 (0.13)
0.25 (0.14)
−0.19 (0.14)
0.14 (0.13)
 Listens to me when I have something to say
−0.22 (0.07)
−0.08 (0.07)
0.48 (0.09)
−0.08 (0.09)
−0.24 (0.08)
School connection
At school, there is an adult who…
 Really cares about me
0.04 (0.06)
0.000 (0.003)
0.37 (0.07)
0.05 (0.04)
−0.38 (0.09)
0.05 (0.04)
0.08 (0.08)
0.01 (0.01)
−0.16 (0.08)
0.01 (0.01)
 Tells me when I do a good job
0.03 (0.06)
−0.02 (0.07)
0.28 (0.09)
−0.21 (0.08)
0.09 (0.08)
 Listens to me when I have something to say
−0.11 (0.06)
−0.18 (0.08)
0.03 (0.09)
0.04 (0.08)
0.18 (0.08)
 Believes that I will be a success
0.04 (0.06)
−0.23 (0.08)
0.12 (0.09)
0.08 (0.08)
−0.09 (0.08)
Community connection
Away from school, there is an adult who…
 Really cares about me
0.39 (0.08)
0.05 (0.04)
−0.15 (0.09)
−0.001 (0.01)
−0.05 (0.11)
−0.01 (0.004)
0.19 (0.11)
0.01 (0.02)
−0.05 (0.10)
−0.01 (0.003)
 Tells me when I do a good job
−0.10 (0.08)
0.00 (0.09)
−0.08 (0.10)
−0.21 (0.10)
−0.01 (0.10)
 Believes that I will be a success
0.04 (0.08)
0.05 (0.09)
0.11 (0.11)
0.11 (0.10)
−0.07 (0.10)
  I trust
−0.23 (0.08)
0.07 (0.09)
0.02 (0.10)
−0.05 (0.10)
0.10 (0.10)
Participation in home and school life
 I do things at home that make a difference (i.e. make things better)
−0.07 (0.06)
0.001 (0.003)
−0.05 (0.06)
0.10 (0.07)
0.13 (0.08)
0.001 (0.01)
−0.11 (0.07)
0.02 (0.02)
−0.01 (0.07)
0.01 (0.01)
 I help my family make decisions
−0.03 (0.06)
−0.22 (0.06)
−0.10 (0.08)
0.27 (0.07)
−0.14 (0.07)
 At school, I decide things like class activities or rules
−0.01 (0.05)
−0.22 (0.06)
0.01 (0.07)
−0.06 (0.07)
−0.02 (0.07)
 I do things at my school that make a difference (i.e. make things better)
0.10 (0.06)
0.56 (0.06)
−0.04 (0.08)
−0.12 (0.07)
0.19 (0.07)
Self-esteem
 I can work out my problems
−0.10 (0.06)
0.03 (0.02)
−0.35 (0.07)
0.07 (0.06)
0.17 (0.08)
0.01 (0.02)
0.21 (0.08)
0.02 (0.02)
0.18 (0.08)
0.01 (0.02)
 I can do most things if I try
0.26 (0.07)
0.23 (0.07)
−0.15 (0.09)
−0.11 (0.09)
−0.09 (0.08)
 There are many things that I do well
−0.11 (0.06)
0.21 (0.07)
−0.07 (0.08)
−0.16 (0.08)
−0.13 (0.08)
Problem solving
 When I need help, I find someone to talk to
0.36 (0.06)
0.06 (0.05)
0.08 (0.07)
0.004 (0.01)
0.04 (0.08)
−0.01 (0.001)
−0.21 (0.08)
0.02 (0.02)
0.01 (0.08)
0.003 (0.01)
 I know where to go for help when I have a problem
−0.20 (0.06)
0.06 (0.07)
−0.01 (0.09)
0.11 (0.09)
−0.13 (0.08)
 I try to work out problems by talking about them
−0.15 (0.06)
−0.13 (0.07)
−0.03 (0.09)
0.10 (0.08)
0.11 (0.08)
Peer support
Are there students at your school who would…
 Choose you on their team at school
−0.40 (0.06)
0.12 (0.05)
0.19 (0.06)
0.13 (0.06)
0.26 (0.08)
0.04 (0.02)
−0.10 (0.07)
0.07 (0.03)
0.10 (0.07)
0.004 (0.004)
 Explain the rules of a game if you didn’t understand them
−0.26 (0.06)
0.53 (0.07)
−0.10 (0.08)
−0.40 (0.08)
−0.10 (0.08)
 Invite you to their home
−0.17 (0.06)
−0.32 (0.06)
0.25 (0.08)
0.75 (0.08)
0.20 (0.07)
 Share things with you
0.01 (0.06)
−0.28 (0.07)
0.05 (0.08)
−0.13 (0.08)
0.07 (0.08)
 Help you if you hurt yourself
−0.03 (0.07)
0.76 (0.08)
−0.27 (0.09)
−0.27 (0.09)
0.01 (0.08)
 Miss you if you weren’t at school
0.76 (0.06)
−0.23 (0.06)
−0.16 (0.08)
−0.01 (0.08)
0.03 (0.07)
 Make you feel better if something is bothering you
0.28 (0.06)
0.24 (0.07)
−0.29 (0.08)
0.08 (0.08)
-0.13 (0.08)
 Pick you for a partner
0.45 (0.07)
−0.14 (0.07)
0.33 (0.08)
0.04 (0.09)
0.02 (0.08)
 Help you if other students are being mean to you
−0.03 (0.06)
0.16 (0.07)
−0.17 (0.08)
−0.12 (0.08)
−0.14 (0.08)
 Tell you you’re their friend
−0.07 (0.07)
0.16 (0.08)
−0.07 (0.09)
0.03 (0.09)
−0.08 (0.09)
 Ask you to join in when you are all alone
−0.34 (0.07)
−0.02 (0.07)
−0.28 (0.09)
−0.29 (0.09)
−0.08 (0.08)
 Tell you secrets
0.45 (0.06)
0.62 (0.06)
0.20 (0.08)
0.08 (0.08)
−0.02 (0.07)
EAL English as an additional language, FSM free school meals, SEN special educational needs. DIF and DTF analyses for subscales with less than 3 items (participation in community life, empathy, and goals and aspirations) are not conducted. The size of the DIF is considered negligible if L-A-LOR < 0.43, moderate if between 0.43 and 0.64, and large if >0.64. The size of the DTF ν2 < 0.07 indicates small DTF, 0.07 ≤ ν2 ≤ 0.14 indicates medium DTF, and ν2 > 0.14 indicates large DTF
Differential test functioning (DTF) assesses the aggregate effect of DIF across all the items in a scale [37]. A scale with a DIF effect variance of ν2 below 0.07 can be classified as having small DTF, whereas DTF would be considered medium for 0.07 ≤ ν2 ≤ 0.14 and large for ν2 > 0.14 [37]. All subscales, except the peer support subscale, showed small DTF whereas the peer support subscale showed moderate DTF (Table 3).

The student resilience survey and mental health problems

A pattern was identified whereby the subscales of the SRS are negatively associated with mental health difficulties, global subjective distress and impact on health (Table 4). In terms of negligible associations, empathy demonstrated very low correlations with both emotional problems and health. On the other hand, both self-esteem and problem solving had moderate correlations with emotional problems and global subjective distress.
Table 4
Correlations between student resilience subscales and other scales
 
Emotional problems
Behavioural problems
Global subjective distress
Impact of health on daily life
Health today
(0–100)
Family connection
−.29*
(n = 6944)
−.34*
(n = 6995)
−.41*
(n = 7160)
−.31*
(n = 6775)
−.30*
(n = 7033)
School connection
−.22*
(n = 6911)
−.26*
(n = 6963)
−.39*
(n = 7134)
−.21*
(n = 6752)
−.25*
(n = 7013)
Community connection
−.29*
(n = 6876)
−.25*
(n = 6926)
−.36*
(n = 7082)
−.27*
(n = 6717)
−.28*
(n = 6967)
Participation in home and school life
−.32*
(n = 6903)
−.29*
(n = 6958)
−.40*
(n = 7089)
−.26*
(n = 6753)
−.27*
(n = 7002)
Participation in community life
−.15*
(n = 6882)
−.10*
(n = 6932)
−.16*
(n = 7101)
−.11*
(n = 6736)
−.19*
(n = 6987)
Self-esteem
−.44*
(n = 6969)
−.32*
(n = 7018)
−.49*
(n = 7157)
−.35*
(n = 6816)
−.36*
(n = 7072)
Empathy
−.07*
(n = 7004)
−.27*
(n = 7045)
−.21*
(n = 7189)
−.10*
(n = 6847)
−.17*
(n = 7105)
Problem solving
−.37*
(n = 6933)
−.33*
(n = 6980)
−.44*
(n = 7112)
−.30*
(n = 6778)
−.29*
(n = 7038)
Goals and aspirations
−.32*
(n = 6939)
−.23*
(n = 6980)
−.37*
(n = 7122)
−.25*
(n = 6787)
−.28*
(n = 7042)
Peer support
−.36*
(n = 6681)
−.24*
(n = 6725)
−.35*
(n = 6855)
−.31*
(n = 6533)
−.27*
(n = 6772)
* p < 0.0001
Furthermore, in order to investigate whether internal or external factors had an impact on mental health outcomes, all subscales of the SRS were entered into linear regression models (with school as a random effects term) at the same time predicting each of the health outcomes (Table 5). The adjusted regression results showed that family connection, self-esteem, problem solving and peer support were negatively associated with all the mental health outcomes. On the other hand, those who had high empathy were more likely to display mental health difficulties, global subjective distress and impact on health.
Table 5
Unadjusted and adjusted associations between student resilience survey subscales and other scales
Unadjusted analysis
Emotional problems
(n = 5561)
Behavioural problems
(n = 5595)
Global subjective distress
(n = 5655)
Impact of health on daily life (n = 5439)
Health today (0–100)
(n = 5621)
Student resilience survey subscales
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Fixed effects
 Intercept
15.06 (0.34)
<.0001
9.72 (0.23)
<.0001
36.07 (0.65)
<.0001
9.84 (0.14)
<.0001
71.78 (1.81)
<.0001
 Family connection
−0.08 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.16 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.46 (0.04)
<.0001
−0.07 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.83 (0.11)
<.0001
 School connection
0.02 (0.01)
0.216
−0.03 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.20 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.00 (0.01)
0.773
0.22 (0.07)
0.003
 Community connection
−0.06 (0.02)
0.001
−0.01 (0.01)
0.563
−0.12 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.02 (0.01)
0.009
−0.20 (0.09)
0.026
 Participation in home and school life
−0.03 (0.02)
0.095
−0.02 (0.01)
0.040
−0.08 (0.03)
0.012
0.01 (0.01)
0.217
0.00 (0.09)
0.992
 Participation in community life
−0.01 (0.02)
0.516
0.03 (0.01)
0.024
0.05 (0.03)
0.112
0.00 (0.01)
0.493
−0.39 (0.09)
<.0001
 Self-esteem
−0.42 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.14 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.80 (0.05)
<.0001
−0.12 (0.01)
<.0001
−1.46 (0.14)
<.0001
 Empathy
0.52 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.12 (0.02)
<.0001
0.48 (0.05)
<.0001
0.12 (0.01)
<.0001
0.38 (0.15)
0.012
 Problem solving
−0.22 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.10 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.35 (0.04)
<.0001
−0.05 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.36 (0.10)
<.0001
 Goals and aspirations
−0.10 (0.03)
0.001
0.04 (0.02)
0.050
−0.13 (0.06)
0.029
−0.01 (0.01)
0.460
−0.37 (0.16)
0.024
 Peer support
−0.06 (0.01)
<.0001
0.01 (0.00)
0.013
−0.04 (0.01)
0.001
0.02 (0.00)
<.0001
0.12 (0.03)
<.0001
Variance components
 Residual variance
3.21 (0.03)
 
2.24 (0.02)
 
6.11 (0.06)
 
1.30 (0.01)
 
17.49 (0.17)
 
 School-level
0.72 (0.09)
 
0.38 (0.06)
 
0.92 (0.14)
 
0.23 (0.04)
 
2.22 (0.43)
 
Adjusted analysisa
Emotional problems
(n = 5488)
Behavioural problems
(n = 5522)
Global subjective distress
(n = 5584)
Impact of health on daily life (n = 5371)
Health today (0–100)
(n = 5548)
Student resilience survey subscales
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Estimate (SE)
P
Fixed effects
 Intercept
18.87 (0.46)
<.0001
8.87 (0.31)
<.0001
36.29 (0.87)
<.0001
10.55 (0.19)
<.0001
70.66 (2.46)
<.0001
 Family connection
−0.08 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.16 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.47 (0.04)
<.0001
−0.07 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.83 (0.11)
<.0001
 School connection
0.01 (0.01)
0.476
−0.04 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.20 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.01 (0.01)
0.245
−0.23 (0.08)
0.002
 Community connection
−0.06 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.01 (0.01)
0.625
−0.13 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.02 (0.01)
0.008
−0.17 (0.09)
0.052
 Participation in home and school life
−0.03 (0.02)
0.055
−0.03 (0.01)
0.014
−0.09 (0.03)
0.005
0.01 (0.01)
0.420
−0.04 (0.09)
0.630
 Participation in community life
0.01 (0.02)
0.640
0.03 (0.01)
0.011
0.07 (0.03)
0.036
0.01 (0.01)
0.183
−0.31 (0.09)
0.001
 Self-esteem
−0.36 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.14 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.76 (0.05)
<.0001
−0.11 (0.01)
<.0001
−1.37 (0.14)
<.0001
 Empathy
0.45 (0.03)
<.0001
−0.09 (0.02)
<.0001
0.44 (0.05)
<.0001
0.11 (0.01)
<.0001
0.31 (0.15)
0.047
 Problem solving
−0.20 (0.02)
<.0001
−0.10 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.34 (0.04)
<.0001
−0.05 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.32 (0.10)
0.001
 Goals and aspirations
−0.07 (0.03)
0.024
0.03 (0.02)
0.114
−0.09 (0.06)
0.109
−0.01 (0.01)
0.594
−0.37 (0.16)
0.024
 Peer support
−0.08 (0.01)
<.0001
0.02 (0.00)
<.0001
0.05 (0.01)
<.0001
−0.02 (0.00)
<.0001
−0.13 (0.03)
<.0001
Variance components
 Residual variance
3.15 (0.03)
 
2.22 (0.21)
 
6.07 (0.06)
 
1.29 (0.01)
 
17.42 (0.17)
 
 School-level
0.54 (0.08)
 
0.27 (0.06)
 
0.79 (0.15)
 
0.18 (0.04)
 
2.03 (0.44)
 
Each column represents a new regression model. All models were tested using multilevel linear regression
aAdjusted analysis are controlling for gender, school level (primary/secondary), special education needs, English as an additional language and free school meals

Discussion

The aim of this study was to replicate the psychometric properties of the student resilience survey (SRS) within an English sample, to investigate the measurement invariance in subgroups, and to investigate the relationship between SRS subscales and mental health.
On the basis of the confirmatory factor analysis, the factor structure of the measure was similar to the original validation study showing that 41 items loaded uniquely onto their respective 10 subscales. Moreover, similar to the validation study, the analyses provided evidence for acceptable reliability of all the subscales for this sample, especially considering that some of them are very short//have very few items//something like that [10]. Nevertheless, the ‘participation in home and school life’ showed in individual analyses that it is not unidimensional and will need further research investigating its structure.
This study extended previous research by generating evidence of the SRS’s validity via analysis of DIF and DTF. Our DIF analysis indicated differences between boys and girls in terms of how they answered the peer support subscale of the SRS. This may be due to the differences between boys’ and girls’ expectations from friends. One study has shown that boys describe friends as “people you play with” whereas girls describe them as “people you can trust” [38]. Moreover, DIF analyses suggested that children in secondary school were more likely to agree that their peers shared secrets with them. This is in line with literature suggesting that during middle childhood, the quality of friendship changes from relationships characterised by the temporary sharing of activities and leisure time to friendships enhanced by intimacy, help, certainty, loyalty and confidence [39]. Primary school children were also more likely to agree that their peers explained the rules of play to them and helped if they hurt themselves. This may be due to children in secondary school becoming more independent and working through their problems more easily than younger children [40]. Children with special educational needs were more likely to agree that their families listened to them when they have something to say. The quality of caregiving by parents is crucial for children with special educational needs [41], hence parents of children with special educational needs might be making an extra effort to engage with their children. Children with English as a first language were more likely to agree that peers invited them to their home. Literature suggests that children prefer same-ethnic friends, even when the opportunity to meet out-group friends has been taken into account [42]. Hence, children with English as a first language might get more invites to friends’ homes. All subscales, except the peer support subscale, showed small DTF whereas the peer support subscale showed moderate DTF. Future research should explore in more detail whether the DIF found is due to qualitative differences and constitutes relevant item and scale bias that needs to be addressed by changing items or separate norming tables (especially for school and gender [43]).
As expected on the basis of related evidence, our results showed negative correlations between all the SRS subscales and emotional, behavioural and health problems. Adjusted random effects linear regression models showed that family connection, self-esteem, problem solving and peer support were negatively associated with emotional, behavioural and health problems. The capacity of supportive families to buffer children from the impact of stressful life events is well documented [44]. For example, family-related factors such as parenting style and parent–child relationship quality have been associated with improved psychosocial adjustment [e.g. 45, 46] and attainment of good health [18]. Having a positive parent–child relationship may translate into an opportunity for parents to guide their children in understanding emotions and dealing with stress. Additionally, it has been shown that healthy peer relationships and healthy school environment can protect youth from maladjustment (i.e. emotional and behavioural problems) [47] and health problems [18]. Similar to previous literature, self-esteem and problem-solving skills have been negatively associated with emotional problems [48, 49]. Problem-solving skills and self-esteem may enable children to alter the source of stress which in return may have a positive impact on mental and physical health problems [50]. On the other hand our results showed that the correlation between empathy and emotional problems was very low. Moreover, when all the subscales were loaded into the random effects regression models predicting mental health outcomes, empathy was positively associated with emotional, behavioural and health problems. The literature suggests that extreme empathy may increase vulnerability to mental health difficulties [51, 52].
In general, protective factors alter responses to adverse events so that potential negative outcomes can be avoided [9]. According to Benzies and Mychasiuk [53], resilience is optimised when protective factors are strengthened at all interactive levels of the socio-ecological model (i.e. individual, family and community). Identifying protective factors that promote resilience to mental health problems could lead to improved intervention strategies [1].
It is important to note the methodological limitations of the study. Firstly, the population of the study was not drawn to be representative of all school children in England; however, the participants were from 12 local areas of England which increased the generalisability of the results. Secondly, due to the overlap between the items of the SRS and the mental health difficulties questionnaires and the cross-sectional nature of this study, we cannot disentangle temporal precedence (what comes first) and causal mechanisms between the resilience factors and mental health outcomes. However, future studies should apply longitudinal methods to elucidate the relationship between the SRS protective factors and maladjustment. Thirdly, this study did not investigate the stability of scores; hence future studies should examine the test–retest reliability of the SRS. Lastly, our approach addressed the clustering due to schools in our sample with an appropriate multilevel approach to identify between-student variation [25]; but we did not model the differences between schools. The corresponding SRMR values for the between-school variance–covariance matrices were consequently bad (SRMR = 0.61 for full model; see also Additional file 1: Table S1). The intra-class correlations presented in Table 1 also show that relevant amounts of variance in item responses were due to differences between schools. Future research should explore school-characteristics to further explore these differences. Additionally, future research may investigate whether the SRS could be effectively or appropriately utilised with children who have clinical levels of mental health problems.

Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the validity of the student resilience survey in the UK and to explore its relationship with mental health and wellbeing. Notwithstanding the limitations, our results showed that SRS subscales have loaded uniquely onto their respective 10-factor structure. All the SRS subscales had high reliability and the subscales (except for empathy) were negatively associated with mental health problems, global subjective distress and impact on health. Given the interest of investigating the relationship between risk, psychological outcomes and development, the SRS provides an exciting possibility to assess several different protective factors, and can thus be used as a valuable measurement tool in resilience and risk factor research.

Authors’ contributions

STL, NH and JD conceived of the study. STL performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript; JB supervised the statistical analysis; NH, PP, MW, JB, AM, and JD revised and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the students who took part in this study, the local authorities and schools for their help in recruiting them.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

No additional data available.
Children completed questionnaires using a secure online system during their usual school day with parent consent. Children provided informed consent prior to completing the survey.
Ethical approval was obtained from the University College London Research Ethics Committee.

Funding

HeadStart was funded by the Big Lottery Fund. STL, JD and MW’s time were partially supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR), Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC), North Thames at Bart’s Health NHS Trust. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, the Department of Health or Big Lottery Fund.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Fußnoten
1
A statement of special educational needs is a formal document outlining the nature of a given child’s needs that is produced following a process of statutory assessment (by, for example, an educational psychologist).
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Windle G. What is resilience? A review and concept analysis. Rev Clin Gerontol. 2011;21(2):152–69.CrossRef Windle G. What is resilience? A review and concept analysis. Rev Clin Gerontol. 2011;21(2):152–69.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Schultze-Lutter F, Schimmelmann BG, Schmidt SJ. Resilience, risk, mental health and well-being: associations and conceptual differences. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(5):459–66.CrossRefPubMed Schultze-Lutter F, Schimmelmann BG, Schmidt SJ. Resilience, risk, mental health and well-being: associations and conceptual differences. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(5):459–66.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Fergus S, Zimmerman MA. Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26(1):399–419.CrossRefPubMed Fergus S, Zimmerman MA. Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26(1):399–419.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 2000;71(3):543–62.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 2000;71(3):543–62.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Luthar SS. Resilience in development: a synthesis of research across five decades. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: vol 3 risk, disorder and adaptation. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. p. 739–95. Luthar SS. Resilience in development: a synthesis of research across five decades. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: vol 3 risk, disorder and adaptation. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. p. 739–95.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Garmezy N. Stress resistant children: the search for protective factors. In: Stevenson J, editor. Recent research in developmental psychology. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1985. p. 213–33. Garmezy N. Stress resistant children: the search for protective factors. In: Stevenson J, editor. Recent research in developmental psychology. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1985. p. 213–33.
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Shiner RL, Masten AS. Childhood personality as a harbinger of competence and resilience in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(2):507–28.CrossRefPubMed Shiner RL, Masten AS. Childhood personality as a harbinger of competence and resilience in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(2):507–28.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Bai S, Repetti RL. Short-term resilience processes in the family. Fam Rel. 2015;64(1):108–19.CrossRef Bai S, Repetti RL. Short-term resilience processes in the family. Fam Rel. 2015;64(1):108–19.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner EE. Protective factors and individual resilience. In: Shonkoff J, Meisels S, editors. Handbook of early childhood intervention. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000. p. 115–32.CrossRef Werner EE. Protective factors and individual resilience. In: Shonkoff J, Meisels S, editors. Handbook of early childhood intervention. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000. p. 115–32.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun J, Stewart D. Development of population-based resilience measures in the primary school setting. Health Educ. 2007;7(6):575–99.CrossRef Sun J, Stewart D. Development of population-based resilience measures in the primary school setting. Health Educ. 2007;7(6):575–99.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat California Department of Education: California healthy kids survey. America: California safe and healthy kids program office; 2004. California Department of Education: California healthy kids survey. America: California safe and healthy kids program office; 2004.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Friendship quality as a predictor of young children’s early school adjustment. Child Dev. 1996;67:1103–18.CrossRefPubMed Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Friendship quality as a predictor of young children’s early school adjustment. Child Dev. 1996;67:1103–18.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Raykov T, Marcolides GA. Introduction to psychometric theory. New York: Routledge; 2011. Raykov T, Marcolides GA. Introduction to psychometric theory. New York: Routledge; 2011.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker CM. What’s the DIF? Why differential item functioning analyses are an important part of instrument development and validation. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2011;29:364–76.CrossRef Walker CM. What’s the DIF? Why differential item functioning analyses are an important part of instrument development and validation. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2011;29:364–76.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers HJ. Differential item functioning. Encyclopedia of statistics in behavioral science. Oxford: Wiley; 2005. Rogers HJ. Differential item functioning. Encyclopedia of statistics in behavioral science. Oxford: Wiley; 2005.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Rae-Grant N, Thomas BH, Offord DR, Boyle MH. Risk, protective factors, and the prevalence of behavioral and emotional disorders in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28(2):262–8.CrossRefPubMed Rae-Grant N, Thomas BH, Offord DR, Boyle MH. Risk, protective factors, and the prevalence of behavioral and emotional disorders in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28(2):262–8.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Formoso D, Gonzales NA, Aiken LS. Family conflict and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior: protective factors. Am J Community Psychol. 2000;28(2):175–99.CrossRefPubMed Formoso D, Gonzales NA, Aiken LS. Family conflict and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior: protective factors. Am J Community Psychol. 2000;28(2):175–99.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Viner RM, Ozer EM, Denny S, Marmot M, Resnick M, Fatusi A, Currie C. Adolescence and the social determinants of health. The Lancet. 2012;379(9826):1641–52.CrossRef Viner RM, Ozer EM, Denny S, Marmot M, Resnick M, Fatusi A, Currie C. Adolescence and the social determinants of health. The Lancet. 2012;379(9826):1641–52.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Deighton J, Tymms P, Vostanis P, Belsky J, Fonagy P, Brown A, Martin A, Patalay P, Wolpert M. The development of a school-based measure of child mental health. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2013;31:247–57.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Deighton J, Tymms P, Vostanis P, Belsky J, Fonagy P, Brown A, Martin A, Patalay P, Wolpert M. The development of a school-based measure of child mental health. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2013;31:247–57.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Patalay P, Deighton J, Fonagy P, Vostanis P, Wolpert M. Clinical validity of the me and my school questionnaire: a self-report mental health measure for children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2014;8(1):1–7.CrossRef Patalay P, Deighton J, Fonagy P, Vostanis P, Wolpert M. Clinical validity of the me and my school questionnaire: a self-report mental health measure for children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2014;8(1):1–7.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Duncan BL, Miller S, Sparks J. The children’s outcome rating scale. Chicago, IL: ISTC; 2003. Duncan BL, Miller S, Sparks J. The children’s outcome rating scale. Chicago, IL: ISTC; 2003.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Ravens-Sieberer U, Wille N, Badia X, Bonsel G, Burström K, Cavrini G, Devlin N, Egmar A-C, Gusi N, Herdman M, et al. Feasibility, reliability, and validity of the EQ-5D-Y: results from a multinational study. Qual Life Res. 2010;19(6):887–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ravens-Sieberer U, Wille N, Badia X, Bonsel G, Burström K, Cavrini G, Devlin N, Egmar A-C, Gusi N, Herdman M, et al. Feasibility, reliability, and validity of the EQ-5D-Y: results from a multinational study. Qual Life Res. 2010;19(6):887–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 6th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998-2011. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 6th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998-2011.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Stochl J, Jones PB, Perez J, Khandaker GM, Böhnke JR, Croudace TJ. Effects of ignoring clustered data structure in confirmatory factor analysis of ordered polytomous items: a simulation study based on PANSS. Int J Methods Psych. 2015;25(3):205–2019.CrossRef Stochl J, Jones PB, Perez J, Khandaker GM, Böhnke JR, Croudace TJ. Effects of ignoring clustered data structure in confirmatory factor analysis of ordered polytomous items: a simulation study based on PANSS. Int J Methods Psych. 2015;25(3):205–2019.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu IM, Agresti A. Mantel-Haenszel-type inference for cumulative odds ratios with a stratified ordinal response. Biometrics. 1996;52:1223–34.CrossRefPubMed Liu IM, Agresti A. Mantel-Haenszel-type inference for cumulative odds ratios with a stratified ordinal response. Biometrics. 1996;52:1223–34.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Penfield R. DIFAS: differential item functioning analysis system. Appl Psychl Meas. 2005;29:150–1.CrossRef Penfield R. DIFAS: differential item functioning analysis system. Appl Psychl Meas. 2005;29:150–1.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén B, du Toit SHC, Spisic D. Robust inference using weighted least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. In unpublished technical report. Los Angeles: Psychometrika; 1997. Muthén B, du Toit SHC, Spisic D. Robust inference using weighted least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. In unpublished technical report. Los Angeles: Psychometrika; 1997.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka JS. How big is big enough?: sample size and goodness of fit in structural equation models with latent variables. Child Dev. 1987;58:134–46.CrossRef Tanaka JS. How big is big enough?: sample size and goodness of fit in structural equation models with latent variables. Child Dev. 1987;58:134–46.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6:1–55.CrossRef Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6:1–55.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Cronbach LJ, Shavelson RJ. My current thoughts on coefficient alpha and successor procedures. Educ Psychol Meas. 2004;64(3):391–418.CrossRef Cronbach LJ, Shavelson RJ. My current thoughts on coefficient alpha and successor procedures. Educ Psychol Meas. 2004;64(3):391–418.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Trizano-Hermosilla I, Alvarado JM. Best alternatives to Cronbach’s alpha reliability in realistic conditions: congeneric and asymmetrical measurements. Front Psychol. 2016;7:769.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Trizano-Hermosilla I, Alvarado JM. Best alternatives to Cronbach’s alpha reliability in realistic conditions: congeneric and asymmetrical measurements. Front Psychol. 2016;7:769.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
33.
Zurück zum Zitat McDonald RP. Generalizability in factorable domains: “Domain validity and generalizability”. Educ Psychol Meas. 1978;38(1):75–9.CrossRef McDonald RP. Generalizability in factorable domains: “Domain validity and generalizability”. Educ Psychol Meas. 1978;38(1):75–9.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Penfield RD, Camilli G. Differential item functioning and item bias. Handb Stat. 2007;26:125–67.CrossRef Penfield RD, Camilli G. Differential item functioning and item bias. Handb Stat. 2007;26:125–67.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Wetzel E, Carstensen CH. Reversed thresholds in partial credit models: a reason for collapsing categories? Assessment. 2014;21(6):765–74.CrossRefPubMed Wetzel E, Carstensen CH. Reversed thresholds in partial credit models: a reason for collapsing categories? Assessment. 2014;21(6):765–74.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Penfield R. An approach for categorizing DIF in polytomous items. Appl Meas Educ. 2007;20:335–55.CrossRef Penfield R. An approach for categorizing DIF in polytomous items. Appl Meas Educ. 2007;20:335–55.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Penfield RD, Algina J. A generalized DIF effect variance estimator for measuring unsigned differential test functioning in mixed format tests. J Educ Meas. 2006;43:295–312.CrossRef Penfield RD, Algina J. A generalized DIF effect variance estimator for measuring unsigned differential test functioning in mixed format tests. J Educ Meas. 2006;43:295–312.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Mellor DJ. Every day and eternal acts: exploring children’s friendships in the primary school. In working paper 88. School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University: Cardiff; 2007. Mellor DJ. Every day and eternal acts: exploring children’s friendships in the primary school. In working paper 88. School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University: Cardiff; 2007.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Rabaglietti E, Vacirca MF, Zucchetti G, Ciairano S. Similarity, cohesion, and friendship networks among boys and girls: a one-year follow-up study among Italian children. Curr Psychol. 2012;31(3):246–62.CrossRef Rabaglietti E, Vacirca MF, Zucchetti G, Ciairano S. Similarity, cohesion, and friendship networks among boys and girls: a one-year follow-up study among Italian children. Curr Psychol. 2012;31(3):246–62.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Huston A, Ripke M. Developmental contexts in middle childhood. Bridges to adolescence and adulthood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006.CrossRef Huston A, Ripke M. Developmental contexts in middle childhood. Bridges to adolescence and adulthood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Benn R, Akiva T, Arel S, Roeser RW. Mindfulness training effects for parents and educators of children with special needs. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1476–87.CrossRefPubMed Benn R, Akiva T, Arel S, Roeser RW. Mindfulness training effects for parents and educators of children with special needs. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1476–87.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Mouw T, Entwisle B. Residential segregation and interracial friendship in schools. Am J Sociol. 2006;112:394–441.CrossRef Mouw T, Entwisle B. Residential segregation and interracial friendship in schools. Am J Sociol. 2006;112:394–441.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Wetzel E, Böhnke JR, Brown A. Response biases. In: Leong FTL, Bartram D, Cheung FM, Geisinger KF, Iliescu D, editors. The ITC International Handbook of Testing and Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016. p. 349–63. Wetzel E, Böhnke JR, Brown A. Response biases. In: Leong FTL, Bartram D, Cheung FM, Geisinger KF, Iliescu D, editors. The ITC International Handbook of Testing and Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016. p. 349–63.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Masten AS, Shaffer A. How families matter in child development: reflections from research on risk and resilience. In: Clarke-Stewart A, Dunn J, editors. Families count: effects on child and adolescent development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2006. p. 5–25.CrossRef Masten AS, Shaffer A. How families matter in child development: reflections from research on risk and resilience. In: Clarke-Stewart A, Dunn J, editors. Families count: effects on child and adolescent development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2006. p. 5–25.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams LR, Degnan KA, Perez-Edgar KE, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Pine DS, Steinberg L, Fox NA. Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2009;37(8):1063–75.CrossRef Williams LR, Degnan KA, Perez-Edgar KE, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Pine DS, Steinberg L, Fox NA. Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2009;37(8):1063–75.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Bank L, Burraston B, Snyder J. Sibling conflict and ineffective parenting as predictors of adolescent boys’ antisocial behavior and peer difficulties: additive and interactional effects. J Res Adolesc. 2004;14(1):99–125.CrossRef Bank L, Burraston B, Snyder J. Sibling conflict and ineffective parenting as predictors of adolescent boys’ antisocial behavior and peer difficulties: additive and interactional effects. J Res Adolesc. 2004;14(1):99–125.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Bukowski WM, Brengden M, Vitaro F. Peer relations. In: Grusex JE, Hastings PD, editors. Handbook of socialization. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 355–81. Bukowski WM, Brengden M, Vitaro F. Peer relations. In: Grusex JE, Hastings PD, editors. Handbook of socialization. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 355–81.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Sowislo JF, Orth U. Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(1):213–40.CrossRefPubMed Sowislo JF, Orth U. Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(1):213–40.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Becker-Weidman EG, Jacobs RH, Reinecke MA, Silva SG, March JS. Social problem-solving among adolescents treated for depression. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(1):11–8.CrossRefPubMed Becker-Weidman EG, Jacobs RH, Reinecke MA, Silva SG, March JS. Social problem-solving among adolescents treated for depression. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(1):11–8.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Lohman BJ, Jarvis PA. Adolescent stressors, coping strategies, and psychological health studied in the family context. J Youth Adolesc. 2000;29(1):15–43.CrossRef Lohman BJ, Jarvis PA. Adolescent stressors, coping strategies, and psychological health studied in the family context. J Youth Adolesc. 2000;29(1):15–43.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connor LE, Berry JW, Lewis T, Mulherin K, Crisostomo PS. Empathy and depression: the moral system on overdrive. In: Farrow TFD, Woodruff PWR, editors. Empathy in mental illness. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 49–75.CrossRef O’Connor LE, Berry JW, Lewis T, Mulherin K, Crisostomo PS. Empathy and depression: the moral system on overdrive. In: Farrow TFD, Woodruff PWR, editors. Empathy in mental illness. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 49–75.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Zahn-Waxler C, Cole PM, Barrett KC. Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression. In: Garber J, Dodge KA, editors. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1991. p. 243–72.CrossRef Zahn-Waxler C, Cole PM, Barrett KC. Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression. In: Garber J, Dodge KA, editors. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1991. p. 243–72.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Benzies K, Mychasiuk R. Fostering family resiliency: a review of the key protective factors. Child Fam Soc Work. 2009;14:103–14.CrossRef Benzies K, Mychasiuk R. Fostering family resiliency: a review of the key protective factors. Child Fam Soc Work. 2009;14:103–14.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The student resilience survey: psychometric validation and associations with mental health
verfasst von
Suzet Tanya Lereya
Neil Humphrey
Praveetha Patalay
Miranda Wolpert
Jan R. Böhnke
Amy Macdougall
Jessica Deighton
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1753-2000
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0132-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.