Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research article

Trajectories and the influencing factors of behavior problems in preschool children: a longitudinal study in Guangzhou, China

verfasst von: Peng Bao, Jin Jing, Yu Jin, Xumin Hu, Buyun Liu, Min Hu

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Since child mental health problem was a global health issue, many researchers in western countries has focused on the trajectory of it to provide evidence for prevention programs. We designed this study to determine the trajectories of children’s behavior problems, and to explore the effect of parent predictors on children’s behavior problems in Guangzhou, China.

Methods

Children (N = 1480) for this longitudinal, population-based survey, were recruited from eight regular kindergartens (October, 2010) across four districts in Guangzhou. Repeated measurement design analysis was used to compare the variation in behavioral problems by gender, only child status, and temperament. Logistic regression was applied to analyze the effect of parents’ risks (maternal depression, parenting style) on the change in child problem behaviors.

Results

The scores of behavior problems (externalizing, emotional, social communication problems) were stable during the entire preschool period by gender and child number. Children with difficult temperament exhibited more problem behaviors than children with easy temperament in the early years, and the misbehaviors declined significantly over time. Moreover, maternal depression and the increase in excessive interference/over protective or punishing parenting strategies resulted in an increase in child behavior problems.

Conclusion

There was no difference between the only-child status and child with siblings in the trajectory of problem behaviors. Parent factors were significant predictions of trajectory of child behavior problem during preschool age.
Abkürzungen
BSQ
Behavioral Style Questionnaire
CES-D
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression
EMBU
Egma Minnenav Bardndosna Uppforstran
RMANOVA
Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance
SDQ
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
SPSS
Statistical Package for the Social Science

Background

The preschool period is important for brain and self-regulation skill in children. Mental health symptoms, beginning in the preschool period, frequently impede the completion of adolescent and adult life tasks [1]. The sequelae include friendship and learning difficulties, school dropout, substance abuse, family violence, and suicide [2]. Mental health problems primarily consist of externalizing (aggression, oppositional defiance, attention deficit, and hyperactive disorder) and internalizing (anxiety and depression) behavior problems [3], and affect proximately 20 % of children in modern societies [46]. Moreover, the preschool period is a critical period for the development of social skills, especially the theory of mind. The child social communication problems, such as peer relationships, popularity and pro-social behaviors can be recognized in this period. They may result from externalizing or internalizing problems, or might have been originally present. In the recent years, social communication problems have gradually become the third most common behavior problem [7].
Over the past decade, substantial researches in the western countries have pointed to the prevalence and trajectory of behavior problems in preschool and school age children. In general, the decline in externalizing behavior problems and the relatively stable rate in internalizing behavior problems were in common during the preschool age, either in community or population-based studies [8, 9]. However, the results showed significant diversity in the different social-cultural environment. A comparative study of behavioral problems between China and Germany showed that Chinese children had more emotional and behavioral problems than German children, especially internalizing problems among girls [10]. The research in Shenzhen, China has shown a lower prevalence in emotional problems in 6-year old than in 3-year old children [11], while another investigation in Guiyang indicated a higher rate of behavior problems in 6-year old as opposed to 3–5 year old children [12]. However, the studies in China were all cross sectional ones. As far as we know, no longitudinal study has been conducted in China to report the trajectory of children’s behavior problems.
Another special social factor that may influence the behavior and developmental patterns of children in China is the ‘one child’ policy. There are over 80 % children who are the only child in their families in China. These children have much less social interaction with peers than children have siblings, which cannot be replaced by parents [13]. Earlier studies from other countries indicated that the only children are often overprotected and self-centered, which may have a negative effect on their psychological development [1416], and especially on the externalizing problems [17]. However, in Bayer’s study, the absence of older siblings was a key predictor of internalizing problems [18]. Research in Japan has also revealed that children with a larger number of older brothers/sisters had a smaller incidence of problematic behaviors [19]. On the other hand, the cross-sectional research in China has found that the children with siblings had worse mental health based on the resource dilution theory which asserts that family resources are divided by the number of children [20]. Although there were many studies focused on the only children, there is no population-based literature that dealt with the trajectory of their behavior problems.
According to Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory, experiences within the family, which is the main social contextual system for the preschool period, influence the developmental trajectories of children’s behavioral orientations [21, 22]. Accumulated evidence indicates a number of risks associated with children’s externalizing and internalizing difficulties. Generally, child risks include physical health problems and difficult temperament [1, 8, 23, 24], while family risks include time-invariant factors, such as young and poorly educated parents, divorced family, low-income, and time-varying factors, such as maternal depression, controlling parents, lack of warmth/sensitivity, overprotective parenting interactions and harsh discipline. Although these risk factors influence the occurrence or continuity of children’s behavior problems, the trajectories of parents’ factors, especially maternal depression and parenting styles were most direct and important ones [2427]. A deep understanding of parents’ factors effect on trajectory of child problem behaviors, which can support the intervention project, is still required in China.
Therefore, the main purpose of this study was to investigate the trajectories of preschool child behavior problems, especially those of only children, based on a population sample from Guangzhou, China. An additional goal was to explore how maternal depression and parenting strategies predict trajectory of child behavior problems in this sample.

Methods

Sampling methods and study population

This study was a prospective investigation with a stratified cluster sampling method. The data were collected in three waves (Oct, 2010, Nov, 2011, and Jun, 2013). The vast majority of children (3–6 years) attended a kindergarten near their home in China. The families were recruited at eight kindergartens in two of eight Guangzhou districts with a medium economic level. 2258 parents of 3–4-year-old children (healthy babies at birth, not adopted) were enrolled in the study and 2048 participants were willing to participate in the research. In wave 2 investigation, 1747 participants completely finished the questionnaires, while 1480 (87.4 %) participants were retained for the final investigation in 2013. The mean age of the children was 3.65 ± 0.59 years at the time of recruitment.
The reasons for participants’ exclusion in wave 2 and 3 were moving from the area, the inability to contact them after the initial investigations, loss of interest in the study, other duties, poor physical condition of the child, or missing over 5 % of independent variables in the questionnaire. The socio-demographic characteristics and the mean score of behavior problems in wave 1 for the missing people did not significantly differ from those for the final participants. This implied that the participants included in the analyses were broadly representative of those originally recruited.

The questionnaires

Socio-demographic characteristics

Based on the research of the interaction between the socioeconomic context and human development [28], we designed a questionnaire based on the socioeconomic and family background. The questionnaire included four questions including the parental childbearing age, education level, the family’s monthly income per capita and the family structure. The parental education level was divided between less than or over 12 years, which presented that whether they finished formal education (secondary school). The family’s monthly income per capita was classified into less than or over 5000 China Yuan (CNY), which corresponds to the average income level of Guangzhou families. The family structure was categorized as the nuclear family, extended family and single-parent family.

Behavior problems

The instrument used to assess the child behavior problems was a parent-report version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). The questionnaire included 25 items on three-point Likert scales, which is widely used in screening behavior problems. This method has demonstrated acceptable levels of reliability and validity in Chinese children [29]. The SDQ items were divided into five subscales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention problems, peer relationship problems, and pro-social behavior, with five items in each domain. The first four of these subscales could be combined to a total difficulty score [30]. In this study, we also combined conduct problems and hyperactivity problems, as externalizing problems, and peer-relationship problems and pro-social behavior problem (reverse scoring), as social interaction problems. The repeated reliability (the test-retest reliability) of each domain in this study was between 0.45–0.80.

Child temperament

The Behavioral Style Questionnaire (BSQ) included 100 items, with a six-level scoring system [31]. The items presented nine dimensions of child temperament: rhythmicity, activity level, approach/withdrawal, adaptability, reaction intensity, mood quality, persistence, distractibility, and sensory threshold. The items were averaged to specify a global measure of child temperament as easy, difficult, or slow to warm-up. The slow to warm-up and difficult temperament types both have similar evaluation standards, e.g. both of them are characterized by low rhythmicity, withdrawal in a new environment, poor adaptability, and negative emotions [31]. Moreover, lots of studies in China indicate that children with difficult and slow to warm-up temperaments exhibited no significant difference in behavior problems, and that both had more behavioral problems than the easy temperament children [32, 33]. Therefore, we combined the slow to warm-up into difficult temperament children in our analyses. The repeated reliability of each domain in this study was between 0.57 and 0.85.

Maternal depression

Maternal depression was measured by the standardized Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Inventory (CES-D) [34]. This is a 20-item, self-report checklist that assesses the adult depressive symptoms. The repeated reliability in this study was 0.74.

Parenting style

We used the Egma Minnenav Bardndosna Uppforstran (EMBU), revised in Chinese, to assess parenting strategies and behaviors [31]. We excluded the subscale of partiality to one child more than the others, and chose the remaining four subscales (emotional warmth/understanding, excessive interference/protection, rejection/denial, and harsh/punishment) to evaluate the parenting strategies. The questions were changed from a child-report to a parent-report, with acceptable validity and reliability [35].

Data collection

The children in this study were all a part of large kindergarten classes, with teacher to children ratios below 1:10. Due to the large class sizes, we assumed that the teacher reports of child behavior may be less accurate than their primary caretaker reports. Therefore, we chose the primary taker to be the main respondent for completing the BSQ, SDQ, EMBU, and socio-demographic questionnaires. The primary caretaker was defined as the person with the greatest responsibility for the daily care of the child in the family [26]. In 95 % of cases, the children’s primary care taker was the mother, while in 5 % cases it was either the father or the grandparents.
Wave 1 survey was conducted after receiving written informed consents signed by every child’s parents. BSQ, SDQ, EMBU, and socio-demographic questionnaires were completed in period 1. SDQ and CES-D were completed in wave 2. In the last wave, SDQ, CES-D, and EMBU, questionnaires were completed. The repeated reliability of the questionnaires was tested 4 weeks after the completion of period 1 by 202 parents (10 % of the sample). In wave 1 survey, the questionnaires were sent out and collected in kindergartens by the teacher in charge, with two investigators in each of the participating classes present during parent meetings. In the last two surveys, the questionnaires were distributed among the parents when they picked up their children from kindergarten and were returned 1 week later. The drop-out families were interviewed by telephone or email to inquire about the reason why they discontinued in the study.

Statistical analysis

The data was uploaded in Epidata 3.1 software, and the analyses were conducted using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 22.0. To examine the trajectory of problem behaviors in children, the Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (RMANOVA) was conducted based on gender, child number (only child or child with siblings), and child temperament (easy or passive). We chose to model a continuous (rather than dichotomized) measure of behavior problems, since a dimensional approach better characterizes the processes across the entire range of functioning in the population and since subclinical difficulties are also associated with impaired functioning and development of clinical disorders [4].
In order to analyze the effect of parent factors (parenting style, and maternal depression) on behavior problems, logistic models were used. Firstly, logistic regressions for each dependent variable, including maternal depression and parenting strategies, were carried out. Variables showing significant contribution to changes of child behavior problem (Wave 1 to Wave 3) were entered into a backward step-wise logistic regression, and the demographic characteristics were included as controlling factors in the adjusted model [36].
To account for missing values in the independent variables in our analyses, maximum likelihood estimating method was employed to reduce the bias in the parameter estimates.

Results

Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables

Sociodemographic characteristics

As presented in Table 1, sociodemographic characteristics were stable in all three waves. The final sample consisted of: 51.7 % boys, 82.3 % of children the only child, approximately 75 % highly educated parents, and 57.8 % nuclear families. 45.2 % families had lower income than average (RMB 5000).
Table 1
Sociodemographic characteristics in every period
Characteristics
Wave 1 n(%) n t = 2048
Wave 2 n(%) n t = 1747
Wave 3 n(%) n t = 1480
 Child
  
 Male
1057(51.6)
891(51.0)
765(51.7)
 Only child
1560(82.5)
1431(81.9)
1218(82.3)
Parents
  
 Father’s age (mean ± SD, range)
33.14 ± 4.14,21-54
34.09 ± 4.17,23-54
35.24 ± 4.19,24-54
 Mother’s age(mean ± SD, range)
29.89 ± 3.68,20-52
30.98 ± 3.85,21-50
32.10 ± 3.74,21-49
 Father’s education ≥ 12y
1568(77.9)
1378(78.9)
1162(78.5)
 Mother’s education ≥ 12y
1506(74.8)
1310(75.0)
1095(74.0)
Family
   
 Nuclear family
1154(56.9)
984(56.3)
855(57.8)
 Income per capita < 5000 CNY
960(46.9)
847(48.5)
669(45.2)

Child behavior problems, parenting styles, and maternal depression in three waves

The general trajectory of externalizing, emotional, social communication problems, and the total difficulty were stable during the entire preschool period (Table 2).
Table 2
Scores of SDQ, EMBU and CES-D in the three waves (Mean ± SD)
 
Wave 1
Wave 2
Wave 3
Child Behavior (SDQ)
   
Emotional symptoms
1.97 ± 1.62
1.89 ± 1.72
1.95±1.65
Externalizing problems
   
 Conduct
1.89 ± 1.36
1.71 ± 1.31
1.73 ± 1.41
 Hyperactivity
4.10 ± 2.10
4.18 ± 2.16
4.08 ± 2.11
Social communication
   
 Peer relationship
2.30 ± 1.56
2.11 ± 1.57
2.29 ± 1.55
 Prosocial behavior
6.94 ± 1.92
7.21 ± 1.94
7.06 ± 1.94
Total difficulty
10.26 ± 4.53
9.88 ± 4.58
10.07 ± 4.61
Parenting Styles (EMBU)
   
 Emotional warmth
52.04 ± 8.98
n/a
54.67 ± 10.60
 Excessive interference
34.68 ± 6.64
n/a
35.02 ± 6.87
 Rejection
13.26 ± 3.46
n/a
11.97 ± 2.81
 Punishment
11.45 ± 1.98
n/a
10.83 ± 1.79
Maternal Depression (CES-D)
n/a
18.34 ± 2.58
18.43 ± 2.64

Trajectory of child problem behaviors

RMANOVA analysis of children’s problem behaviors were preceded by gender, temperament, and only child status.

Trajectory difference by gender

The age slopes for boys and girls were examined and compared in the unconditional model (Fig. 1). The data was fitted according to the normal distribution, in the Mauchly sphericity test, P <0.01, with Greenhouse Geisser method chosen for the adjustment. No significant difference was observed in total difficulty, during 3 years (main effect of age: F = 0.929, p = 0.388). The differences between boys and girls were not significant either (main effect of gender: F = 1.650, p = 0.199). Therefore, it is important to emphasize the lack of significant difference between both boys and girls in trajectory of total difficulty. For emotional and externalizing problems, no significant difference was observed during the 3 waves in boys or girls (main effect of age—emotional: F = 0.039, p = 0.955, externalizing: F = 0.686, p = 0.482; main effect of gender—emotional: F = 3.188, p = 0.075, externalizing: F = 2.556, p = 0.110; interaction effect of age and gender—emotional: F = 0.020, p = 0.976, externalizing: F = 3.016, p = 0.058). In addition, child social problem behaviors exhibited a declining trajectory in this period (main effect of age: F = 3.398, p = 0.037), while in boys it declined more than in girls (main effect of gender: F = 12.801, p < 0.001). There was no significant difference between the interactive effect between age and gender (F = 0.073, p = 0.919).

Trajectory difference by child number

The data was fitted according to normal distribution. The Mauchly sphericity test showed P <0.01, while the Greenhouse Geisser method was used for the adjustment. The data indicated no significant difference between the only or non-only child in total difficulty, emotional, externalizing and social communication problems during these 3 years (main effect of only child: F = 0.888, p = 0.347; F = 1.481, p = 0.224; F = 0.341, p = 0.559; F = 0.297, p = 0.586; interaction effect between age and number of children: F = 0.577, p = 0.544; F = 0.535, p = 0.575, F = 0.033, p = 0.949, F = 0.055, p = 0.936) (Fig. 2).

Trajectory difference by child temperament

The data was fitted according to normal distribution and spherical symmetry. Children with easy or difficult temperament presented significantly different trajectories for each of the problem behaviors (main effects of child temperament: total difficulty: F = 36.750, p = 0.000; emotional: F = 34.600, p = 0.000; externalizing: F = 16.142, p = 0.000; social communication: F = 28.995, p = 0.000). Difficult temperament in children (3–4 years old) in wave 1 predicted more problem behaviors than easy temperament. Children with easy temperament maintained almost the same score of problem behaviors from preschool period until 6–7 years (wave 3). However, problem behaviors of children with difficult temperament declined substantially and no significant difference was observed in comparison to children with easy temperament in 6–7 years old. Moreover, problem behaviors declined more sharply in the early (W 1 to W 2) compared to the late preschool period (W 2 to W 3) (main effects of time: F = 13.669, p = 0.000; F = 5.593, p = 0.004; F = 8.699, p = 0.000; F = 9.129, p = 0.000; interaction effect of age and child temperament: F = 14.344, p = 0.000; F = 7.841, p = 0.000; F = 11.775, p = 0.000; F = 8.259, p = 0.000, respectively) (Fig. 3).

Factors of parents predicted the change of child behavior problem

In unadjusted analysis (Table 3), most factors, excluded maternal depression change from wave 1 to wave 3 and rejection parenting, were predictors of change of child behavior problems. Although emotional warmth and punishment parenting were not significant predictors, they were included in the adjusted model.
Table 3
Prediction of change of child behavior problem from wave 1 to wave 3
Potential Predictors
Unadjusted Resultsa
Adjusted Resultsa
OR (95 % CI)
P
OR (95 % CI)
P
Child behavior problem (SDQ) in Wave 1
0.745(0.703–0.790)
<0.001
0.709(0.661–0.760)
<0.001
Maternal Depression (CES-D)
    
 Wave 1
1.087(1.015–1.165)
0.017
1.133(1.036–1.239)
0.005
 Change from wave 1 to wave 3
0.955(0.903–1.011)
0.111
-
-
Change of Parenting strategies from wave 2 to wave 3 (EMBU)
    
 Emotional warmth
0.983(0.966–1.000)
0.054
0.976(0.954–0.998)
0.030
 Excessive interference
1.019(0.998–1.041)
0.075
1.024(0.998–1.052)
0.069
 Rejection
0.991(0.948–1.036)
0.697
-
-
 Punishment
1.071(0.992–1.155)
0.078
1.124(1.022–1.237)
0.016
a Sample size was 1480
In the multilevel analysis, the model explained 41 % of variations. Child behavior problems in wave 1 and increase of emotional warmth/understanding parenting were factors that related to decrease of child behavior problems (OR < 1). Meanwhile, maternal depression in wave 1 and increase of punishment predicted the increase of child behavior problems significantly (OR > 1). Excessive interferencing parenting was also in the adjusted model of effect on increase of child behavior problems although not significantly (p = 0.069).

Discussion

The trajectories of child problem behaviors

According to care taker’s report, children’s externalizing, internalizing, and social communication problems were stabilized during the preschool period. The results were consistent with those obtained from cross-sectional research with similar age groups in Guangzhou or the surrounding cities [3739].
Firstly, we did not find gender differences in externalizing (Fig. 1b), emotional (Fig. 1c) or social communication problems (Fig. 1d) in this population sample. Some studies from other countries reported a steep decline in externalizing problems during the preschool period, with boys exhibiting more aggressive behavior than girls [9, 40]. The others concluded that the children were already at their peak levels of disruptive behavior when they enrolled in school [41]. While searching the studies in China, we ran into similar results that demonstrated invariance of externalizing problems in both boys and girls [38, 42]. We assumed that the difference in the results from these studies could be attributed to research methodology or a distinction between children themselves in different countries and cultural environments [43]. Similarly, with externalizing problem, the trajectory of emotional problems presented the same stable trend in both boys and girls, which was consistent with other research findings [1, 44]. Girls exhibited higher internalizing problems than boys, but the difference was not significant in our study. The finding agreed with the mainstream view that gender difference in internalizing problems becomes gradually magnified from the early childhood to adolescence [45]. The third type of problem behaviors we focused on were social communication problems, with a significantly declining trajectory in both boys and girls. This result was in line with the development of child’s social skills, which showed rapid improvement during the preschool period [46]. Children (3–4-years-old) exhibited a tendency for sharing toys, being friendly during play-time and felt content. Children 5–6 years old exhibited more helpful behaviors [46]. The boys exhibited more problems than girls, which was attributed to the later language development, more externalizing behaviors that influence their peer relationships, or higher morbidity of social communication problems [7, 46].
In this study, 82.3 % of the participants were the only children, which is representative of the current situation in China. Certain previous studies indicated that the only child was thought to be overprotected, self-centered and lacking peer interaction before the school age, which may aggravate the problem [1416, 18]. However, no difference in the trajectory of behavior problems was observed between the only or non-only child in this study, which was consistent with other studies in China [20, 38]. The reasons behind the different results between China and the other countries, we assumed, were partially attributed to the Chinese one-child policy. The one child policy was implemented by a series of provisions, for example, families with two or more children would suffer financial punishment while the only child family would be encouraged by obtaining more financial aid [47]. This could result in an economic burden on families with more than one child, and, furthermore, decrease the economic resources available to each child. In addition, in comparison with children with siblings, the only child would obtain more interpersonal resources from the parents, such as attention, time and energy, which may be conductive to their well-being [20, 48]. Lastly, the participants in this study were all enrolled in kindergartens, therefore, the children could learn the skills of cooperation, emotional control, compromise, and understanding from their interaction with peers in class who played the role of siblings. Overall, although the only children could have some negative characteristics in their behavior, they also had more economical and interpersonal family resources available to them, especially due to the only child policy in China, which played a positive role on their behavior [48].
Child temperament is the third factor that has been found to predict child problem behaviors. Children who had more difficult temperaments as infants or toddlers have been found to have more externalizing and internalizing behavior problems during childhood [40, 49]. Miner’s research reported similar findings, that externalizing behavior trajectory declined more steeply for children exhibiting with difficult temperaments early in life [24]. We extended this conclusion to emotional and social communication problems as well. In 6-year-old children, there was no perceived difference in problem behaviors between the children with difficult and easy temperaments, in comparison to the infant phase. This sharp decline may reflect the fact that children with difficult temperaments exhibited more frustration, anger, withdrawal and anxiety at home early on, but over time, became more aligned with peers after they enrolled in kindergarten. Another study reported that this difference in trajectory of temperament was possibly due to a biased report by the parents, the evaluation of which was influenced by the early months dealing with an irritable infant [24].

Parent effect on change of child problem behaviors

Sufficient literature has documented that family time-varying factors, such as maternal depression, harsh parenting and the family environment, contributed to a high risk of occurrence and continuation of child problem behaviors [22, 24, 25, 4952]. These factors change over time and with their effects on child behaviors. Moreover, the change in these factors was affected by child behavior [44]. Nevertheless, few studies paid attention to the change in risk factors associated with a change in child problem behavior. This study provided a primary analysis of parent risk factors effect change of child behavior and may provide evidence for an intervention program.
Our results on maternal depression showed that children whose mothers had depressive symptoms during the early years, exhibited increased problem behaviors in later years. This result supported the vast majority of previous researches findings that maternal depression has been shown to have deleterious and long-lasting effects on a child’s cognitive and socio-emotional development [5356].
Parenting factors that influenced the change in child behavior were the trajectory of warmth/understanding as protective factors, and excessive interference/protection, punishment as the risk factor. This result was in line with previous findings from longitudinal researches of other countries and cross-sectional researches in our country [24, 57, 58]. The increase in the positive parenting attitude and a sensitive response to the child could contributed to a secure attachment, which was linked to many positive outcomes in children and a decrease in problem behavior [59, 60]. However, early negative parenting interactions, whether authoritarian or over interferencing/overprotective, could predict adverse outcomes. Jordana K found overinvolved/protective parenting also being important at preschool age [57]. The mechanisms of risk transmission may be that the parents, who interfered too much with the child’s thoughts and behavior, caused an inhibition in child’s self-expression, thereby promoting problem behaviors [57, 61]. In addition, the present findings confirmed data of other researches highlighting hash discipline as key predictor of early childhood externalizing and internalizing symptoms [18].
As prevention of child mental health is a global public health program, researches that attend to suggest the prevention are warranted. The present findings indicated that prevention efforts should focus on parenting strategies and maternal depression, and such programs could begin in preschool period.

Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions

The present study provided the first longitudinal data on the development of children’s problem behaviors during the preschool period in China, with mostly only children in the population sample. We applied contemporary statistical techniques of repeated measures to examine the patterns over time. Last but not the least, the parent effect of change in risk factors and child problem behaviors was analyzed instead of focusing on the one-time point status. This finding strongly supported Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory and directly suggested that intervention programs should pay more attention to children with aggravating family risks [21].
Besides these strengths, the findings from this study should be considered with several limitations. Despite the size and diversity of the sample, the study still lacked scattered children not attending kindergarten. Since the exact proportion of children attending kindergarten could not be found in the year book which was published by statistical bureau of Guangzhou, we could only know from our life experience that vast majority of children went to kindergarten at 3-year old in urban area. Therefore, the sample could not be regarded as representative of the general population. Another limitation of the study was single informants of the predictors and independents, which may have resulted in bias in maternal perception [24]. At last, there were still 3.8 % missing data in every questionnaire, which may be the cause of some bias in the study.

Conclusions

The results of our study provide an important direction for future researches to determine the effect of influencing factors in more comprehensive, nationwide level. Based on this, population level, randomized, controlled trials of intervention studies are required to test the cost-effectiveness of prevention aimed at reducing children’s problem behaviors.

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by the Science and Technology Program of Guangzhou. We would like to thank the kindergartens of Feng Le, Tai Jing, Fu Li Hui, Hai Guan, Yu Lei, Jin Bi, Yi Yuan, Wen Chong, and the bureau of maternal and child health of the Guangzhou and Huangpu districts for their assistance with data collection.

Availability of data and materials

All data and materials related to the study can be obtained through contacting the first author at baopeng1987@163.com.

Authors’ contributions

PB and JJ proposed the study and wrote the first draft. YJ and XH analyzed the data. BL and MH contributed to the design and interpretation of the study. All authors contributed to the whole work of the investigation and further drafts. JJ is the guarantor. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Not applicable
This study was approved by the Ethics Reviews Committee for Biomedical Research of the School of Public Health, Sun Yat-Sen University, China. Parents of every participated child provided written informed consents before the research started.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bosquet M, Egeland B. The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18(2):517–50.CrossRefPubMed Bosquet M, Egeland B. The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18(2):517–50.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Bor W, McGee TR, Fagan AA. Early risk factors for adolescent antisocial behaviour: an Australian longitudinal study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2004;38(5):365–72.CrossRefPubMed Bor W, McGee TR, Fagan AA. Early risk factors for adolescent antisocial behaviour: an Australian longitudinal study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2004;38(5):365–72.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Sawyer MG, Arney FM, Baghurst PA, et al. The mental health of young people in Australia: key findings from the child and adolescent component of the national survey of mental health and well-being. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35(6):806–14.CrossRefPubMed Sawyer MG, Arney FM, Baghurst PA, et al. The mental health of young people in Australia: key findings from the child and adolescent component of the national survey of mental health and well-being. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35(6):806–14.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(3-4):313–37.CrossRefPubMed Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(3-4):313–37.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, et al. How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychol Med. 2010;40(6):899–909.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, et al. How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychol Med. 2010;40(6):899–909.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Jin J. Progress in diagnosis and treatment for children with autistic spectrum disorders. J Appl Clin Pediatr. 2010;25(23):1777–82. Jin J. Progress in diagnosis and treatment for children with autistic spectrum disorders. J Appl Clin Pediatr. 2010;25(23):1777–82.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat van Zeijl J, Mesman J, Stolk MN, et al. Terrible ones? Assessment of externalizing behaviors in infancy with the Child Behavior Checklist. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(8):801–10.CrossRefPubMed van Zeijl J, Mesman J, Stolk MN, et al. Terrible ones? Assessment of externalizing behaviors in infancy with the Child Behavior Checklist. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(8):801–10.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Gilliom M, Shaw DS. Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16(2):313–33.CrossRefPubMed Gilliom M, Shaw DS. Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16(2):313–33.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Meng H, Haffner J, Parzer P, et al. Comparison on emotional and behavioral problems of children in China and Germany. Chin J Sch Health. 2009;12:1092–4. Meng H, Haffner J, Parzer P, et al. Comparison on emotional and behavioral problems of children in China and Germany. Chin J Sch Health. 2009;12:1092–4.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Wan G, Wei Z, He H, et al. Prevalence of emotional disorders in preschool children in Shenzhen. CJCHC. 2011;19(12):1077–1079. Wan G, Wei Z, He H, et al. Prevalence of emotional disorders in preschool children in Shenzhen. CJCHC. 2011;19(12):1077–1079.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang W, Jian J, Xia Z. Epidemiological investigation for the behavior paoblems of preschool children in City. Appl J Gen Pract. 2006;03:360–1. Zhang W, Jian J, Xia Z. Epidemiological investigation for the behavior paoblems of preschool children in City. Appl J Gen Pract. 2006;03:360–1.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang X. Study of chindren’s peer interaction problem in perspective of family. Early Education. 2012(02):26–28. Wang X. Study of chindren’s peer interaction problem in perspective of family. Early Education. 2012(02):26–28.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Laybourn A. Only children in Britain: Popular stereotype and research evidence. Children Soc. 1990;4(4):386–400.CrossRef Laybourn A. Only children in Britain: Popular stereotype and research evidence. Children Soc. 1990;4(4):386–400.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Doh H-S, Falbo T. Social competence, maternal attentiveness, and overprotectiveness: Only children in Korea. Int J Behav Dev. 1999;23(1):149–62.CrossRef Doh H-S, Falbo T. Social competence, maternal attentiveness, and overprotectiveness: Only children in Korea. Int J Behav Dev. 1999;23(1):149–62.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Veenhoven R, Verkuyten M. The well-being of only children. Adolescence. 1989;24(93):155–66.PubMed Veenhoven R, Verkuyten M. The well-being of only children. Adolescence. 1989;24(93):155–66.PubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kemppainen L, Jokelainen J, Jarvelin MR, et al. The one-child family and violent criminality: a 31-year follow-up study of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):960–2.CrossRefPubMed Kemppainen L, Jokelainen J, Jarvelin MR, et al. The one-child family and violent criminality: a 31-year follow-up study of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):960–2.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, et al. Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(11):1166–74.PubMed Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, et al. Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(11):1166–74.PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Teramoto S, Soeda A, Hayashi Y, et al. Problematic behaviours of 3-year-old children in Japan: relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds. Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(6):563–9.CrossRefPubMed Teramoto S, Soeda A, Hayashi Y, et al. Problematic behaviours of 3-year-old children in Japan: relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds. Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(6):563–9.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu RX, Lin W, Chen ZY. School performance, peer association, psychological and behavioral adjustments: a comparison between Chinese adolescents with and without siblings. J Adolesc. 2010;33(3):411–7.CrossRefPubMed Liu RX, Lin W, Chen ZY. School performance, peer association, psychological and behavioral adjustments: a comparison between Chinese adolescents with and without siblings. J Adolesc. 2010;33(3):411–7.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological system theory. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 1989. Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological system theory. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 1989.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards SL, Rapee RM, Kennedy S. Prediction of anxiety symptoms in preschool-aged children: examination of maternal and paternal perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(3):313–21.CrossRefPubMed Edwards SL, Rapee RM, Kennedy S. Prediction of anxiety symptoms in preschool-aged children: examination of maternal and paternal perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(3):313–21.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Miner JL, Clarke-Stewart KA. Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(3):771–86.CrossRefPubMed Miner JL, Clarke-Stewart KA. Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(3):771–86.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Ashford J, Smit F, van Lier PA, et al. Early risk indicators of internalizing problems in late childhood: a 9-year longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(7):774–80.CrossRefPubMed Ashford J, Smit F, van Lier PA, et al. Early risk indicators of internalizing problems in late childhood: a 9-year longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(7):774–80.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Dwyer SB, Nicholson JM, Battistutta D. Population level assessment of the family risk factors related to the onset or persistence of children’s mental health problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(5):699–711.CrossRefPubMed Dwyer SB, Nicholson JM, Battistutta D. Population level assessment of the family risk factors related to the onset or persistence of children’s mental health problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(5):699–711.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat McCarty CA, Zimmerman FJ, Digiuseppe DL, et al. Parental emotional support and subsequent internalizing and externalizing problems among children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(4):267–75.CrossRefPubMed McCarty CA, Zimmerman FJ, Digiuseppe DL, et al. Parental emotional support and subsequent internalizing and externalizing problems among children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(4):267–75.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:175–99.CrossRefPubMed Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:175–99.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Kou J, Yasong D, Xia L. Reliability and validity of “children strengths and difficulties questionnair”in Shanghai norm. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. 2005;01:25–8. Kou J, Yasong D, Xia L. Reliability and validity of “children strengths and difficulties questionnair”in Shanghai norm. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. 2005;01:25–8.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, et al. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–9.CrossRefPubMed Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, et al. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–9.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang Z. Behavioral medicine inventory manual. Beijing: Chinese Medical Multimedia Press; 2005. Zhang Z. Behavioral medicine inventory manual. Beijing: Chinese Medical Multimedia Press; 2005.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhu X, Liang A. The relationship between children’s temperament, parent educating ay and the behavior Problem of 2 to 3 Year oU childeren in the country of BeJing. Chin Ment Health J. 2007;21(11):795–9. Zhu X, Liang A. The relationship between children’s temperament, parent educating ay and the behavior Problem of 2 to 3 Year oU childeren in the country of BeJing. Chin Ment Health J. 2007;21(11):795–9.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang J. A study of child temperament and behavior peoblems. Chin Ment Health J. 1996;06:9–13. Zhang J. A study of child temperament and behavior peoblems. Chin Ment Health J. 1996;06:9–13.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Radloff LS. The CES–D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401.CrossRef Radloff LS. The CES–D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Yunfen L, Luowei Z, Hui L. et al. Relative study of parents in child-rearing methods and temperament, between acts. CJCHC. 2009;17(01):56–58. Yunfen L, Luowei Z, Hui L. et al. Relative study of parents in child-rearing methods and temperament, between acts. CJCHC. 2009;17(01):56–58.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(2):155–72.CrossRefPubMed McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(2):155–72.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Guo-bin W, Zhen W, Hui-jing HE, et al. Prevalence of emotional disorders in preschool children in Shenzhen. Chin J Child Health Care. 2011;19(12):1077–9. Guo-bin W, Zhen W, Hui-jing HE, et al. Prevalence of emotional disorders in preschool children in Shenzhen. Chin J Child Health Care. 2011;19(12):1077–9.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Liping GU, Ang C, Cheng D, et al. The relationship between behavioral problems and family environment in preschool-age children. J Clin Pediatr. 2014;10:965–9. Liping GU, Ang C, Cheng D, et al. The relationship between behavioral problems and family environment in preschool-age children. J Clin Pediatr. 2014;10:965–9.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Min HU, Jing J, Jin Y, et al. Cross-sectional study on behavior problems and influential factors among preschool children in Huangpu district of Guangzhou. Chin J Evid Based Pediatr. 2012;07(1):11–8. Min HU, Jing J, Jin Y, et al. Cross-sectional study on behavior problems and influential factors among preschool children in Huangpu district of Guangzhou. Chin J Evid Based Pediatr. 2012;07(1):11–8.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Owens EB, Shaw DS. Predicting growth curves of externalizing behavior across the preschool years. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31(6):575–90.CrossRefPubMed Owens EB, Shaw DS. Predicting growth curves of externalizing behavior across the preschool years. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31(6):575–90.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, et al. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: a six-site, cross-national study. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):222–45.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, et al. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: a six-site, cross-national study. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):222–45.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Hongming Qu, Ling Lv, Qin Li, et al. Behavior Problems and the Influencing Factors of School Aged Children in Nanjing. Chin J Sch Health, 2008;29(09):810–812. Hongming Qu, Ling Lv, Qin Li, et al. Behavior Problems and the Influencing Factors of School Aged Children in Nanjing. Chin J Sch Health, 2008;29(09):810–812.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin L. A comparative study of behavior problems of preschool children between China and America. Sci Social Psychol. 2013;28(03):83–85. Lin L. A comparative study of behavior problems of preschool children between China and America. Sci Social Psychol. 2013;28(03):83–85.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Sterba SK, Prinstein MJ, Cox MJ. Trajectories of internalizing problems across childhood: heterogeneity, external validity, and gender differences. Dev Psychopathol. 2007;19(2):345–66.CrossRefPubMed Sterba SK, Prinstein MJ, Cox MJ. Trajectories of internalizing problems across childhood: heterogeneity, external validity, and gender differences. Dev Psychopathol. 2007;19(2):345–66.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Mathiesen KS, Sanson A, Stoolmiller M, et al. The nature and predictors of undercontrolled and internalizing problem trajectories across early childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(2):209–22.CrossRefPubMed Mathiesen KS, Sanson A, Stoolmiller M, et al. The nature and predictors of undercontrolled and internalizing problem trajectories across early childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(2):209–22.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaffer D, Kipp K. Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence. Belmont: Cengage Learning; 2013. Shaffer D, Kipp K. Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence. Belmont: Cengage Learning; 2013.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang W, Du W, Liu P, et al. Five-factor personality measures in Chinese university students: effects of one-child policy? Psychiatry Res. 2002;109(1):37–44.CrossRefPubMed Wang W, Du W, Liu P, et al. Five-factor personality measures in Chinese university students: effects of one-child policy? Psychiatry Res. 2002;109(1):37–44.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhu Z, Tingting C. Comparative study between single child and non-single child’s rearing patterns in Rural Primary School. China J Health Psychol. 2010;18(11):1346–8. Zhu Z, Tingting C. Comparative study between single child and non-single child’s rearing patterns in Rural Primary School. China J Health Psychol. 2010;18(11):1346–8.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Marakovitz SE, Wagmiller RL, Mian ND, et al. Lost toy? Monsters under the bed? Contributions of temperament and family factors to early internalizing problems in boys and girls. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(2):233–44.CrossRefPubMed Marakovitz SE, Wagmiller RL, Mian ND, et al. Lost toy? Monsters under the bed? Contributions of temperament and family factors to early internalizing problems in boys and girls. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(2):233–44.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, et al. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):189–200.CrossRefPubMed Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, et al. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(2):189–200.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Leve LD, Kim HK, Pears KC. Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing trajectories from ages 5 to 17. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33(5):505–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Leve LD, Kim HK, Pears KC. Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing trajectories from ages 5 to 17. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33(5):505–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Colder CR, Mott JA, Berman AS. The interactive effects of infant activity level and fear on growth trajectories of early childhood behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(1):1–23.CrossRefPubMed Colder CR, Mott JA, Berman AS. The interactive effects of infant activity level and fear on growth trajectories of early childhood behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(1):1–23.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Mantymaa M, Puura K, Luoma I, et al. Predicting internalizing and externalizing problems at five years by child and parental factors in infancy and toddlerhood. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012;43(2):153–70.CrossRefPubMed Mantymaa M, Puura K, Luoma I, et al. Predicting internalizing and externalizing problems at five years by child and parental factors in infancy and toddlerhood. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012;43(2):153–70.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw DS, Connell A, Dishion TJ, et al. Improvements in maternal depression as a mediator of intervention effects on early childhood problem behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(2):417–39.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Shaw DS, Connell A, Dishion TJ, et al. Improvements in maternal depression as a mediator of intervention effects on early childhood problem behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(2):417–39.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Fox NA, et al. Predicting social wariness in middle childhood: the moderating roles of child care history, maternal personality and maternal behavior. Soc Dev. 2008;17(3):471–87.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Fox NA, et al. Predicting social wariness in middle childhood: the moderating roles of child care history, maternal personality and maternal behavior. Soc Dev. 2008;17(3):471–87.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Goosby BJ. Poverty duration, maternal psychological resources, and adolescent socioemotional outcomes. J Family Issues. 2007;28(8):1113–34.CrossRef Goosby BJ. Poverty duration, maternal psychological resources, and adolescent socioemotional outcomes. J Family Issues. 2007;28(8):1113–34.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayer JK, Ukoumunne OC, Lucas N, et al. Risk factors for childhood mental health symptoms: national longitudinal study of Australian children. Pediatrics. 2011;128(4):e865–79.CrossRefPubMed Bayer JK, Ukoumunne OC, Lucas N, et al. Risk factors for childhood mental health symptoms: national longitudinal study of Australian children. Pediatrics. 2011;128(4):e865–79.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Yang X, Yin L. Correlation Between Behavior Problems In Preschool Children And Parental Rearing Styles. ACTA Acad Med Qingdao Universitatis. 2009;06:511–3. Yang X, Yin L. Correlation Between Behavior Problems In Preschool Children And Parental Rearing Styles. ACTA Acad Med Qingdao Universitatis. 2009;06:511–3.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Mesman J, Stoel R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, et al. Predicting growth curves of early childhood externalizing problems: differential susceptibility of children with difficult temperament. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(5):625–36.CrossRefPubMed Mesman J, Stoel R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, et al. Predicting growth curves of early childhood externalizing problems: differential susceptibility of children with difficult temperament. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(5):625–36.CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Rapee RM, Kennedy S, Ingram M, et al. Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(3):488–97.CrossRefPubMed Rapee RM, Kennedy S, Ingram M, et al. Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(3):488–97.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Wobber V, Herrmann E, Hare B, et al. Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56(3):547–73.CrossRefPubMed Wobber V, Herrmann E, Hare B, et al. Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56(3):547–73.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Trajectories and the influencing factors of behavior problems in preschool children: a longitudinal study in Guangzhou, China
verfasst von
Peng Bao
Jin Jing
Yu Jin
Xumin Hu
Buyun Liu
Min Hu
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0864-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

BMC Psychiatry 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe