Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Research

Time course of changes in motor-cognitive exergame performances during task-specific training in patients with dementia: identification and predictors of early training response

Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Some studies have already suggested that exergame interventions can be effective to improve physical, cognitive, motor-cognitive, and psychological outcomes in patients with dementia (PwD). However, little is known about the training volume required to induce such positive effects and the inter-individual differences in training response among PwD. The aim of the study was to analyze the time course of changes in motor-cognitive exergame performances during a task-specific training program and to identify predictors of early training response in PwD.

Methods

Secondary analyses of data from the intervention group (IG) of a randomized, placebo-controlled trial to improve motor-cognitive performances in PwD. Fifty-six geriatric patients with mild-to-moderate dementia randomized to the IG underwent a 10-week, task-specific training program (2×/week) on an exergame-based balance training system (Physiomat®), combining postural control tasks with cognitive tasks of an established neuropsychological test (Trail Making Test). Main outcome was the time required to complete different Physiomat®-Tasks (PTs) assessed at baseline (T1), training session 7 (TS7) and 14 (TS14), and post-intervention after 20 training sessions (T2). Reliable change indices were used to identify early responders from T1 to TS7. A multivariate logistic regression analysis was performed to determine independent predictors of early training response.

Results

Completion time significantly improved already from T1 to TS7 in all PTs (p ≤ .001–.006), with moderate to very large effect sizes (r = .38–.52; Cohen’s d = .85–1.45). For most PTs, significant progressive improvements from TS7 to TS14 and TS14 to T2 were not observed. Thirty-one (59.6%) participants were classified as early responders and 21 (40.4%) as non-early responders. Lower baseline exergame performance and lower visuospatial and divided attention abilities were independently associated with early training response.

Conclusions

Substantial task-specific improvements in complex motor-cognitive exergame performances can be obtained within a surprisingly short intervention period in PwD. Our results confirm that not only an excellent training response can be achieved in this patient population, but also that more vulnerable patients with greater deficits in domain-specific cognitive functions associated with fall risk may even reap the most and fastest benefit from motor-cognitive exergame interventions.

Trial registration

ISRCTN registry, ISRCTN37232817 (retrospectively registered on 04/02/2012).
Abkürzungen
ANOVA
Analysis of variance
CERAD
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease
CERAD-TS
Total score of the CERAD test battery
CI
Confidence interval
DST-NAI
Digit-Span Test of the Nuremberg Age Inventory
DTC
Dual-task cost
ER
Early responder
FTBT
Physiomat®-Follow The Ball Task
IG
Intervention group
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
NER
Non-early responder
OR
Odds ratio
POMA
Performance Oriented Mobility Assessment
PTMT
Physiomat®-Trail Making Task
PwD
Patients with dementia
RCI
Reliable change index
RCT
Randomized controlled trial
rtt
Test-retest reliability
SD
Standard deviation
SEM
Standard error of measurement
T2
Post-intervention
TMT-NAI
‘Number-Connection-Test’ (Trail Making Test) of the Nuremberg Age Inventory
TS14
Training session 14
TS7
Training session 7

Background

Attention is the first non-memory cognitive domain to be affected in dementia [1]. Divided attention, which is part of attentional control and the ability that allows individuals to perform two tasks simultaneously (i.e. dual tasks), represents the most affected aspect of attention [2, 3]. Under dual-task conditions, patients with dementia (PwD) or older people with cognitive impairment showed significantly reduced physical functions such as muscle strength [4], gait performance [5], and postural control [6, 7] compared to cognitively healthy older adults. Because everyday life involves many dual-task situations (e.g. walking while talking to someone) and deficits in dual-tasking have been associated with functional decline [8, 9] and falls [10, 11], it has been suggested to incorporate dual-task exercises into preventive or rehabilitative training programs [1214].

Exergaming

Exergaming represents an emerging and unique form of dual-task training [15, 16], combining physical exercise with cognitively-challenging tasks in an interactive game-based way. In contrast to more traditional motor-cognitive dual-task exercises that combine distinct training tasks (e.g. walking while counting backwards), exergaming typically involves cognitive challenges directly embedded within the physical body movements that need to be performed to complete the game tasks projected onto a display screen [16]. The use of exergames in physical exercise and rehabilitation programs is progressively expanding as their playful character might help to encourage older people to participate in physical activity and to enhance their motivation toward exercise adherence [17, 18].

Potential benefits of exergaming

Recent systematic reviews have shown positive effects of exergaming on physical, cognitive, dual-task and psychosocial outcomes in cognitively healthy older adults [1926]. Given the growing evidence of its beneficial effects, increasing attention has recently been paid to exergaming also in older people with cognitive impairment and PwD. Some studies have already suggested that exergame interventions can be effective to improve balance and/or gait [2729], motor-functional status and exercise capacity [30], global cognitive functioning and/or domain-specific cognitive functions (e.g. memory, attention, visuospatial and constructional abilities) [3133], fear of falling [28, 29], depressive symptoms [31], and exergame performances in these populations [3436] (for review, see also [37]).

Time course of exergame-induced benefits

General dose-response relationships for traditional physical exercise suggest that untrained individuals may experience significant training benefits from low intensities, frequencies, and/or durations with small increases already during the early training period; however, with increasing performance of individuals, the magnitude of benefit may become less for a similar increase in intensity or amount of activity in the following training period [38, 39]. Although such knowledge is highly relevant for clinicians and practitioners to design time-efficient training protocols, little is yet known about dose-response relationships between exergaming (e.g. total duration of training period, number of training sessions) and its beneficial effects in older people with and without cognitive impairment or PwD [23, 40, 41]. Most frequently, previous studies did not include multiple assessment tests during the intervention period to address this research gap, but tested their participants only before and after the exergame-based training program, with intervention periods ranging widely from 1 to 24 weeks [2835] (for review, see also [1926]). Few studies conducted in older patients with Parkinson’s disease and healthy elderly [42, 43], older patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease [44], older adults with depression [45], or older community-dwelling fallers [46] used mid-term tests (at week 3–6) to assess physical and psychological outcomes after 30% or 50% of the total exergame intervention period (6–12 weeks, 2–5×/week, 30–40 min) [42, 45, 46] or investigated the intersession progression in exergame performances during a 7-week intervention period (2×/week) [43]. Most frequently, these studies observed significant improvements in study outcomes (e.g. balance, depressive symptoms, exergame performance) already after 2 to 4 weeks [42, 43, 46]. To our knowledge, in PwD, a similar study design including not only pre- and post-intervention assessments has been used only in two studies [27, 36]. Padala et al. [27], demonstrated significant balance improvements at post-intervention after 8 weeks but not at the mid-term of their exergame intervention after 4 weeks (5×/week, 30 min); and Fenney and Lee [36], who addressed the intersession changes of exergame performance during a 9-week intervention period (1×/week, 60 min), only described case reports, with heterogeneous results among cases (i.e. both improvements and deteriorations during the early training period), making it difficult to interpret their findings. In all these studies, training-induced changes in outcomes were always evaluated only relative to the pre-intervention assessment. None of them addressed comparisons between mid-term and post-intervention assessments [27, 42, 4446], nor did they compare other assessment sessions during the intervention period among each other [43], which might have provided an even more detailed insight into the time course of changes in outcomes.

Predictors of exergame training response

In people with cognitive impairment and PwD, effective exergame-based intervention studies have most frequently reported only main effects or mean group differences in changes without addressing inter-individual variability for their outcomes. However, individual differences and the identification of factors associated with training response has high clinical relevance [47]. For example, some persons may respond more favorably to an exergame intervention or the duration of the intervention period necessary to produce significant benefits may differ between persons with specific characteristics. To our knowledge, only Schwenk et al. [28] analyzed predictors of training response to a 4-week exergame-based balance training program in people with cognitive impairment (2×/week, 45 min), suggesting that low baseline performance and a history of falls were associated with greater improvements in balance.

Previous work

We recently evaluated a 10-week, task-specific, motor-cognitive training program with an interactive, exergame-based balance training system (Physiomat®) in PwD. Results of our randomized controlled trial (RCT) demonstrated that, compared to a non-specific, motor placebo activity (unspecific, low-intensity strength and flexibility exercises for the upper body while seated), an exergame intervention significantly improved motor-cognitive performances of PwD in trained and untrained exergame tasks, with partly sustainable effects up to 3 months after training cessation [35]. Training-induced changes during the intervention period and predictors of early training response have not been addressed in this study.

Study aims and hypotheses

The primary aim of these secondary analyses was to provide a more detailed insight into the time course of improvements in motor-cognitive exergame performances during a task-specific training program in patients with mild-to-moderate dementia. A secondary aim was to identify predictive factors associated with early training response. Based on general dose-response relationships for physical exercise [38, 39] and previous findings on predictors of exergame training response in people with cognitive impairment [28], we hypothesized that (1) time course of improvements in exergame performances would also be asymptotic, with the greatest training gains occurring during the early training period and (2) patients with the lowest initial performance would benefit most and the fastest from the exergame intervention.

Methods

Study design

This study presents secondary analyses of a double-blinded, randomized, placebo-controlled intervention trial on the effects of a dementia-specific motor-cognitive training program in patients with mild-to-moderate dementia. Details about the design, intervention, and main analyses of the RCT have been described previously [35, 48, 49]. The secondary analyses involved the motor-cognitive exergame performance of the intervention group (IG) on the Physiomat®, which was assessed at baseline before training (T1), at training session 7 (TS7) and 14 (TS14), and at post-intervention after 20 training sessions (T2) to analyze changes in the Physiomat® performance over the time course of the study period. Baseline patient characteristics were analyzed as potential predictors of early training response from T1 to TS7 for the Physiomat® performance.

Study population

The process of screening, recruitment, enrollment and randomization of participants has been previously described in detail [35]. Inclusion criteria were as follows: ≥ 65 years; Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE, [50]) score 17–26; diagnosis of probable dementia based on comprehensive neuropsychological testing (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease [CERAD] test battery, [51]); ability to walk at least 10 m without a walking aid; no severe neurologic, cardiovascular, metabolic, or psychiatric disorders; residence within 15 km of the study center, and written informed consent.

Intervention

Participants took part in an interactive, exergame-based training on the Physiomat® for 10 weeks (2×/week à 10 min; 20 sessions in total) supervised by a qualified trainer experienced in training PwD. The Physiomat® includes a balance platform moveable in the sagittal, frontal, and transversal plane. By platform-integrated displacement sensors, the three-dimensional movements of the platform are recorded and translated into linear movement of a cursor displayed on a 17-in. computer screen (Fig. 1). To solve a Physiomat® game task shown on the screen, the player must control and move the cursor by bending, tilting, and rotation movements while standing on the platform. A dementia-specific, patient-centered training approach (e.g. verbal instructions and cueing, haptic assistance, verbal praise) [52, 53] was used to train participants in performing a Physiomat®-Follow The Ball Task (FTBT) and more cognitively challenging Physiomat®-Trail Making Tasks (PTMTs) on five different complexity levels as defined by the number of digits to be connected (i.e. 4, 7, 9, 14, or 20 digits). The PTMTs were based on an established neuropsychological test (‘Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test’ = ‘Number-Connection-Test’ of the Nuremberg Age Inventory, TMT-NAI, [54], which is a modified version of the Trail Making Test validated for use in PwD.
Participants were instructed to move the cursor on the screen by weight shifting on the platform while holding onto the Physiomat® handles to follow a moving ball on the screen (FTBT) or to connect the digits in ascending order (PTMTs) as fast as possible (for further details on the intervention, see [35]). According to the participants’ individual performance level, the complexity of the Physiomat® tasks was successively increased as training progressed. The decision point for increasing the complexity was the ability to complete a Physiomat® task safely without assistance.

Measurements

Outcome variable

The main outcome variable of this study was the Physiomat® performance assessed as the time required to complete the Physiomat® tasks (duration), which was directly derived from the data stream of the Physiomat®-integrated sensors during the gameplay [55]. The tasks included the same FTBT and PTMTs trained during the intervention period, and the same instructions were given as used for the Physiomat® training. No cueing or haptic assistance was provided by test administrator during the assessment. Each test session was started with the FTBT. After successfully completing the FTBT, the PTMT level was successively increased during the assessment until the participant was no longer able to complete a level. Participants performed two trials for each Physiomat® task, where the trial with the shortest duration was used for statistical analysis.

Descriptive and predictor variables

Demographic and clinical characteristics including age, gender, education, comorbidity (number of diagnoses, medications), recent history of falls (previous year), social status (community-dwelling vs. institutionalized), and physiological status for depression (Geriatric Depression Scale, 15-item version [56]) and fear of falling (Falls Efficacy Scale-International, 7-item version [57]) were documented from patient charts or by standardized patient interview.
Motor-functional status was measured by the Performance-Oriented Mobility Assessment (POMA, [58]), the Timed Up and Go [59], and the 5-chair stand test [60].
Cognitive status was screened using the MMSE [50]. Domain-specific cognitive functions were assessed by the CERAD test battery [51], including subtests of verbal fluency, visual naming (Boston Naming Test), verbal episodic memory encoding (word list memory), recall (word list recall) and recognition (word list recognition), visuospatial ability (constructional praxis), non-verbal episodic memory (constructional recall), and phonemic fluency; the modified Trail Making Test (TMT-NAI, [54]) for speed of information processing; and a Digit-Span Test (DST-NAI, [54]) for working memory. To assess global cognitive functioning, the demographically corrected CERAD total score (CERAD-TS) was calculated [61].
Motor-cognitive dual-task performance (divided attention) was assessed using a simultaneous walking and working memory task. Participants walked along a 5.79 m long GAITRite® instrumented walkway (CIR Systems Inc., Havertown, PA, USA: 4.88 m active area; 120 Hz sampling rate) at a maximum pace while counting backwards as fast as possible in steps of three. Each walk was initiated and terminated 1 m before and after the walkway to account for acceleration and deceleration. To quantify the overall motor-cognitive dual-task performance, the relative dual-task costs (DTC = ([dual task – single task]/single task × 100, [62]) of gait speed (DTCgait speed) and calculation steps (DTCcounting) were combined (DTCcombined = (DTCgait speed + DTCcounting)/2). The dual-task test procedure and data processing as well as the biometrical quality of this dual-task assessment have been previously described in detail [63].
Initial training adherence from T1 to TS7 was documented as the percentage of the first seven training sessions attended relative to the number of the maximum possible training sessions offered in this period in which the participant completed these seven training sessions.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive data were presented as frequencies and percentages for categorical variables, and means, standard deviations (SD) and ranges or medians and ranges for continuous variables as appropriate. To identify significant differences in the Physiomat® performance between the individual test sessions (T1, TS7, TS14, T2), we used one-way repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) or Friedman ANOVAs on ranks (for non-normally distributed data) with Bonferroni-adjusted post-hoc paired-samples t-tests and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, respectively. Effect sizes for post-hoc comparisons were calculated as Cohen’s d for paired-samples t-tests (0.2 ≤ d < 0.5 = small, 0.5 ≤ d < 0.8 = moderate, 0.8 ≤ d < 1.3 = large, d ≥ 1.3 = very large effect) and as effect size r for the Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (0.1 ≤ r < 0.3 = small, 0.3 ≤ r < 0.5 = moderate, 0.5 ≤ r < 0.7 = large, r ≥ 0.7 = very large effect) [64, 65]. Bonferroni-adjusted p-values were reported for the post-hoc multiple comparisons.
Reliable change indices (RCIs [66, 67]) were computed for the duration of each Physiomat® task to identify early responders from T1 to TS7. The RCI can be used to evaluate whether a participant’s change in pre- and post-intervention scores is beyond that which might be due to random measurement error, considering the instrument reliability and the sample variability specific for the population of interest. Based on data (i.e. test-retest reliabilities [rtt]; standard deviations of test [SD1] and retest [SD2]) of our previously published study on the biometrical quality of the Physiomat® assessment in PwD [55], the RCI for each Physiomat® task was calculated as follows: (1) standard error of measurement (SEM1/2 = SD1/2 × √[1 – rtt]); (2) standard error of the difference (Sdiff = √[SEM12 + SEM22]), and (3) reliable change index (RCI = 1.96 × Sdiff). The calculated RCIs (FTBT: ± 9.5 s; PTMT level 1: ± 5.0 s, level 2: ± 7.5 s, level 3: ± 9.1, level 4: ± 11.0 s, level 5: ± 17.7 s) were used to determine whether a participant’s improvement in a specific Physiomat® task was sufficiently large to yield confidence that it was not due to measurement error. Taking into account the participant’s individual Physiomat® performance at baseline and the different complex Physiomat® tasks, an early responder (ER) was defined as a participant with an individual decrease in the duration after TS7 that exceeded the RCI either (1) for the most complex Physiomat® task completed at T1 or (2) for at least 50% of the Physiomat® tasks completed at T1. All other participants were defined as “non-early responders” (NER). To identify potential predictive factors associated with early training response, univariate analyses examined differences in participant baseline characteristics between ER and NER using unpaired t-tests, Mann-Whitney U-tests, and χ2 tests as appropriate. Independent baseline variables included demographic characteristics, comorbidity, psychological and motor-functional status, history of falls, global and domain-specific cognitive functioning, dual-task performance, baseline Physiomat® performances, and initial training adherence (Table 2). For the subsequent analysis, global cognitive functioning was defined by the CERAD-TS, which is regarded as being superior to scores of simplified screening measures such as the MMSE [68], and the baseline Physiomat® performance was defined by the FTBT duration, as the FTBT was the only Physiomat® task with baseline data available for all participants. Variables that showed significant differences in the univariate analyses were entered in a multivariate logistic regression analysis (likelihood ratio-based forward stepwise method) to determine independent predictors of early training response. Results of the regression model were reported as odds ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The goodness-of-fit of the regression model was assessed by the Hosmer-Lemeshow test, and the amount of variance explained by model was expressed as Nagelkerke R2. Prediction accuracy of the model was defined as the percentage of correctly classified ERs and NERs. A two-sided p-value of ≤ .05 indicated statistical significance. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Sample characteristics

The sample for the secondary analyses included the 56 RCT participants allocated to the IG. Participants were multimorbid older patients with mild-to-moderate dementia and impaired motor-functional status (Table 1). Forty-five participants completed all four Physiomat® test sessions (T1, TS7, TS14, T2) over the study period. Four participants dropped out before TS7 due to serious medical events (n = 2, 3.6%) or lack of motivation (n = 2, 3.6%), and another seven dropped out during the later course of the intervention due to lack of motivation (n = 4, 7.1%), injurious falls (n = 2, 3.6%), and death (n = 1, 1.8%). Participants who dropped out did not differ significantly from those who stayed in the study for any descriptive variable at baseline (p = .327–.909) or any parameter for effects of intervention (p = .235–.933).
Table 1
Sample characteristics
Variable
Total sample (n = 56)
Age, years
82.7 ± 6.2 [65–94]
Females
39 (69.6)
Mini-Mental State Examination, score
22.2 ± 2.8 [17–26]
Education, years
11 [7–20]
Diagnoses
7.7 ± 3.8 [1–18]
Medications
7.6 ± 3.4 [0–14]
Taking cholinesterase inhibitors or memantine
13 (23.2)
Timed Up and Go, s
14.6 [6.5–52.7]
Performance Oriented Mobility Assessment, score
22.4 ± 4.3 [9–28]
5-chair stand test, s
14.8 ± 7.6 [6.8–39.1]
Geriatric Depression Scale, score
2 [0–9]
Falls Efficacy Scale-International, score
8.5 [7–19]
Fall in the previous year
23 (41.1)
Living situation
 Community-dwelling
39 (69.6)
 Institutionalized
17 (30.4)
Data are presented as mean ± SD [range], n (%), or median [range]

Time course of Physiomat® performance

All 45 participants who stayed in the study successfully completed the rather low cognitively challenging FTBT at all four test sessions; however, the increasing difficulty of the Physiomat® tasks led to gradually decreasing samples sizes for the other, more complex PTMTs (level 1: n = 43 to level 5: n = 13, Fig. 2) as the participants reached their performance limit at individually different Physiomat® tasks.
Significant differences in the Physiomat® performance between the individual test sessions were found for all Physiomat® tasks (p < .001). Post-hoc analyses revealed significant improvements from T1 to TS7 in all Physiomat® tasks (p ≤ .001–.006), with moderate to very large effect sizes (FTBT, PTMT level 1 & 2: r = .38–.52; PTMT level 3–5: d = .85–1.45). For 4 out of 6 Physiomat® tasks, the Physiomat® performance did not significantly change between TS7 and TS14 (p = .359–.999). During this intermediate phase of the intervention period, significant positive intervention effects were observed only in the PTMT level 2 (p = .035) and level 4 (p = .026), with moderate effect sizes (level 2: r = .32; level 4: d = .68). From TS14 to T2, the Physiomat® performance significantly improved only in the PTMT level 1 (p = .009) and level 4 (p = .016). Effect sizes for these significant improvements were moderate (level 1: r = .34; level 4: d = .74). For all four other Physiomat® tasks, no significant improvements (p = .153–.999) were found during this late phase of the intervention period (TS14 to T2). For each Physiomat® task, the largest effect size between two consecutive test sessions was observed from T1 to TS7 (e.g. PTMT level 3: T1 to TS7: d = .85; TS7 to TS14: d = .34; TS14 to T2: d = .22). Over the last two thirds of the intervention period (TS7 to T2), the Physiomat® performance significantly improved across all Physiomat® tasks (p ≤ .001–.036), with moderate to very large effect sizes (FTBT, PTMT level 1 & 2: r = .34–.39; PTMT level 3–5: d = .53–1.34) and the largest effect sizes for the most complex Physiomat® tasks (PTMT level 4 & 5: d = 1.28–1.34).

Predictors of early training response

Potential predictive factors associated with early training response were analyzed in all the participants who completed the Physiomat® test sessions at T1 and TS7 (n = 52). Based on the definition of early training response by the RCIs across the different Physiomat® tasks, 31 (59.6%) participants were classified as ERs and 21 (40.4%) as NERs. The changes of ERs and NERs from T1 to TS7 within the individual Physiomat® tasks were presented in Fig. 3, suggesting that ERs showed lower baseline performance with higher improvements during this early training period up to a performance level at TS7 similar to those of the NERs. At post-intervention (T2), no significant differences in the Physiomat® tasks were found between ERs and NERs (p = .101–.911).
Comparisons between the baseline characteristics of two subgroups revealed that ERs initially showed significantly lower Physiomat® performances (FTBT: p < .001; PTMT: p ≤ .001–.036) as already indicated in Fig. 3, lower global cognitive functioning (MMSE: p = .039; CERAD-TS: p = .026), lower visuospatial ability (constructional praxis: p = .006), lower speed of information processing (TMT-NAI: p = .001), and lower dual-task performance (DTCcombined: p = .040) than NERs (Table 2).
Table 2
Baseline comparisons between participant characteristics for early responders and non-early responders
Variable
ERs (n = 31)
NERs (n = 21)
p-value
Age, yearsa
83.5 ± 6.5 [65–93]
82.3 ± 6.0 [70–94]
.514
Femalesb
23 (74.2)
14 (66.7)
.557
Mini-Mental State Examination, scorea
21.4 ± 2.8 [17–26]
23.0 ± 2.4 [17–26]
.039
Diagnosesa
7.5 ± 3.4 [1–17]
7.5 ± 4.1 [1–17]
.994
Medicationsa
7.3 ± 3.3 [0–13]
8.1 ± 3.6 [0–14]
.419
Timed Up and Go, sc
14.8 [9.8–52.7]
13.9 [6.5–51.2]
.714
Performance Oriented Mobility Assessment, scorea
22.2 ± 4.0 [12–28]
23.1 ± 3.9 [15–28]
.405
5-chair stand test, s
14.8 ± 7.6 [6.8–39.1]
13.5 ± 5.1 [7.2–29.4]
.522
Geriatric Depression Scale, scorec
2 [0–9]
2 [0–8]
.799
Falls Efficacy Scale-International, scorec
8 [7–13]
8 [7–19]
.826
Fall in the previous year
13 (41.9)
8 (38.1)
.782
CERAD scoresa
 Total score
70.7 ± 8.7 [54–87]
77.2 ± 12.0 [54–87]
.026
 Verbal fluency
9.7 ± 3.7 [3–17]
11.3 ± 3.5 [4–18]
.124
 Boston Naming Test
9.9 ± 2.7 [5–15]
11.0 ± 2.4 [5–14]
.150
 Word list memory
10.5 ± 3.1 [4–16]
11.2 ± 3.8 [5–16]
.484
 Word list recall
2.1 ± 1.8 [0–6]
2.0 ± 1.8 [0–5]
.900
 Word list recognition
6.1 ± 2.7 [1–10]
7.1 ± 2.4 [2–10]
.172
 Constructional praxis
7.0 ± 2.3 [2–11]
8.7 ± 1.8 [6–11]
.006
 Constructional recall
1.7 ± 2.1 [0–7]
2.3 ± 2.2 [0–6]
.337
 Phonemic fluency
6.8 ± 3.1 [0–15]
6.7 ± 4.0 [0–16]
.900
TMT-NAI, sc
114 [35–300]
57 [32–300]
.001
DST-NAI scorea
8.6 ± 1.3 [6–11]
9.2 ± 1.0 [7–11]
.098
Dual-task performance (DTCcombined), %a
−36.1 ± 22.3 [− 82.5- -0.4]
−24.0 ± 16.8 [− 54.0- -0.2]
.040
Baseline Physiomat® performance, s
 FTBTc
37.6 [18.5–121.1]
21.2 [17.1–31.5]
< .001
 PTMT-L1c,d
15.6 [5.8–136.1]
8.2 [5.3–19.8]
< .001
 PTMT-L2c,e
24.7 [10.3–57.3]
15.8 [10.5–21.5]
< .001
 PTMT-L3a,f
34.7 ± 12.4 [21.6–64.5]
22.4 ± 6.4 [13.2–37.6]
< .001
 PTMT-L4a,g
61.5 ± 16.2 [37.8–91.4]
44.9 ± 12.0 [31.6–67.5]
.007
 PTMT-L5a,h
66.4 ± 8.7 [52.6–75.1]
53.7 ± 10.7 [39.1–76.9]
.036
Training adherence at TS7a
81.1 ± 20.3 [42.9–100]
76.7 ± 20.9 [33.3–100]
.451
Data are presented as mean ± SD [range], n (%), or median [range]. ERs, early responders; NERs, non-early responders; CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease, TMT-NAI, Trail Making Test from the Nuremberg Age Inventory; DST-NAI, Digit-Span Test from the Nuremberg Age Inventory; DTCcombined, combined dual-task costs (i.e. [motor + cognitive dual-tasks costs]/2); FTBT, Physiomat®-Follow The Ball Task; PTMT, Physiomat®-Trail Making Task; L1–5, level 1–5. P-values for at-tests, bχ2 test, and cMann-Whitney U-tests. P-values in bold indicate statistical significance (p ≤ .05). Comparison between dn = 30 ERs vs. n = 20 NERs, en = 27 ERs vs. n = 18 NERs, fn = 21 ERs vs. n = 18 NERs; gn = 12 ERs vs. n = 13 NERs, hn = 5 ERs vs. n = 9 NERs
When the variables significantly associated with early training response were entered into the multivariate logistic regression model, baseline Physiomat® performance (OR = 1.261, p = .003), constructional praxis (OR = 0.558, p = .019), and dual-task performance (OR = 0.943, p = .031), were identified as independent predictors of early training response (Table 3). The regression model was significant (χ2 = 33.96, df = 3, p < .001) and demonstrated goodness-of-fit (Hosmer-Lemeshow-Test: χ2 = 5.45, df = 8, p = .709). It accounted for 64.8% of variance in training response (Nagelkerke R2 = .648) and correctly classified 84.6% participants as ERs or NERs.
Table 3
Multivariate logistic regression model for predictors of early training response
Variable
β
SEM
OR (95% CI)
P-value
Constructional praxisa
−.583
.248
.558 (.344–.907)
.019
Baseline Physiomat® performanceb
−.232
.079
1.261 (1.081–1.471)
.003
Dual-task performancec
−.058
.027
.943 (.895–.995)
.031
Removed from model: TMT-NAI (p = .745), CERAD total score (p = .565). aLower scores indicate lower constructional praxis ability; bhigher scores indicate lower baseline Physiomat® performance; clower scores indicate lower dual-task performance

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the presented study is the first (1) to provide a detailed insight into the time course of changes in motor-cognitive exergame performances during a task-specific training program and (2) to examine potential predictive factors that are associated with early training response in the vulnerable population of multimorbid older patients with mild-to-moderate dementia and impaired motor-functional status.

Time course of Physiomat® performance

Deficits in motor-cognitive dual-tasking has been repeatedly identified as predictor for functional decline [8, 9] and falls [10, 11], and dual-task abilities have been reported to be reduced in PwD [57]. One of the major findings of this study was that in PwD exergame-based dual-task performances can be substantially improved after a surprisingly short period of task-specific interactive motor-cognitive training. We observed significant improvements in all Physiomat® tasks already after 3 weeks.
In older patients with Parkinson’s disease and healthy elderly, significant improvements in some but not all Nintendo Wii Fit™ exergames have also been reported after similar durations of task-specific training (2–3 weeks) [43]. In PwD, only a usability study was found, showing that exergame performances can be improved after 4 weeks of task-specific training [34]. However, the frequency and total amount of exercise during the intervention period in this study were substantially higher (4 × 60 min/week = 240 min), and the exergame intervention covered interactive video-sports games with considerably less complex concurrent motor-cognitive tasks (Nintendo Wii Sports™ Bowling and Tennis), compared to our study with a training protocol of two 10 min-exergame sessions per week (3 × 20 min/week = 60 min) on an exergame-based balance platform combining whole-body postural control tasks with complex cognitive tasks of an established neuropsychological test (TMT-NAI, [54]). The prompt training-induced effects on such interactive motor-cognitive dual-task performances have not been previously reported in PwD. These findings suggest that an exergame-based motor-cognitive training program has the potential to represent a time-efficient intervention for improving movement control under cognitive load in PwD, which may contribute to reduced fall risk in this highly vulnerable patient population [14]. Such interactive motor-cognitive training programs might also be associated with more general effects on global cognitive functioning, as previously reported for exergaming [32] or interactive visuomotor game training [69] in cognitively impaired older adults after 10- to 14-week training periods. Based on our findings that improvements in task-specific exergame performances can be made after a much shorter training period, future studies should assess whether potential transfer effects related to exergame interventions can be achieved also after short intervention periods, or even assess the time course of such potential transfer effects.
The time course of improvements in exergame performances seemed to be asymptotic rather than linear, with the greatest part of the total training effects already apparent after the early, 3-week intervention phase (T1 to TS7) across all Physiomat® tasks. After these prompt initial improvements at TS7, the positive effect of the intervention decreased and participants seemed to reach a training plateau during the intermediate intervention phase given the chosen training frequency and intensity. For most of the Physiomat® tasks, the extension of the intervention period by another two training intervals of 3 weeks did not result in significant progressive improvements compared to the corresponding previous test session (TS7 vs. TS14, TS14 vs. T2). However, taking this two training intervals together (TS7 to T2 = 6 weeks), the performance in all Physiomat® tasks significantly improved over the last two thirds of the intervention period. This time course of exergame performances confirmed our hypothesis and is in line with established exercise training principles and general dose-response relationships reported for exercise training [38, 39], suggesting that in training novices and untrained individuals with high potential for improvements, a low training dosage (intensity, frequency, duration) with small increases may already be sufficient to induce significant improvements after a short training period; however, as training duration continues and the performance of individuals increases, the rate of improvements begins to slow down for a similar increase in intensity or amount of activity in the following training period. Thus, a higher rate in the increase of the training parameters (intensity, frequency, and/or duration) is required to provide further effective training stimuli. For each Physiomat® task, we observed significant improvements after the initial intervention phase (T1 to TS7), which in turn may have decreased the participant’s potential for improvements within the tasks during the subsequent training period. To achieve further training gains from the increased performance level and this test session on, a training intensification in the following training period seems to have been necessary. The frequency, i.e. the number of training sessions per week, could not be increased in our study due to its design, but we were able to successively further increase the intensity, i.e. the complexity level of the Physiomat® tasks, as participants improved and training progressed. However, for some participants, a higher rate in increase of the intensity was not possible, as the number of available complexity levels of the Physiomat® tasks was limited. For example, the intensity for high-preforming participants that reached the most complex Physiomat® tasks during the early training period could only be further increased by instructions to complete the most complex Physiomat® tasks even more quickly. This rather low rate of further increase in intensity for these participants may have also affected the impact of the intervention during the subsequent training period. Overall, our approach for increasing the intensity might not have been sufficient to induce the same significant improvements observed for the initial 3-week training interval (T1 to TS7) also for the consecutive training intervals of equal length (TS7 to TS14, T14 to T2). Rather, it seemed that the extension of the training duration (TS7 to T2 = 6 weeks) might have played a more important role to further improve participants’ performance beyond that reached after the early intervention phase. This finding is also consistent with the idea of the general dose-response principle for exercise training [38], indicating that improvements in some health-related variables may be more related to the volume and amount of exercise than to its intensity.
Over the last two thirds of the intervention period (TS7 to T2) the largest effects were observed for the highest PTMT levels. This might be related to their higher complexity and to the greater sensitivity to detect changes that has been previously reported for these Physiomat® tasks [35]. For the less complex Physiomat® tasks, potential ceiling effects for those participants who completed the higher PTMT levels already at baseline and possibly reached their maximum performance level on the less complex Physiomat® tasks already after the initial training period (T1 to TS7) may have affected the impact of the extended training duration. In the highest PTMT levels, however, there may still have been a higher potential for these high-performing participants to further improve during the subsequent training period, even after initial significant training response, due to the higher complexity of these tasks.

Predictors of early training response

Previous studies in older people with cognitive impairment or PwD identified low baseline performance in primary outcomes (i.e., balance, maximal strength, and/or motor-functional performance) to be independently predictive for positive training response to physical exercise or exergame interventions [28, 52, 70]. Supporting our hypothesis, based on these previous findings, the regression analysis of our study revealed similar results for exergame-based motor-cognitive performances, such that participants with lower initial performance in these outcomes were those who experienced the greatest training gains over the first 3 weeks of the intervention period. The inverse relationship between the magnitude of benefits and the baseline status for an initial training phase was also described in the general dose-response principles for exercise training [38, 39], indicating that those individuals with the lowest performance benefit the most and the fastest from an exercise intervention.
Some studies have found that lower global cognitive functioning negatively influence training response to physical exercise interventions [7175], whereas other studies have not [28, 52, 70, 76, 77]. Interestingly, and in contrast to all these studies, more severe global cognitive impairment was initially associated with early training response in the univariate analysis of our study. However, the multivariate analysis suggested that this association might actually be explained by the lower performance in domain-specific cognitive functions, whose effects on trainability have rarely been examined in previous studies [52, 76], rather than by the level of global cognitive functioning, which was not independently associated with early training response in our regression model. In particular, the multivariate analysis revealed that lower visuospatial ability and divided attention were independent predictors of early training response. It is conspicuous that lower performance in these cognitive subdomains was identified to be predictive for early training response as spatial orientation and divided attention were stated to be required and trained on the Physiomat® [35]. Because of their greater deficits in these task-related cognitive functions, ERs may have had more room to improve during the early training period than NERs.
In the univariate analysis, lower performance in speed of information processing as assessed by the TMT-NAI was initially associated with early training response; however, this association was lost in the multivariate analysis, maybe because it was captured by other model covariates, especially by the baseline performance on the Physiomat®, whose game tasks were based also on the TMT-NAI.
Results confirm that not only an excellent training response can be achieved despite cognitive impairment, but also that those participants with greater deficits in training-related, domain-specific cognitive functions of visuospatial ability and divided attention, which both have been associated with fall risk [10, 78], benefit the most and the fastest from the exergame intervention.

Limitations and future research

The present study has some limitations. First, the exergame performance of the control group was only assessed before and at the end of the intervention period. Therefore, the time course of the effects of the exergame intervention in the IG could not be compared to those of the non-specific, motor placebo activity. However, in our previous RCT, we have already demonstrated that improvements in the IG over the entire study period are specifically related to the exergame intervention, with no significant improvements in the control group for exergame-related performances [35]. Second, the increasing difficulty of the Physiomat® tasks during the test sessions led to gradually decreasing sample sizes across the tasks. The statistical analyses of the more complex tasks may therefore be limited by a small sample size. By using increasing difficulty levels for assessment, however, it was ensured that each participant could have been adequately challenged to assess his/her individual maximum performance level ('testing the limits') and to prevent ceiling and floor effects at all test sessions. Third, our results are restricted to patients with mild-to-moderate dementia and so cannot be generalized to those with more severe dementia. Fourth, the small sample size may have affected the results of the regression analysis and limited the precious of our conclusions. Fifth, according to the preplanned study design, potential transfer effects of the exergame intervention (e.g. on balance, cognitive functioning) were not analyzed in this study, representing potential targets for future research.

Conclusions

The present study reveals for the first time that substantial task-specific improvements in complex motor-cognitive performances can be achieved after a surprisingly short exergame-based training program in patients with mild-to-moderate dementia. According to general dose-response relationships for physical exercise, our findings demonstrate that the rate of improvements induced by the exergame intervention decreases as training progresses and that with increasing performance level of individuals a longer training period is required to achieve further improvements. Current findings also highlight the trainability and rehabilitation potential of PwD, especially for more vulnerable patients with low initial performance and more severe impairments in training-related cognitive functions who may even reap the most and fastest benefit from motor-cognitive exergame interventions.

Acknowledgements

We thank Michaela Günther-Lange (Agaplesion Bethanien Hospital Heidelberg, Geriatric Center at the Heidelberg University) for her assistance in training and supervision of participants. We also thank the volunteers for their willingness to participate in the study.

Funding

The study was supported by the Dietmar Hopp Foundation, the Robert Bosch Foundation, and the Network of Aging Research (NAR) at the Heidelberg University. The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
The RCT was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the Heidelberg University on May 7, 2012 (S-360/2010) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants (or legal representatives) prior to study inclusion.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review Brain. 1999;122:383–404.PubMed Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review Brain. 1999;122:383–404.PubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley AD, Baddeley HA, Bucks RS, Wilcock GK. Attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2001;124:1492–508.PubMedCrossRef Baddeley AD, Baddeley HA, Bucks RS, Wilcock GK. Attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2001;124:1492–508.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR. The nature and staging of attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(3):252–71.PubMedCrossRef Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR. The nature and staging of attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(3):252–71.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hauer K, Marburger C, Oster P. Motor performance deteriorates with simultaneously performed cognitive tasks in geriatric patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2002;83(2):217–23.PubMedCrossRef Hauer K, Marburger C, Oster P. Motor performance deteriorates with simultaneously performed cognitive tasks in geriatric patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2002;83(2):217–23.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, Borrie M, Gopaul K, Montero-Odasso M. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait Posture. 2012;35(1):96–100.PubMedCrossRef Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, Borrie M, Gopaul K, Montero-Odasso M. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait Posture. 2012;35(1):96–100.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hauer K, Pfisterer M, Weber C, Wezler N, Kliegel M, Oster P. Cognitive impairment decreases postural control during dual tasks in geriatric patients with a history of severe falls. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(11):1638–44.PubMedCrossRef Hauer K, Pfisterer M, Weber C, Wezler N, Kliegel M, Oster P. Cognitive impairment decreases postural control during dual tasks in geriatric patients with a history of severe falls. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(11):1638–44.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Manckoundia P, Pfitzenmeyer P, d’Athis P, Dubost V, Mourey F. Impact of cognitive task on the posture of elderly subjects with Alzheimer’s disease compared to healthy elderly subjects. Mov Disord. 2006;21(2):236–41.CrossRef Manckoundia P, Pfitzenmeyer P, d’Athis P, Dubost V, Mourey F. Impact of cognitive task on the posture of elderly subjects with Alzheimer’s disease compared to healthy elderly subjects. Mov Disord. 2006;21(2):236–41.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Lundin-Olsson L, Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. Attention, frailty, and falls: the effect of a manual task on basic mobility. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1998;46(6):758–61.PubMedCrossRef Lundin-Olsson L, Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. Attention, frailty, and falls: the effect of a manual task on basic mobility. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1998;46(6):758–61.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Faulkner KA, Redfern MS, Rosano C, Landsittel DP, Studenski SA, Cauley JA, et al. Reciprocal influence of concurrent walking and cognitive testing on performance in older adults. Gait Posture. 2006;24(2):182–9.PubMedCrossRef Faulkner KA, Redfern MS, Rosano C, Landsittel DP, Studenski SA, Cauley JA, et al. Reciprocal influence of concurrent walking and cognitive testing on performance in older adults. Gait Posture. 2006;24(2):182–9.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Muir-Hunter SW, Wittwer JE. Dual-task testing to predict falls in community-dwelling older adults: a systematic review. Physiotherapy. 2016;102(1):29–40.PubMedCrossRef Muir-Hunter SW, Wittwer JE. Dual-task testing to predict falls in community-dwelling older adults: a systematic review. Physiotherapy. 2016;102(1):29–40.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Dubost V, Allali G, Kressig RW, Bridenbaugh S, et al. Stops walking when talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur J Neurol. 2009;16(7):786–95.PubMedCrossRef Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Dubost V, Allali G, Kressig RW, Bridenbaugh S, et al. Stops walking when talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur J Neurol. 2009;16(7):786–95.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk M, Lauenroth A, Oster P, Hauer K. Effektivität von körperlichem Training zur Verbesserung motorischer Leistungen bei Patienten mit demenzieller Erkrankung. In: Braumann K-M, Stiller N, editors. Bewegungstherapie bei internistischen Erkrankungen. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2010. p. 167–84.CrossRef Schwenk M, Lauenroth A, Oster P, Hauer K. Effektivität von körperlichem Training zur Verbesserung motorischer Leistungen bei Patienten mit demenzieller Erkrankung. In: Braumann K-M, Stiller N, editors. Bewegungstherapie bei internistischen Erkrankungen. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2010. p. 167–84.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Interv Aging. 2017;12:557–77.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Interv Aging. 2017;12:557–77.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Montero-Odasso M, Speechley M. Falls in cognitively impaired older adults: implications for risk assessment and prevention. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(2):367–75.PubMedCrossRef Montero-Odasso M, Speechley M. Falls in cognitively impaired older adults: implications for risk assessment and prevention. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(2):367–75.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Monteiro-Junior RS, Vaghetti CA, Nascimento OJ, Laks J, Deslandes AC. Exergames: Neuroplastic hypothesis about cognitive improvement and biological effects on physical function of institutionalized older persons. Neural Regen Res. 2016;11(2):201–4.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Monteiro-Junior RS, Vaghetti CA, Nascimento OJ, Laks J, Deslandes AC. Exergames: Neuroplastic hypothesis about cognitive improvement and biological effects on physical function of institutionalized older persons. Neural Regen Res. 2016;11(2):201–4.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Tait JL, Duckham RL, Milte CM, Main LC, Daly RM. Influence of sequential vs. simultaneous dual-task exercise training on cognitive function in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:368.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tait JL, Duckham RL, Milte CM, Main LC, Daly RM. Influence of sequential vs. simultaneous dual-task exercise training on cognitive function in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:368.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat van Diest M, Lamoth CJ, Stegenga J, Verkerke GJ, Postema K. Exergaming for balance training of elderly: state of the art and future developments. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2013;10:101.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef van Diest M, Lamoth CJ, Stegenga J, Verkerke GJ, Postema K. Exergaming for balance training of elderly: state of the art and future developments. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2013;10:101.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Molina KI, Ricci NA, de Moraes SA, Perracini MR. Virtual reality using games for improving physical functioning in older adults: a systematic review. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11:156.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Molina KI, Ricci NA, de Moraes SA, Perracini MR. Virtual reality using games for improving physical functioning in older adults: a systematic review. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11:156.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Stanmore E, Stubbs B, Vancampfort D, de Bruin ED, Firth J. The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;78:34–43.PubMedCrossRef Stanmore E, Stubbs B, Vancampfort D, de Bruin ED, Firth J. The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;78:34–43.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Schoene D, Valenzuela T, Lord SR, de Bruin ED. The effect of interactive cognitive-motor training in reducing fall risk in older people: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:107.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schoene D, Valenzuela T, Lord SR, de Bruin ED. The effect of interactive cognitive-motor training in reducing fall risk in older people: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:107.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Skjaeret N, Nawaz A, Morat T, Schoene D, Helbostad JL, Vereijken B. Exercise and rehabilitation delivered through exergames in older adults: an integrative review of technologies, safety and efficacy. Int J Med Inform. 2016;85(1):1–16.PubMedCrossRef Skjaeret N, Nawaz A, Morat T, Schoene D, Helbostad JL, Vereijken B. Exercise and rehabilitation delivered through exergames in older adults: an integrative review of technologies, safety and efficacy. Int J Med Inform. 2016;85(1):1–16.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Laufer Y, Dar G, Kodesh E. Does a Wii-based exercise program enhance balance control of independently functioning older adults? A systematic review. Clin Interv Aging. 2014;9:1803–13.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Laufer Y, Dar G, Kodesh E. Does a Wii-based exercise program enhance balance control of independently functioning older adults? A systematic review. Clin Interv Aging. 2014;9:1803–13.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Chao YY, Scherer YK, Montgomery CA. Effects of using Nintendo Wii exergames in older adults: a review of the literature. J Aging Health. 2015;27(3):379–402.PubMedCrossRef Chao YY, Scherer YK, Montgomery CA. Effects of using Nintendo Wii exergames in older adults: a review of the literature. J Aging Health. 2015;27(3):379–402.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogawa EF, You T, Leveille SG. Potential benefits of exergaming for cognition and dual-task function in older adults: a systematic review. J Aging Phys Act. 2016;24(2):332–6.PubMedCrossRef Ogawa EF, You T, Leveille SG. Potential benefits of exergaming for cognition and dual-task function in older adults: a systematic review. J Aging Phys Act. 2016;24(2):332–6.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor LM, Kerse N, Frakking T, Maddison R. Active video games for improving physical performance measures in older people: a meta-analysis. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2018;41(2):108–23.PubMedCrossRef Taylor LM, Kerse N, Frakking T, Maddison R. Active video games for improving physical performance measures in older people: a meta-analysis. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2018;41(2):108–23.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Li J, Theng YL, Foo S. Effect of exergames on depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2016;19(1):34–42.PubMedCrossRef Li J, Theng YL, Foo S. Effect of exergames on depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2016;19(1):34–42.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Padala KP, Padala PR, Malloy TR, Geske JA, Dubbert PM, Dennis RA, et al. Wii-fit for improving gait and balance in an assisted living facility: a pilot study. J Aging Res. 2012;2012:597573.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Padala KP, Padala PR, Malloy TR, Geske JA, Dubbert PM, Dennis RA, et al. Wii-fit for improving gait and balance in an assisted living facility: a pilot study. J Aging Res. 2012;2012:597573.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk M, Sabbagh M, Lin I, Morgan P, Grewal GS, Mohler J, et al. Sensor-based balance training with motion feedback in people with mild cognitive impairment. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2016;53(6):945–58.PubMedCrossRef Schwenk M, Sabbagh M, Lin I, Morgan P, Grewal GS, Mohler J, et al. Sensor-based balance training with motion feedback in people with mild cognitive impairment. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2016;53(6):945–58.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Padala KP, Padala PR, Lensing SY, Dennis RA, Bopp MM, Roberson PK, et al. Home-based exercise program improves balance and fear of falling in community-dwelling older adults with mild Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59(2):565–74.PubMedCrossRef Padala KP, Padala PR, Lensing SY, Dennis RA, Bopp MM, Roberson PK, et al. Home-based exercise program improves balance and fear of falling in community-dwelling older adults with mild Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59(2):565–74.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Ben-Sadoun G, Sacco G, Manera V, Bourgeois J, Konig A, Foulon P, et al. Physical and cognitive stimulation using an exergame in subjects with normal aging, mild and moderate cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53(4):1299–314.PubMedCrossRef Ben-Sadoun G, Sacco G, Manera V, Bourgeois J, Konig A, Foulon P, et al. Physical and cognitive stimulation using an exergame in subjects with normal aging, mild and moderate cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53(4):1299–314.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Gonzalez-Palau F, Franco M, Bamidis P, Losada R, Parra E, Papageorgiou SG, et al. The effects of a computer-based cognitive and physical training program in a healthy and mildly cognitive impaired aging sample. Aging Ment Health. 2014;18(7):838–46.PubMedCrossRef Gonzalez-Palau F, Franco M, Bamidis P, Losada R, Parra E, Papageorgiou SG, et al. The effects of a computer-based cognitive and physical training program in a healthy and mildly cognitive impaired aging sample. Aging Ment Health. 2014;18(7):838–46.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Yamaguchi H, Maki Y, Takahashi K. Rehabilitation for dementia using enjoyable video-sports games. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011;23(4):674–6.PubMedCrossRef Yamaguchi H, Maki Y, Takahashi K. Rehabilitation for dementia using enjoyable video-sports games. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011;23(4):674–6.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Weybright E, Dattilo J, Rusch F. Effects of an interactive video game (Nintendo Wii) on older women with mild cognitive impairment. Ther Recreat J. 2010;44(4):271. Weybright E, Dattilo J, Rusch F. Effects of an interactive video game (Nintendo Wii) on older women with mild cognitive impairment. Ther Recreat J. 2010;44(4):271.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Legouverneur G, Pino M, Boulay M, Rigaud A-S. Wii sports, a usability study with MCI and Alzheimer’s patients. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(4):S500–S1.CrossRef Legouverneur G, Pino M, Boulay M, Rigaud A-S. Wii sports, a usability study with MCI and Alzheimer’s patients. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(4):S500–S1.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Fenney A, Lee TD. Exploring spared capacity in persons with dementia: what WiiTM can learn. Act Adapt Aging. 2010;34(4):303–13. Fenney A, Lee TD. Exploring spared capacity in persons with dementia: what WiiTM can learn. Act Adapt Aging. 2010;34(4):303–13.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat van Santen J, Droes RM, Holstege M, Henkemans OB, van Rijn A, de Vries R, et al. Effects of exergaming in people with dementia: results of a systematic literature review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;63(2):741–60.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef van Santen J, Droes RM, Holstege M, Henkemans OB, van Rijn A, de Vries R, et al. Effects of exergaming in people with dementia: results of a systematic literature review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;63(2):741–60.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Haskell WL. J.B. Wolffe Memorial Lecture. Health consequences of physical activity: understanding and challenges regarding dose-response. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1994;26(6):649–60.PubMedCrossRef Haskell WL. J.B. Wolffe Memorial Lecture. Health consequences of physical activity: understanding and challenges regarding dose-response. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1994;26(6):649–60.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoffmann J. Physiological aspects of sport training and performance. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2002. Hoffmann J. Physiological aspects of sport training and performance. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2002.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Manera V, Ben-Sadoun G, Aalbers T, Agopyan H, Askenazy F, Benoit M, et al. Recommendations for the use of serious games in neurodegenerative disorders: 2016 Delphi panel. Front Psychol. 2017;8(1243). Manera V, Ben-Sadoun G, Aalbers T, Agopyan H, Askenazy F, Benoit M, et al. Recommendations for the use of serious games in neurodegenerative disorders: 2016 Delphi panel. Front Psychol. 2017;8(1243).
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Manlapaz DG, Sole G, Jayakaran P, Chapple CM. A narrative synthesis of Nintendo Wii fit gaming protocol in addressing balance among healthy older adults: what system works? Games Health J. 2017;6(2):65–74.PubMedCrossRef Manlapaz DG, Sole G, Jayakaran P, Chapple CM. A narrative synthesis of Nintendo Wii fit gaming protocol in addressing balance among healthy older adults: what system works? Games Health J. 2017;6(2):65–74.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Esculier JF, Vaudrin J, Beriault P, Gagnon K, Tremblay LE. Home-based balance training programme using Wii fit with balance board for Parkinsons’s disease: a pilot study. J Rehabil Med. 2012;44(2):144–50.PubMedCrossRef Esculier JF, Vaudrin J, Beriault P, Gagnon K, Tremblay LE. Home-based balance training programme using Wii fit with balance board for Parkinsons’s disease: a pilot study. J Rehabil Med. 2012;44(2):144–50.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat dos Santos Mendes FA, Pompeu JE, Modenesi Lobo A, Guedes da Silva K, Oliveira Tde P, Peterson Zomignani A, et al. Motor learning, retention and transfer after virtual-reality-based training in Parkinson’s disease-effect of motor and cognitive demands of games: a longitudinal, controlled clinical study. Physiotherapy. 2012;98(3):217–23.PubMedCrossRef dos Santos Mendes FA, Pompeu JE, Modenesi Lobo A, Guedes da Silva K, Oliveira Tde P, Peterson Zomignani A, et al. Motor learning, retention and transfer after virtual-reality-based training in Parkinson’s disease-effect of motor and cognitive demands of games: a longitudinal, controlled clinical study. Physiotherapy. 2012;98(3):217–23.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Albores J, Marolda C, Haggerty M, Gerstenhaber B, Zuwallack R. The use of a home exercise program based on a computer system in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. J Cardiopulm Rehabil Prev. 2013;33(1):47–52.PubMedCrossRef Albores J, Marolda C, Haggerty M, Gerstenhaber B, Zuwallack R. The use of a home exercise program based on a computer system in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. J Cardiopulm Rehabil Prev. 2013;33(1):47–52.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenberg D, Depp CA, Vahia IV, Reichstadt J, Palmer BW, Kerr J, et al. Exergames for subsyndromal depression in older adults: a pilot study of a novel intervention. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(3):221–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rosenberg D, Depp CA, Vahia IV, Reichstadt J, Palmer BW, Kerr J, et al. Exergames for subsyndromal depression in older adults: a pilot study of a novel intervention. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(3):221–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams MA, Soiza RL, Jenkinson AM, Stewart A. EXercising with computers in later life (EXCELL) - pilot and feasibility study of the acceptability of the Nintendo® WiiFit in community-dwelling fallers. BMC Res Notes. 2010;3:238.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Williams MA, Soiza RL, Jenkinson AM, Stewart A. EXercising with computers in later life (EXCELL) - pilot and feasibility study of the acceptability of the Nintendo® WiiFit in community-dwelling fallers. BMC Res Notes. 2010;3:238.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Chmelo EA, Crotts CI, Newman JC, Brinkley TE, Lyles MF, Leng X, et al. Heterogeneity of physical function responses to exercise training in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(3):462–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chmelo EA, Crotts CI, Newman JC, Brinkley TE, Lyles MF, Leng X, et al. Heterogeneity of physical function responses to exercise training in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(3):462–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Werner C, Wiloth S, Lemke NC, Kronbach F, Jansen CP, Oster P, et al. People with dementia can learn compensatory movement maneuvers for the sit-to-stand task: a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;60(1):107–20.PubMedCrossRef Werner C, Wiloth S, Lemke NC, Kronbach F, Jansen CP, Oster P, et al. People with dementia can learn compensatory movement maneuvers for the sit-to-stand task: a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;60(1):107–20.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Lemke NC, Werner C, Wiloth S, Oster P, Bauer JM, Hauer K. Transferability and sustainability of motor-cognitive dual-task training in patients with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Gerontology. 2018:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1159/000490852. Lemke NC, Werner C, Wiloth S, Oster P, Bauer JM, Hauer K. Transferability and sustainability of motor-cognitive dual-task training in patients with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Gerontology. 2018:1–16. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1159/​000490852.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.PubMedCrossRef Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris JC, Mohs RC, Rogers H, Fillenbaum G, Heyman A. Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(4):641–52.PubMed Morris JC, Mohs RC, Rogers H, Fillenbaum G, Heyman A. Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(4):641–52.PubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Hauer K, Schwenk M, Zieschang T, Essig M, Becker C, Oster P. Physical training improves motor performance in people with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(1):8–15.PubMedCrossRef Hauer K, Schwenk M, Zieschang T, Essig M, Becker C, Oster P. Physical training improves motor performance in people with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(1):8–15.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk M, Oster P, Hauer K. Kraft- und Funktionstraining bei älteren Menschen mit demetieller Erkrankung. Praxis Physiotherapie. 2008;2:59–65. Schwenk M, Oster P, Hauer K. Kraft- und Funktionstraining bei älteren Menschen mit demetieller Erkrankung. Praxis Physiotherapie. 2008;2:59–65.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Oswald WD, Fleischmann UM. Das Nürnberger-alters-Inventar (NAI) - Testinventar & NAI-Testmanual und Textband. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1999. Oswald WD, Fleischmann UM. Das Nürnberger-alters-Inventar (NAI) - Testinventar & NAI-Testmanual und Textband. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1999.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiloth S, Lemke N, Werner C, Hauer K. Validation of a computerized, game-based assessment strategy to masure training effects on motor-cognitive functions in people with dementia. JMIR Serious Games. 2016;4(2):e12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wiloth S, Lemke N, Werner C, Hauer K. Validation of a computerized, game-based assessment strategy to masure training effects on motor-cognitive functions in people with dementia. JMIR Serious Games. 2016;4(2):e12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical gerontology: a guide to assessment and intervention. New York, NY: the Haworth press; 1986. p. 165–73. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical gerontology: a guide to assessment and intervention. New York, NY: the Haworth press; 1986. p. 165–73.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Hauer KA, Kempen GI, Schwenk M, Yardley L, Beyer N, Todd C, et al. Validity and sensitivity to change of the falls efficacy scales international to assess fear of falling in older adults with and without cognitive impairment. Gerontology. 2011;57(5):462–72.PubMed Hauer KA, Kempen GI, Schwenk M, Yardley L, Beyer N, Todd C, et al. Validity and sensitivity to change of the falls efficacy scales international to assess fear of falling in older adults with and without cognitive impairment. Gerontology. 2011;57(5):462–72.PubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Tinetti ME. Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1986;34(2):119–26.CrossRef Tinetti ME. Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1986;34(2):119–26.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “up & go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39(2):142–8.PubMedCrossRef Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “up & go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39(2):142–8.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Glynn RJ, Berkman LF, Blazer DG, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol. 1994;49(2):M85–94.CrossRef Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Glynn RJ, Berkman LF, Blazer DG, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol. 1994;49(2):M85–94.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandler MJ, Lacritz LH, Hynan LS, Barnard HD, Allen G, Deschner M, et al. A total score for the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Neurology. 2005;65(1):102–6.PubMedCrossRef Chandler MJ, Lacritz LH, Hynan LS, Barnard HD, Allen G, Deschner M, et al. A total score for the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Neurology. 2005;65(1):102–6.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Abernethy B. Dual-task methodology and motor skills research: some applications and methodological constraints. J Hum Mov Stud. 1988;14:101–32. Abernethy B. Dual-task methodology and motor skills research: some applications and methodological constraints. J Hum Mov Stud. 1988;14:101–32.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Lemke NC, Wiloth S, Werner C, Hauer K. Validity, test-retest reliability, sensitivity to change and feasibility of motor-cognitive dual task assessments in patients with dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2017;70:169–79.PubMedCrossRef Lemke NC, Wiloth S, Werner C, Hauer K. Validity, test-retest reliability, sensitivity to change and feasibility of motor-cognitive dual task assessments in patients with dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2017;70:169–79.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge; 1988. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge; 1988.
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenthal JA. Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. J Soc Serv Res. 1996;21(4):37–59.CrossRef Rosenthal JA. Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. J Soc Serv Res. 1996;21(4):37–59.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991;59(1):12–9.CrossRef Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991;59(1):12–9.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Iverson GL. Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;16(2):183–91.CrossRef Iverson GL. Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;16(2):183–91.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Ehrensperger MM, Berres M, Taylor KI, Monsch AU. Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease with a total score of the German CERAD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(5):910–20.PubMedCrossRef Ehrensperger MM, Berres M, Taylor KI, Monsch AU. Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease with a total score of the German CERAD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(5):910–20.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat de Boer C, Echlin HV, Rogojin A, Baltaretu BR, Sergio LE. Thinking-while-moving exercises may improve cognition in elderly with mild cognitive deficits: a proof-of-principle study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2018:248–58. de Boer C, Echlin HV, Rogojin A, Baltaretu BR, Sergio LE. Thinking-while-moving exercises may improve cognition in elderly with mild cognitive deficits: a proof-of-principle study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2018:248–58.
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk M, Dutzi I, Englert S, Micol W, Najafi B, Mohler J, et al. An intensive exercise program improves motor performances in patients with dementia: translational model of geriatric rehabilitation. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;39(3):487–98.PubMedCrossRef Schwenk M, Dutzi I, Englert S, Micol W, Najafi B, Mohler J, et al. An intensive exercise program improves motor performances in patients with dementia: translational model of geriatric rehabilitation. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;39(3):487–98.PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Uemura K, Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, et al. Cognitive function affects trainability for physical performance in exercise intervention among older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:97–102.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Uemura K, Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, et al. Cognitive function affects trainability for physical performance in exercise intervention among older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:97–102.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghisla MK, Cossi S, Timpini A, Baroni F, Facchi E, Marengoni A. Predictors of successful rehabilitation in geriatric patients: subgroup analysis of patients with cognitive impairment. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2007;19(5):417–23.PubMedCrossRef Ghisla MK, Cossi S, Timpini A, Baroni F, Facchi E, Marengoni A. Predictors of successful rehabilitation in geriatric patients: subgroup analysis of patients with cognitive impairment. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2007;19(5):417–23.PubMedCrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Rösler A, Krause T, Niehuus C, von Renteln-Kruse W. Dementia as a cofactor for geriatric rehabilitation-outcome in patients with osteosynthesis of the proximal femur: a retrospective, matched-pair analysis of 250 patients. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2009;49(1):e36–9.PubMedCrossRef Rösler A, Krause T, Niehuus C, von Renteln-Kruse W. Dementia as a cofactor for geriatric rehabilitation-outcome in patients with osteosynthesis of the proximal femur: a retrospective, matched-pair analysis of 250 patients. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2009;49(1):e36–9.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Morghen S, Gentile S, Ricci E, Guerini F, Bellelli G, Trabucchi M. Rehabilitation of older adults with hip fracture: cognitive function and walking abilities. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59(8):1497–502.PubMedCrossRef Morghen S, Gentile S, Ricci E, Guerini F, Bellelli G, Trabucchi M. Rehabilitation of older adults with hip fracture: cognitive function and walking abilities. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59(8):1497–502.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Hershkovitz A, Kalandariov Z, Hermush V, Weiss R, Brill S. Factors affecting short-term rehabilitation outcomes of disabled elderly patients with proximal hip fracture. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(7):916–21.PubMedCrossRef Hershkovitz A, Kalandariov Z, Hermush V, Weiss R, Brill S. Factors affecting short-term rehabilitation outcomes of disabled elderly patients with proximal hip fracture. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(7):916–21.PubMedCrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenk M, Zieschang T, Englert S, Grewal G, Najafi B, Hauer K. Improvements in gait characteristics after intensive resistance and functional training in people with dementia: a randomised controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schwenk M, Zieschang T, Englert S, Grewal G, Najafi B, Hauer K. Improvements in gait characteristics after intensive resistance and functional training in people with dementia: a randomised controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Beloosesky Y, Grinblat J, Epelboym B, Weiss A, Grosman B, Hendel D. Functional gain of hip fracture patients in different cognitive and functional groups. Clin Rehabil. 2002;16(3):321–8.PubMedCrossRef Beloosesky Y, Grinblat J, Epelboym B, Weiss A, Grosman B, Hendel D. Functional gain of hip fracture patients in different cognitive and functional groups. Clin Rehabil. 2002;16(3):321–8.PubMedCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Naslund J. Visuospatial ability in relation to fall risk and dementia. Arch Neurol 2010;67(5):643; author reply −4.PubMedCrossRef Naslund J. Visuospatial ability in relation to fall risk and dementia. Arch Neurol 2010;67(5):643; author reply −4.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Time course of changes in motor-cognitive exergame performances during task-specific training in patients with dementia: identification and predictors of early training response
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Erschienen in
Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1743-0003
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-018-0433-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe