Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Understanding pathways to social inequalities in childhood unintentional injuries: findings from the UK millennium cohort study

verfasst von: M. Campbell, E. T. C. Lai, A. Pearce, E. Orton, D. Kendrick, S. Wickham, D. C. Taylor-Robinson

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Childhood unintentional injuries (UI) are common but continue to happen more often to children living in less advantaged socioeconomic circumstances (SEC). Our aim was to explore how early life factors mediate the association between SEC and UIs, using the UK Millennium Cohort Study.

Methods

We calculated risk ratios (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (95%CI) for parental report of UI occurring between age 3 and 5 years, using Poisson regression according to family income as a measure of SEC. We explored potentially mediating pathways by controlling associations between SEC and UI for groups of early life risks in three domains: factors that may influence environmental safety, supervision and the MCS child’s abilities and behaviours.

Results

Twenty eight percent of children had a UI from 3 to 5 years old. Children from the lowest income quintile were more likely to be injured compared to those from the highest (RR 1.20 95%CI 1.05, 1.37). Sequentially controlling for early life factors that may influence environmental safety (RR 1.19 95%CI 1.02, 1.38), then supervision (RR 1.18, 95%CI 1.02, 1.36), and finally adding child’s behaviour and abilities (RR 1.15, 95%CI 1.00, 1.34) into the model reduced the RR by 5, 10 and 25% respectively.

Conclusions

Addressing factors that may influence environmental safety and supervision, and the child’s abilities and behaviours only partly explains the increased UI risk between the highest and lowest income quintiles. Further research is required to explore factors mediating associations between SEC and specific mechanisms and types of injuries.
Abkürzungen
(N=) or (n=)
(N = denominator number/ count) (n = numerator number or count)
aRR
adjusted Risk Ratio
MCS
Millennium Cohort Study
MCSc
Millennium Cohort Study Child
MI
Multiple Imputations
NDE
Natural Direct Effect shown from the Counter Factual Model
NIE
Natural Indirect Effect shown from the Counter Factual Model
RR
Risk Ratio
SDQ
Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire
SEC
Socio-Economic Circumstances
TE
Total Effect shown from the Counter Factual Model
U.S.
United States
UI
Unintentional Injuries
U.K.
United Kingdom

Background

Childhood unintentional injuries (UI) are common but their frequency, severity and consequences disproportionately impact on those growing up in more disadvantaged circumstances. [14] In England and Wales, death rates of children aged 28 days to 15 years due to UIs were 4.5 times higher from routine or manual worker households compared to those from managerial or professional homes in 2001/03 [5]. Despite a decline in medically attended UI rates over time, the social inequalities gradient has persisted [6].
Underpinning most childhood UI prevention policies, the ‘Haddon Matrix of Injury Occurrence’ [1, 7, 8] categorises most known risk factors into: the host (i.e. in this case the child, including their cognitive and/ or physical characteristics); the physical environment; the social environment at the time of the incident, and the agent of injury defined by the of mechanism of injury [7]. Nearly all injury risk factors are more commonly experienced by children growing up in poverty [9]. Yet, we currently lack the essential understanding of the complex pathways linking adverse social conditions to the heightened risk of UIs in childhood, which are needed in order to develop effective interventions and equitable policies [1].
There are a number of plausible pathways explaining why children growing up in lower income households are more likely to experience UIs [10]. This paper explores three of the most common pathways. First, Children growing up in less advantaged households live in more hazardous environments compared to their more affluent peers (such as less safe housing with a greater likelihood of playing on a street rather than in a garden) [7], potentially explaining their increased risk of UIs during childhood. Second, it is suggested environmental hazards can be mitigated for, if children are supervised adequately and nurtured to develop risk avoidance skills [11, 12]. It is argued that stressors for families living in lower income households may impair supervision and thus, further increase UI risk [11]. Third, some children are at greater risk of a UI, because of their individual abilities and behaviours. Such as ADHD [13], visual impairments [14] and risk-taking behaviours [15] which are more prevalent in lower socioeconomic circumstances.
Using a contemporary, nationally representative sample of children from the UK, we aimed to assess the social patterning of UI in children from 3 to 5 years old. We also examined the extent to which any excess risk in UI for children growing up in disadvantaged circumstances was mediated by potentially modifiable early life factors influencing their environment and supervision, and also measures of the child’s abilities and behaviours.

Methods

Design, setting, and data source

We used data from the UK Millennium Cohort Study (MCS), a nationally representative UK birth cohort of 19,250 children born between September 2000 and January 2002, sourced from the U.K. Data Service in 2015. This cohort study used trained interviewers to carry out home-based survey interviews with the main responder, usually the primary carer, about their child and their life. These interviews started when the MCS child was aged nine months, were repeated at 3 years and again at 5 years old. This study uses data on 10,210 children with recorded responses on our primary outcome (UI) and exposure (household income), which are defined further below. The MCS oversampled children living in disadvantaged areas and, in the case of England, areas with high proportions of ethnic minority groups by means of a stratified clustered sampling design [16]. Further information on the cohort and sampling design can be found in the cohort profile [16] or online (www.​cls.​ioe.​ac.​uk/​mcs). The analysis did not require additional ethical or consent approval [17].

Primary outcome and exposures

The primary outcome was UIs of any injury type. The main respondent was asked if the child, then aged 5 years old, ‘Ever had an accident and was taken to the doctor, health centre or hospital?’ since the previous survey assessment when the child was aged 3; creating a binary outcome variable (yes, no). Our main exposure of interest and potentially mediating explanatory variables were measured before this at ages 9 months and 3 years, to enable temporal sequencing of the exposure, mediator and outcome measures.
Our primary SEC exposure was equivalised household income (EHI), weighted for the number of adults and dependent children in the household, and divided into quintiles. This was used as a stable measure of early life SEC that preceded the mediator and outcome measures [18].

Potential confounding factors

We adjusted a number of potential confounders for the exposure and outcome: ethnicity (white, non-white) [19] maternal age at MCS child’s birth (14–19, 20–24, 25–34, 35+ years old) [20], and number of other children in the family at MCS child’s birth (only MCS child, 2–3 children, 4+ children) [21] in our baseline analysis. We also adjusted for the child’s sex, since it is strongly related to our outcome (UIs) [1].

Early life risk factors (potential mediators)

Based on a literature review we were able to map MCS data to enable us to create categories of early life risk factors (potential mediators) appropriate to three of Haddon’s domains: the child’s environment, supervision and the child’s abilities and behaviours.

Factors that may influence environmental safety

Factors captured in the MCS that also provide proxy measures for safety in the child’s physical environments include, a count of responder-reported child safety equipment used from five potential items: car seat, safety gate, fireguard, plug socket covers and smoke alarm(s), measured at MCS child (MCSc) age 9 months (none, one, two, three, four or five items) [22]; responder-reported ‘safe places in your area to play’ measured at MCSc age 9 months (safe, not safe) [22]; interviewer assessed safe in-house environment at MCSc age 3 years, using the short form Caldwell and Bradley’s Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment scale (safe, not safe) [23]; responder-reported not having access to a garden at MCSc age 9 months (yes, no) [24]; interviewer assessed living in an ‘organised house’ or not at MCSc age 3, (very organised, organised, average, disorganised, very disorganised) as levels of household chaos is a recognised risk for childhood UI [25]. We also included a measure related to having household pets at MCSc age 3, for risks related to bites and falls (no pets, pets – including: dogs, cats, other furry animals, birds and other animals such as reptiles) [12, 26]. Household smoking at MCSc age 3, was also considered as it relates to increased risk of burns and household fires (non-smoker, smoker) [27]. Type of childcare was included as a potential measure of care and environmental quality in the case of registered childcare (in parental care, unregistered childcare/other members of the family or friends, registered childcare) [24]. We also included a binary measure of responder-reported feelings of safety in the local area at MCSc age 3 (very safe and fairly safe; neither safe nor unsafe, fairly unsafe and very unsafe) [28].

Factors that may influence child supervision

Factors that may influence supervision include supervisor’s mental or physical health, risk taking behaviours, and social support [29, 30]. Relevant factors that were also captured in the MCS include main responder’s level of distress in the last month at MCSc age 3, assessed using the Kessler score for mental distress (normal score range 0 to 14, distressed scores > = 15, 30) main responder’s alcohol unit consumption per week (p/w) at MCSc age 3, (none, 1–5, 6–14, > 14 units p/w) [1]; main responder’s style of parenting relevant to the MCS child, (grouped as either ‘structured parenting style with rules’ combining responses to: firm rules and discipline, firm discipline, plus lots of fun, or ‘unstructured/casual parenting style with rules’ combining responses to: doing my best for the children, lots of fun, have not really thought about it) [31]; social support network measured by family and friends living nearby [32] (neither live nearby, just friends, just family or both) and main responder’s having a limiting long term condition (no, yes) [33].

Child’s abilities and behaviours

Factors that provide early life proxy measures for the child’s abilities and behaviours that are also captured in MCS, include: Bracken school readiness (child’s mean school readiness score ≥ 80, not school ready [mean score] from 0 to 79), which measures age-related cognitive ability at MCSc age 3 [9]; socio-emotional development using the total score from the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) which has been categorised (normal score range 0 to 13, borderline score range 14 to 16, abnormal scores equal to or greater than 17), to assess four domains relating to peer problems, conduct disorders, hyperactivity and emotional problems measured at MCSc age 3, based on activities within the last 6 months [34]; responder-reported concerns about MCSc’s hearing (yes, no) [35] and responder-reported concerns about MCSc’s sight both at age 3 (yes, no) [14].

Analysis strategy and statistical methods

First, we assessed the prevalence of experiencing one or more UIs according to income. We then undertook a univariable analysis estimating risk ratios (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (95%CI) by Poisson regression for the association between early life risk factors and UI. We then progressed to multivariable analysis, using a complete case sample, whereby variables that were significant at the P < 0.1 level in the univariable analysis (likelihood ratio test) [36] were adjusted for in order to assess how this changed the RR for UI comparing lowest to highest income quintile.
Using Haddon’s matrix for injury occurrence to provide our three domain definitions, variables were grouped as blocks of potentially mediating risk factors that may influence (i) environmental safety, (ii) supervision and (iii) factors relating to the child abilities and behaviours. Our approach to assess the impact of these three domains on baseline risk was twofold. First, each domain was added to the baseline model individually to assess their potential isolated impact. Second, each domain was sequentially added to baseline: adjusting for environmental safety first, supervision, and finally child’s abilities and behaviours. The order of adjustment in our sequential model reflected our priori hypotheses about the relationship between these three domains (e.g. environment is potentially driving the association between supervision, child abilities and behaviours, and their UI risk). We also assessed the impact of alternative orders of adjustment. Any observed change in RR was taken to indicate potential mediation [24].
We estimated the change in RRs comparing children in the lowest to the highest income quintiles (the SEC gap) after adjusting for each domain of factors to the model. This was calculated as 100x(adj. Baseline RR - adj. Model RR)/(adj. Baseline RR - 1) [37]. Wald tests were used to assess the significance of individual model parameters. All our analyses were conducted in Stata/SE v.13 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, U.S.A.) with survey (svy) commands to account for the sample design and attrition up to age 5.

Sensitivity analyses

We repeated the analyses using two alternative measures of childhood SEC, maternal education [38] and also lone parent status [39] both reported at MCSc birth. We repeated the analysis using UIs requiring hospital admission (not admitted, admitted), a more severe outcome for our final model. We undertook multiple imputations by chained equations to explore the impact of missing data in our primary analysis. Missing data ranged from one missing data point (maternal age) to 2719 for the Kessler scale. We imputed missing data for 4152 cases spread across 15 different variables, giving an analytic sample of 14,355. These were created using all variables in the final multivariable regression model including our study outcome (UI reported between 3 and 5 years old), primary exposure (household income quintile) and survey weightings. Twenty imputed datasets were calculated, and estimates were combined using Rubin’s rules [40].
Finally, we also undertook a mediation analysis using counterfactual methods to assess how much of the effect of SEC income on childhood UIs is mediated via the three domains. We estimated the Natural Direct Effect (NDE), Natural Indirect Effect (NIE) and Total Effect (TE), after accounting for potential confounding by known covariates, using the Medflex package (2018) in R software. This statistical programme gives us the flexibility to assess the effect of specific causal pathways in order to quantify its contribution to the outcome of interest. Unlike previous mediation methods, this contemporary approach takes into account the possible interactions between multiple mediators when calculating the proportion mediated [41].

Results

In total, 14,335 singleton children had data on both UI and their household income meeting the inclusion criteria, accounting for 94% of all successful interviews at age 5 years. Of these, 10,210 cases were used in the complete case analysis (Figure 3 in Appendix: sample flow chart). In this total sample, 28% of children (n = 4019) experienced at least one UI between the ages of 3 to 5 years old. UIs increase in a dose-response manner as household income decreases. The proportion of children that experienced UIs ranged from 24% in the highest income quintile, to 31% in the lowest income quintile (Table 1).
Table 1
Percentage of each variable in household income quintiles using the total cohort sample (N = 14,355)
Variables
Income quintile
Total
P-valueb
First (Highest)
Second
Third
Fourth
Fifth (lowest)
Percent (count)
Count of child in each quintile
2490
2695
2771
3138
3261
14,553
Unintentional injuries between 3 and 5 years old
24.0
26.9
28.7
29.4
31.0
28.0 (4019)
< 0.001
Baseline Risks
 MCS child (MCSc) is male, recorded at 9 months old
51.3
50.7
51.7
50.8
50.4
51.0 (7335)
0.09
 MCSc from non-white ethnic group recorded at 9 months old
8.0
5.5
8.5
16.6
23.4
12.1 (1919)
< 0.001
Maternal age at birth, recorded at 9 months old
 14–19 years
0.5
1.5
4.2
9.0
23.1
7.7 (1114)
< 0.001
 20–24 years
3.0
8.3
16.8
25.8
29.1
16.6 (2551)
 25–34 years
94.6
88.6
78.2
64.0
46.8
74.4 (10494)
 35+ years
2.0
1.6
0.8
1.3
1.0
1.3 (195)
Number of children in the household at birth (including MCSc) recorded at 9 months old
 MCS lone child
56.8
43.0
40.0
31.6
39.4
42.1 (6008)
< 0.001
 2/3 children
41.1
54.3
54.8
56.5
47.9
50.9 (7226)
 4 or more children
2.2
2.7
5.3
11.9
12.7
7.0 (1121)
Factors That May Influence Environmental Safety
 No safe areas to play, MCSc 9 months old
20.4
25.4
34.1
43.0
51.0
34.8 (5367)
< 0.001
Items of named safety equipment used, MCSc 9 months old (count)a
 None
0.4
0.4
0.7
3.5
7.6
2.5 (452)
< 0.001
 1 piece
3.5
3.8
5.6
11.4
17.6
8.4 (1280)
 2 pieces
18.8
17.6
19.8
22.7
25.4
20.9 (3081)
 3 pieces
25.9
25.3
26.6
26.1
23.8
25.6 (3739)
 4 pieces
31.0
33.4
31.2
23.8
17.3
27.4 (3724)
 5 pieces
20.3
19.6
16.1
12.4
8.2
15.3 (2075)
 No garden, MCSc age 3
1.5
2.3
4.1
9.8
17.0
6.7 (873)
< 0.001
Type of childcare used, MCSc age 3
 Parent care only
47.8
56.2
67.0
84.6
93.5
69.2 (8858)
< 0.001
 Registered childcare (e.g. Ofsted inspected nursery)
19.0
26.2
24.9
12.4
5.1
17.8 (2479)
 Non-registered childcare (e.g. family, friends)
33.2
17.6
8.1
3.1
1.4
13.0 (1575)
 Has a household pets, MCSc age 3
42.5
50.8
54.8
51.8
45.8
49.1 (6029)
< 0.001
 Poor home safety (HOME score), MCSc age 3
0.1
1.1
0.9
2.3
3.8
1.6 (191)
< 0.001
 Exposed to household smoking, MCSc age 3
5.2
6.9
13.3
24.5
36.8
16.9 (2217)
< 0.001
 Responder feels unsafe in area, MCSc age 3
5.4
5.6
10.3
17.0
23.0
12.0 (1656)
< 0.001
Disorganised home environment, MCSc age 3
 Very organised
25.0
22.3
19.3
17.4
15.8
20.1 (2608)
< 0.001
 Organised
57.2
56.5
55.6
50.5
48.2
53.7 (6967)
 Average
10.2
11.4
13.5
15.5
16.3
13.3 (1668)
 Disorganised
6.3
8.6
10.0
13.7
16.2
10.8 (1379)
 Very disorganised
1.3
1.2
1.6
2.9
3.5
2.1 (261)
Factors that may influence supervision
 Primary carer distressed, MCSc age 3
0.7
1.0
2.2
3.4
5.9
2.5 (302)
< 0.001
 Primary carer Limiting long term condition, MCSc age 3
15.9
18.5
22.0
26.6
25.4
21.51 (2841)
< 0.001
Primary caregiver alcohol units per week, MCSc age 3
 None
34.5
49.9
62.0
71.5
76.3
58.9 (8947)
< 0.001
 1–6 units
43.7
36.2
28.2
19.7
14.5
28.4 (3735)
 7–14 units
18.2
11.4
8.0
6.8
6.3
10.1 (1349)
 15 or more units (exceeding current UK Guidance)
3.6
2.5
1.8
1.9
2.9
2.5 (317)
 Has an unstructured parenting style (e.g. few rules) MCSc age 3
45.3
49.4
57.3
61.9
65.3
55.5 (7068)
< 0.001
Family and friends live nearby MCSc age 3
 Both
40.0
50.8
55.8
52.5
50.7
50.0 (7285)
< 0.001
 Family only
5.0
6.8
8.8
9.6
12.4
8.5 (1350)
 Friends only
40.3
30.6
23.8
23.7
21.3
27.9 (3796)
 Neither nearby
14.7
11.8
11.6
14.3
15.6
13.6 (1906)
Factors relating to the mcs child’s abilities and behaviours
 Score from Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) MCSc age 3, graded as:
  Average
90.0
88.6
81.1
72.8
63.6
79.9 (9607)
< 0.001
  Borderline
6.4
7.4
11.0
12.5
14.9
10.2 (1258)
  Below average
3.7
4.1
7.9
14.7
21.6
9.9 (1248)
  Sight concerns, MCSc age 3
5.1
5.8
6.4
6.0
6.1
5.9 (566)
0.19
  Hearing concerns, MCSc age 3
5.4
4.0
4.2
4.2
4.0
4.3 (853)
0.52
  Child is NOT school ready, MCSc age 3
3.5
5.3
9.4
18.2
26.5
12.1 (1586)
< 0.001
N = 14,355 with further missing data for some named variables as: Child: minority/ ethnic (n = 1454), Strength and difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) at age 3 (n = 2242), School readiness (n = 2712), hearing concerns at 3 yrs old (n = 69, sight concern at 3 yrs old (n = 59) Maternal age at MCS birth (n = 1), Mum has a LLTC at 3 years (n = 1471) Levels of distress: Kessler score > 5 (n = 2719), Parenting style (n = 1936), Alcohol (n = 7), Social network(n = 18), Number of children in household (n = 563), Household pet (n = 1392), Household smoking at 3 yrs old (n = 1471), No safe places to play at 9 months (n = 180), No safety equipment at 9 months (n = 4), safe areas to play at 3 yrs (n = 2365), Childcare type (n = 1443), Responder feels safe (n = 1471), disorganised home (n = 1472)
a List safety equipment includes [no particular order]: stairgate, fireguard, plug socket covers, car seat, and smoke alarm
b Chi squared test
Table 2 shows the results of the univariable and final multivariable analysis. In our univariable analysis low household income, being male, being white, younger maternal age at MCSc’s birth, more children than the MCSc living in the household, no safe areas to play, not having specified items of safety equipment, using informal childcare, having household pets, exposure to household smoking, responder not feeling safe in their area, living in a disorganised household, main responder higher distress (Kessler scores), main responder having a limiting long term condition, higher SDQ score for socioemotional behavioural difficulties and not being school ready were all associated with an increased RR for UI at P < 0.1 (1 d.p.) (Table 2).
Table 2
Risk ratios 95% confidence intervals unintentional injuries in highest versus lowest income quintiles analysis
Variables
Total sample (n = 14,355)a
FINAL MODEL COMPLETE CASE (n = 10,210)b
Univariable
Multivariable
Risk ratio (RR)
95% LCI
95% UCI
P-value
RR
95% LCI
95% UCI
Household Income Quintiles
 First (refer: highest)
Ref
< 0.001
Ref
 Second
1.12
0.99
1.25
1.08
0.96
1.22
 Third
1.19
1.06
1.33
1.11
0.98
1.26
 Fourth
1.22
1.07
1.38
1.09
0.95
1.26
 Fifth (Lowest)
1.27
1.13
1.43
1.15
0.996
1.34
Baseline Risk Factors
 Child’s sex (Ref: female)
1.22
1.14
1.31
< 0.001
1.22
1.13
1.32
 Child ethnic group (Ref: White)
0.74
0.66
0.82
< 0.001
0.79
0.68
0.91
Maternal age at MCS birth
 35+ years
Ref
< 0.001
Ref
 25–34 years
0.69
0.5
0.93
1.07
0.92
1.25
 20–24 years
0.84
0.76
0.93
0.95
0.82
1.11
 14–19 years
0.99
0.88
1.11
0.77
0.55
1.09
Number of children in the household at birth (including MCS child)
 MCS lone child
Ref
0.01
Ref
 2/3 children
1.1
1.03
1.17
1.10
1.01
1.20
 4 or more children
1.11
0.99
1.25
1.22
1.04
1.44
Factors That May Influence Environmental Safety
 Safe areas to play at home at 9 months (Ref: No safe areas)
1.06
0.99
1.12
0.08
1.00
0.92
1.08
Items of named safety equipment used at 9 months
 None
Ref
0.04
Ref
 1 piece
1.13
0.87
1.47
1.15
0.79
1.66
 2 pieces
1.28
1.01
1.63
1.25
0.9
1.74
 3 pieces
1.32
1.04
1.67
1.36
0.98
1.89
 4 pieces
1.34
1.06
1.7
1.36
0.98
1.90
 5 pieces
1.33
1.04
1.71
1.36
0.97
1.92
 Access to a garden at 9 months (Ref: No access)
1.05
0.91
1.21
0.49
Type of childcare used MCSc age 3
 None (parents look after child)
Ref
0.09
Ref
 Registered childcare provider
0.91
0.83
1
0.99
0.89
1.10
 Non-registered childcare provider (e.g. family, friends)
0.94
0.85
1.04
1.03
0.91
1.17
 Household pets MCSc age 3 (ref: No pets)
1.14
1.07
1.21
< 0.001
1.04
0.97
1.12
 Household smoking MCSc age 3 (ref: None)
1.15
1.05
1.26
< 0.001
1.01
0.90
1.13
 Poor home safety (HOME score) MCSc age 3
1.20
0.93
1.55
0.17
 Responder feels unsafe in area MCSc age 3
1.12
1.03
1.22
0.01
1.08
0.98
1.19
Disorganised home environment MCSc age 3
 Very organised
Ref
< 0.001
Ref
 Organised
1.09
1
1.18
1.05
0.95
1.15
 Average
1.15
1.02
1.29
1.11
0.98
1.27
Disorganised
1.16
1.02
1.31
1.06
0.92
1.22
Very disorganised
1.61
1.32
1.95
1.35
1.09
1.67
Factors That May Influence Supervision
Maternal distress at 3 yrs. (Ref: Kessler < 5)
1.33
1.12
1.59
0.01
1.18
0.94
1.47
Limiting long term condition MCSc age 3 (Ref: none)
1.16
1.07
1.26
< 0.001
1.08
0.98
1.19
Alcohol units per week (MCSc age 3)
None
Ref
0.58
1–6 units
0.98
0.92
1.05
7–14 units
1.04
0.93
1.16
15 or more units+
1.1
0.92
1.32
Approach to parenting MCSc age 3 (Ref: Unstructured)
1
0.93
1.06
0.71
Family and friends live nearby, MCSc age 3 (Ref: both)
Both
Ref
0.19
Family only
1.06
0.95
1.17
Friends only
0.95
0.89
1.02
Neither nearby
1.00
0.91
1.11
Factors Relating To The Mcs Child’s Abilities And Behaviours
Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), MCSc age 3
Average
Ref
< 0.001
Ref
Borderline
0.06
1.03
1.26
1.05
0.93
1.19
Below average
1.32
1.19
1.46
1.16
1.03
1.32
Sight concerns, MCSc age 3
1.02
0.9
1.15
0.75
Hearing concerns, MCSc age 3
1.08
0.95
1.23
0.24
Child is NOT school ready, MCSc age 3
0.91
0.82
1.00
0.06
1.06
0.96
1.18
awhen P < 0.1 (if rounded 1 d.p.) variables were included in the complete case, multivariable analysis. b Using sample comprising complete cases for all variables in final multivariable analysis. + 15 or more units is in excess of UK Guidance for all adult
In the final multivariable model (Table 2) there was no significant association between UI and income. The risk of a UI remained significantly higher for MCSc: with other siblings living at home from birth, living in a very disorganised household, who are male, who are from white ethnic group, and that have below average SDQ scores.
We assessed how the baseline RR for UI in the lowest income quintile compared to the highest adjusting for child’s sex and ethnicity, number of children in household and maternal age at birth (aRR 1.20 95%CI 1.05, 1.37) changed after adjusting for each of the UI risk factor domains individually. Adjusting for the environmental safety domain, (e.g. safe areas to play, safety equipment use, childcare type, household pets, in-house smoking, household organization levels and area safety) attenuated the RR by 5% (aRR 1.19 95% CI 1.03 to 1.38). Adjusting for the supervision domain (e.g. primary carer Kessler score and primary carer has a limiting long term condition) attenuated the RR by 10% (aRR 1.18 95%CI 1.03 to 1.34). Adjusting for the child abilities and behaviours domain (e.g. child’s school readiness and SDQ scores) attenuated the RR by 25% (aRR 1.15, 95%CI 1.005, 1.32) (Fig. 1).
Layering the three domains sequentially, starting with environmental safety, attenuated the baseline risk by 5% (aRR 1.19 95% CI 1.03 to 1.38); then adding the supervision domain attenuated the baseline risk to 10% (aRR 1.18 95%CI 1.02, 1.36), and in our final model (including all three domains) the baseline risk was attenuated to 25% (aRR 1.15 95%CI 0.997 to 1.34), rendering the association between UIs and household income to non-significant (Fig. 2).

Sensitivity analyses

Our findings were similar when we used lone parent status as an alternative SEC exposure measure and an alternative outcome measure of hospital admissions (Table 3 in Appendix). However, maternal educational, as an alternative measure of SEC did not yield a substantial nor significant reduction in the increase UI risk seen in children with mothers qualified to GCSE D-E/ no qualifications, compared to those with a Degree or higher qualification (Table 3 in Appendix). Repeating the analysis using multiple imputations for missing data showed similar results to our complete case analysis (Table 3 in Appendix).
Our mediation analysis using counterfactual approaches also suggested that the three blocks of mediators (factors that may influence environmental safety, quality of supervision and also the child’s abilities and behaviours), only partially explained income inequalities in UI. Overall 32% of the total effect of income (lowest income quintile versus highest) on childhood UIs is mediated through adjusting for factors that may influence environmental safety, quality of supervision and also the child’s abilities and behaviours, with a total effect of, NDE (1.14 (95% CI 0.98, 1.32)) and NIE (1.06 95% CI 0.98,1.14) (Table 4 in Appendix). Our mediation analysis provides comparable results to our primary analysis and also highlights that a large proportion of the pathways to inequalities are unexplained in this analysis (Table 4 in Appendix).

Discussion

Main findings

Using a nationally representative sample of UK children born in 2000–2002, we found more than one in four children (28%) had an unintentional injury (UI) from age 3 to 5 years old. Children from the lowest income households were more likely to have UIs (31%), compared to those living in the highest income homes (24%). We found that the elevated risk in the low income group compared to high was only partially attenuated after adjusting for baseline risks, and potentially mediating factors that may influence environmental safety, supervision, and the child abilities and behaviours.

Comparison with others findings

The current evidence supports our findings of a social gradient in UIs for preschool children with many of these studies also showing an association with one or more of our exposure domains (e.g. environment, supervision and the child’s abilities and behaviours). [24] A systematic review identified 57 empirical studies dated from 1990 to 2009 that explored SEC inequalities for five common UI mechanisms (traffic, drowning, poisoning, burns and falls). [4] The authors concluded that low SEC was associated with increased risk of UI, however the social gradient varied by factors including the environmental settings and the selected measure of SEC. [4] Our study also found the inequalities gap varied by SEC measure used, with lone parent status [versus two parent households] yielding the greatest difference in childhood UI risk, and the least difference seen for maternal educational attainment.
Several studies have sought to better understand the pathways that link SEC to UIs in children. [22, 42, 43] Laflamme and Diderichsen [42] reviewed the literature on traffic injuries in childhood to develop a conceptual framework, based on the Diderichsen model of pathways to social inequalities, [44] that identified potential mechanisms through which social context (e.g. geographical variation in risk), social position (e.g. income, ethnicity and family characteristics), and various exposures (e.g. behaviours) may interact to generate health inequalities. [42] Our analysis shows around 25% of the increased UI risk for children from the lowest income quintile can be explained by early life factors ranging from their social demographics (e.g. family characteristics) to various lifestyle and environmental exposures (e.g. activities and behaviours). This is corroborated by our counterfactual mediation analysis which further suggests that other unexplored pathways to inequalities in UI are likely to exist.
Finally, several factors from across each domain remained significantly associated with increased UI risk, independent of SEC. Similar to other studies, we found an independent increased UI risk in males [15], having below average SDQ scores at 3 years old [45], having a greater number of siblings from birth [21] and living in a very disorganised household at age 3 years [25]. Adding to the on-going debate about ethnicity and UI risk [33], we found that non-white ethnicity was associated with a reduced UI risk. An American based study of preschool-age children also found white children of unemployed mothers living in households needing repair were at higher injury risk than children from other ethnic groups [46].

Strengths and limitations

We have used a large nationally representative UK cohort that has regularly gathered extensive details on the child, their family and home, and community environments using validated approaches from birth. This enabled us to explore a wide variety of covariates associated with UI risks and broadly reflect the domains of Haddon’s matrix (e.g. the characteristics of the child as the host; in and around the home as the physical environment and factors that may influence supervision reflecting the social environment) for UI from 3 to 5 years old [7].
A limitation of this study is that we did not have sufficiently detailed information about the mechanism and types of the injuries to investigate whether pathways linking adverse social conditions to the heightened risk of UIs vary for specific types of injury. For example, the specific pathways to inequalities in accidental poisoning may differ from those for burns or fractures. Larger studies with more detailed information on injury mechanisms and types are required to examine how potentially mediating pathways might vary by injury characteristics.
The MCS dataset does contain some validated measures of household hazards, quality of supervision and child abilities and behaviours relevant to UI (such as Caldwell and Bradley’s Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment scale) [23] and we have used these in our model. However, we acknowledge these are limited in number. Consequently, we have mainly used indicators that may influence these constructs and that have been used in previous studies [43], but we do not have an assessment of their validity. We judge that our non-validated measures may incompletely capture exposures for UI risk factors, potentially underestimating the proportion mediated by each domain, presenting a non-differential bias [47]. Further studies could build upon our findings using validated measures where these data are available. Equally, we were limited to measures and records at specific time points predetermined by the MCS study. It is essential for our analysis that the mediators occurred before the outcome (UI between 3 and 5 years) event, so our mediator data was collected up to age 3 years.
It is also important to recognise that outcome measures (UI for which medical attention was sought) were reported by the parent, predominantly mothers and may be subject to recall bias [48, 49]. Studies seeking to validate the parent reporting approach have shown it to be more complete at capturing UIs than routine medical notes for more severe injuries, but recall is diminished over time [48]. We accept this study’s two year recall period for childhood UIs, may have led to a conservative prevalence estimate [49], but there is little evidence to suggest that might explain variation in childhood UIs by SEC [48].
An inherent challenge in large cohort studies is missing data. Our main analysis used a complete case sample, whereby individuals with incomplete data on covariates were excluded from the analysis. Sampling and response weights were used to account for the sampling design and attrition. Reassuringly, our sensitivity analysis using multiple imputations produced similar results and conclusions. Finally, our sensitivity analyses using two alternative SEC exposure(s) and a more severe outcome measure (hospital admissions for injury) provide some reassurance about the consistency of the findings from our primary analysis.

Conclusions

In our analysis, adjusting for a wide range of factors that may influence environment safety, supervision and the MCS child’s abilities and behaviours partially attenuated the excess UI risk experienced by children growing-up in lower income households. Whist this may partially reflect incomplete measurement of potentially mediating pathways, it is likely that there are other explanations for the observed inequality in UI beyond the domains explored in our study. From a health inequalities perspective, the policy and practice implications of our study are that it is unlikely that inequalities in UI for children can be addressed by interventions and policies that only target environmental safety, supervision, or children’s abilities and behaviours. Furthermore, broader policies that aim to improve socio-economic conditions (e.g. increasing household income) are also necessary, and this is particularly important in the context of rising child poverty in the UK, which is likely to increase the risk of a range of adverse outcomes, including UIs.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all the Millennium Cohort children and their families for participatiing in the study. I am grateful to the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS), UCL Institute of Education, for the use of these data and to the UK Data Service for making them available. However, neither CLS nor the UK Data Service bear any responsibility for the analysis or interpretation of these data.

Funding

No specific funding was received for this study. MC was funded by a Health Education England Fellowship Grant. DTR and ETCL are funded by an MRC Clinician Scientist Fellowship (ref MR/P008577/1). SLW is supported by a Wellcome Trust Society and Ethics fellowship (grant number 200335/Z/15/Z). Our work was also supported by The Farr Institute for Health Informatics Research (Medical Research Council grant MR/M0501633/1).

Availability of data and materials

All MCS data used in this analysis are available from UK Data Service, University of Essex and University of Manchester: https://​doi.​org/​10.​5255/​UKDA-SN-4683-4; https://​doi.​org/​10.​5255/​UKDA-SN-5350-4; https://​doi.​org/​10.​5255/​UKDA-SN-7464-3.-accessed in 2015
The Millennium Cohort Study was approved by the South West and London Multi Centre Research Ethics Committees. Referenced as MREC/01/6/19, MREC/03/2/022, 05/MRE02/46 for sweeps one, two and three respectively [17]. The present analyses did not require additional ethics approval.
The Millennium Cohort Study obtained informed written consent from parent/ guardians of the cohort children in order to participate in the study and other participants as necessary [17]. The present analyses did not require additional consent approval.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors have no financial relationships relevant to this article and no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Anhänge

Appendix

Table 3
Sensitivity analysis for alternative SEC exposures, outcomes and imputed data
Indicators
Adjusted baselinea
Model: factors influencing environment
Model: factors influencing environment and supervision
Final model: factors influencing environment and supervision, and child’s abilities and behaviours
Proportion mediated
Alternative measure of SEC exposure
 Maternal education
1.25 (1.10, 1.42)
1.24 (1.09, 1.42)
1.23 (1.09, 1.41)
1.22 (1.07, 1.38)
12%
 Lone parent status
1.19 (1.09, 1.30)
1.13 (1.01, 1.26)
1.12 (1.00, 1.25)
1.11 (0.99, 1.24)
42%
Alternative measure of outcome
 Hospital admissions – more severe UIs
2.08 (1.11, 3.89)
1.91 (0 .91, 4.01)
1.93 (0.92, 4.08)
1.79 (0.84, 3.80
27%
Accounting for missing data
 Imputed dataset
1.22 (1.09, 1.36)
1.19 (1.05, 1.35)
1.17 (1.04, 1.33)
1.15 (1.02, 1.30)
32%
a Baseline adjusted for Child’s sex, ethnicity, number of other children at home at birth and mother’s age at MCS child’s birth
Table 4
Medflex counter factual mediation analysis for unintentional injuries risk ratios comparing highest versus lowest income quintiles
Models
Effect
RR
95% Lower CI
95% Upper CI
Proportion mediated
MODEL 1: Environmental safety
natural direct effect
1.17
1.01
1.37
9.80%
natural indirect effect
1.02
0.94
1.09
total effect
1.19
1.05
1.36
MODEL 2: Child supervision
natural direct effect
1.17
1.04
1.33
12.7%
natural indirect effect
1.02
1.00
1.04
total effect
1.20
1.05
1.36
MODEL 3: Child’s ability and behaviours
natural direct effect
1.15
1.01
1.30
26.3%
natural indirect effect
1.05
1.02
1.07
total effect
1.20
1.05
1.36
MODEL 4: All three above domains
natural direct effect
1.14
0.98
1.32
31.6%
natural indirect effect
1.06
0.98
1.14
total effect
1.20
1.05
1.37
Table 5
Complete case analysis for variables used in the final modelb
Variables
Complete Case (n = 10,210)b
Univariablea
Multivariable
RR
95% LCI
95% UCI
RR
95% LCI
95% UCI
Household Income Quintiles
 First (refer: highest)
Ref
Ref
 Second
1.12
0.99
1.25
1.08
0.96
1.22
 Third
1.19
1.7
1.33
1.11
0.98
1.26
 Fourth
1.22
1.08
1.38
1.09
0.95
1.26
 Fifth (Lowest)
1.28
1.13
1.44
1.15
0.996
1.34
Baseline Risk Factors
 Child’s sex (Ref: female)
1.23
1.14
1.33
1.22
1.13
1.32
 Child ethnic group (Ref: White)
0.78
0.68
0.9
0.79
0.68
0.91
Maternal age at MCS birth
 35+ years
Ref
Ref
 25–34 years
1.07
0.92
1.24
1.07
0.92
1.25
 20–24 years
0.92
0.82
1.05
0.95
0.82
1.11
 14–19 years
0.76
0.54
1.06
0.77
0.55
1.09
Number of children in the household at birth (including MCS child)
 MCS lone child
Ref
Ref
 2/3 children
1.11
1.02
1.2
1.1
1.01
1.2
 4 or more children
1.25
1.08
1.43
1.22
1.04
1.44
Factors That May Influence Environmental Safety
 Safe areas to play at home at 9 months (Ref: No safe areas)
1.06
0.98
1.14
1
0.92
1.08
Items of named safety equipment used at 9 months
 None
Ref
Ref
 1 piece
1.11
0.76
1.61
1.15
0.79
1.66
 2 pieces
1.18
0.84
1.66
1.25
0.9
1.74
 3 pieces
1.28
0.92
1.79
1.36
0.98
1.89
 4 pieces
1.28
0.91
1.79
1.36
0.98
1.9
 5 pieces
1.27
0.90
1.8
1.36
0.97
1.92
 Access to a garden at 9 months (Ref: No access)
Type of childcare used at 3 yrs. old
 None (parents look after child)
Ref
Ref
 Registered childcare provider
0.94
0.85
1.04
0.99
0.89
1.1
 Non-registered childcare provider (e.g. family, friends)
0.91
0.81
1.02
1.03
0.91
1.17
 Household pets at 3 yrs. old (ref: No pets)
1.09
1.02
1.17
1.04
0.97
1.12
 Household smoking at 3 yrs. old (ref: None)
1.14
1.03
1.26
1.01
0.9
1.13
 Poor home safety (HOME score) at 3 yrs. old
 Responder feels unsafe in area at 3 yrs. old
1.17
1.06
1.29
1.08
0.98
1.19
Disorganised home environment at age 3 years
 Very organised
Ref
Ref
 Organised
1.08
0.98
1.19
1.05
0.95
1.15
 Average
1.19
1.05
1.36
1.11
0.98
1.27
 Disorganised
1.18
1.03
1.35
1.06
0.92
1.22
 Very disorganised
1.55
1.25
1.93
1.35
1.09
1.67
Factors That May Influence Supervision
 Maternal distress measured MCSc age 3 (Ref: Kessler < 5)
1.36
1.09
1.68
1.18
0.94
1.47
 Limiting long term condition MCSc age 3 (Ref: none)
1.12
1.02
1.24
1.08
0.98
1.19
Alcohol units per week MCSc age 3
 None
 1–6 units
 7–14 units
 15 or more unitsc
 Approach to parenting MCSc age 3 (Ref: Unstructured)
Family and friends live nearby, MCSc age 3 (Ref: both)
 Both
 Family only
 Friends only
 Neither nearby
Factors Relating To The Mcs Child’s Abilities And Behaviours
 Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), MCSc age 3
  Average
Ref
Ref
  Borderline
1.12
0.99
1.26
1.05
0.93
1.19
  Below average
1.3
1.16
1.46
1.16
1.03
1.32
  Sight concerns, MCSc age 3
  Hearing concerns, MCSc age 3
  Child is NOT school ready, MCSc age 3
0.85
0.76
0.94
1.06
0.96
1.18
awhen P < 0.1 (if rounded 1 d.p.) variables were included in the complete case, multivariable analysis
b Using sample comprising complete cases for all variables in final multivariable analysis
c 15 or more units is in excess of UK Guidance for all adults
Literatur
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Brownell M, Derksen S, Jutte D, Roos N, Ekuma O, Yallop L. Socio-economic inequities in children’s injury rates: has the gradient changed over time? Can J Public Heal. 2010;101(Suppl 3):S28–31. Brownell M, Derksen S, Jutte D, Roos N, Ekuma O, Yallop L. Socio-economic inequities in children’s injury rates: has the gradient changed over time? Can J Public Heal. 2010;101(Suppl 3):S28–31.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Laflamme L, Hasselberg M, Burrows S. 20 years of research on socioeconomic inequality and children’s unintentional injuries understanding the cause-specific evidence at hand. Int J Pediatr. 2010;2010:23.CrossRef Laflamme L, Hasselberg M, Burrows S. 20 years of research on socioeconomic inequality and children’s unintentional injuries understanding the cause-specific evidence at hand. Int J Pediatr. 2010;2010:23.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Orton E, Kendrick D, West J, Tata LJ. Persistence of health inequalities in childhood injury in the UK; a population-based cohort study of children under 5. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e111631.CrossRef Orton E, Kendrick D, West J, Tata LJ. Persistence of health inequalities in childhood injury in the UK; a population-based cohort study of children under 5. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e111631.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Haddon W. The changing approach to the epidemiology, prevention, and amelioration of trauma: the transition to approaches etiologically rather than descriptively based. Am J Public Health. 1968;58(8):1431–8.CrossRef Haddon W. The changing approach to the epidemiology, prevention, and amelioration of trauma: the transition to approaches etiologically rather than descriptively based. Am J Public Health. 1968;58(8):1431–8.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat The National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control. Injury prevention: meeting the challenge. Am J Prev Med. 1989;5:1–303.CrossRef The National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control. Injury prevention: meeting the challenge. Am J Prev Med. 1989;5:1–303.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Morrongiello BA, Ondejko L, Littlejohn A. Understanding toddlers’ in-home injuries: II. Examining parental strategies, and their efficacy, for managing child injury risk. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29(6):433–46.CrossRef Morrongiello BA, Ondejko L, Littlejohn A. Understanding toddlers’ in-home injuries: II. Examining parental strategies, and their efficacy, for managing child injury risk. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29(6):433–46.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuhn J, Damashek A. The role of proximal circumstances and child behaviour in toddlers’ risk for minor unintentional injuries. Inj Prev. 2015;21(1):30–4.CrossRef Kuhn J, Damashek A. The role of proximal circumstances and child behaviour in toddlers’ risk for minor unintentional injuries. Inj Prev. 2015;21(1):30–4.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee LC, Harrington RA, Chang JJ, Connors SL. Increased risk of injury in children with developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2008;29(3):247–55.CrossRef Lee LC, Harrington RA, Chang JJ, Connors SL. Increased risk of injury in children with developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2008;29(3):247–55.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Legood R, Scuffham P, Cryer C. Are we blind to injuries in the visually impaired? A review of the literature. Inj Prev. 2002;8(2):155–60.CrossRef Legood R, Scuffham P, Cryer C. Are we blind to injuries in the visually impaired? A review of the literature. Inj Prev. 2002;8(2):155–60.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Visser E, Pijl YJ, Stolk RP, Neeleman J, Rosmalen JGM. Accident proneness, does it exist? A review and meta-analysis. Accid Anal Prev. 2007;39(3):556–64.CrossRef Visser E, Pijl YJ, Stolk RP, Neeleman J, Rosmalen JGM. Accident proneness, does it exist? A review and meta-analysis. Accid Anal Prev. 2007;39(3):556–64.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Connelly R, Platt L. Cohort profile: UK millennium cohort study (mcs). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(6):1719–25.CrossRef Connelly R, Platt L. Cohort profile: UK millennium cohort study (mcs). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(6):1719–25.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Steinbach R, Green J, Edwards P, Grundy C. “Race” or place? Explaining ethnic variations in childhood pedestrian injury rates in London. Heal Place. 2010;16(1):34–42.CrossRef Steinbach R, Green J, Edwards P, Grundy C. “Race” or place? Explaining ethnic variations in childhood pedestrian injury rates in London. Heal Place. 2010;16(1):34–42.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Morrongiello BA. Preventing unintentional injuries to young children in the home: understanding and influencing parents’ safety practices. Child Dev Perspect. 2018;0(0):1–6. Morrongiello BA. Preventing unintentional injuries to young children in the home: understanding and influencing parents’ safety practices. Child Dev Perspect. 2018;0(0):1–6.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearson M, Garside R, Moxham T, Anderson R. Preventing unintentional injuries to children in the home: a systematic review of the effectiveness of programmes supplying and/or installing home safety equipment. Health Promot Int. 2010;26:376–92.CrossRef Pearson M, Garside R, Moxham T, Anderson R. Preventing unintentional injuries to children in the home: a systematic review of the effectiveness of programmes supplying and/or installing home safety equipment. Health Promot Int. 2010;26:376–92.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearce A, Li L, Abbas J, Ferguson B, Graham H, Law C. Does the home environment influence inequalities in unintentional injury in early childhood? Findings from the UK millennium cohort study. J Epidemiol community health. 2011;66(2):181–8.CrossRef Pearce A, Li L, Abbas J, Ferguson B, Graham H, Law C. Does the home environment influence inequalities in unintentional injury in early childhood? Findings from the UK millennium cohort study. J Epidemiol community health. 2011;66(2):181–8.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamp Dush CM, Schmeer KK, Taylor M. Chaos as a social determinant of child health: reciprocal associations? Soc Sci Med 2013;95:69–76. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.01.038CrossRef Kamp Dush CM, Schmeer KK, Taylor M. Chaos as a social determinant of child health: reciprocal associations? Soc Sci Med 2013;95:69–76. Available from: http://​dx.​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​socscimed.​2013.​01.​038CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Keuster TD, Lamoureux J, Kahn A. Epidemiology of dog bites: a Belgian experience of canine behaviour and public health concerns. Vet J. 2006;172(3):482–7.CrossRef Keuster TD, Lamoureux J, Kahn A. Epidemiology of dog bites: a Belgian experience of canine behaviour and public health concerns. Vet J. 2006;172(3):482–7.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Rawlins JM, Khan AA, Shenton AF, Sharpe DT. Epidemiology and outcome analysis of 208 children with burns attending an emergency department. Pediatr Emerg Care. 2007;23(5):289–93.CrossRef Rawlins JM, Khan AA, Shenton AF, Sharpe DT. Epidemiology and outcome analysis of 208 children with burns attending an emergency department. Pediatr Emerg Care. 2007;23(5):289–93.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kendrick D, Young B, Mason-Jones AJ, Ilyas N, Achana FA, Cooper NJ, et al. Home safety education and provision of safety equipment for injury prevention (review). Evidence-Based Child Heal. 2013;8(3):761–939.CrossRef Kendrick D, Young B, Mason-Jones AJ, Ilyas N, Achana FA, Cooper NJ, et al. Home safety education and provision of safety equipment for injury prevention (review). Evidence-Based Child Heal. 2013;8(3):761–939.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulvaney C, Kendrick D. Do maternal depressive symptoms, stress and a lack of social support influence whether mothers living in deprived circumstances adopt safety practices for the prevention of childhood injury? Child Care Health Dev. 2006;32(3):311–9.CrossRef Mulvaney C, Kendrick D. Do maternal depressive symptoms, stress and a lack of social support influence whether mothers living in deprived circumstances adopt safety practices for the prevention of childhood injury? Child Care Health Dev. 2006;32(3):311–9.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Morrongiello BA, Widdifield R, Munroe K, Zdzieborski D. Parents teaching young children home safety rules: implications for childhood injury risk. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35(3):254–61.CrossRef Morrongiello BA, Widdifield R, Munroe K, Zdzieborski D. Parents teaching young children home safety rules: implications for childhood injury risk. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35(3):254–61.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Reading R, Langford IH, Haynes R, Lovett A. Accidents to preschool children: comparing family and neighbourhood risk factors. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(3):321–30.CrossRef Reading R, Langford IH, Haynes R, Lovett A. Accidents to preschool children: comparing family and neighbourhood risk factors. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(3):321–30.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Mann JR. Children with hearing loss and increased risk of injury. Ann Fam Med. 2007;5(6):528–33.CrossRef Mann JR. Children with hearing loss and increased risk of injury. Ann Fam Med. 2007;5(6):528–33.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Bursac Z, Gauss CH, Williams DK, Hosmer DW. Purposeful selection of variables in logistic regression. Source Code Biol Med. 2008;3:1–8.CrossRef Bursac Z, Gauss CH, Williams DK, Hosmer DW. Purposeful selection of variables in logistic regression. Source Code Biol Med. 2008;3:1–8.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Richiardi L, Bellocco R, Zugna D. Mediation analysis in epidemiology: methods, interpretation and bias. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:1511–9.CrossRef Richiardi L, Bellocco R, Zugna D. Mediation analysis in epidemiology: methods, interpretation and bias. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:1511–9.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearce A, Li L, Abbas J, Ferguson B, Graham H, Law C. Does childcare influence socioeconomic inequalities in unintentional injury? Findings from the UK millennium cohort study. J Epidemiol community health. 2010;64:161–6.CrossRef Pearce A, Li L, Abbas J, Ferguson B, Graham H, Law C. Does childcare influence socioeconomic inequalities in unintentional injury? Findings from the UK millennium cohort study. J Epidemiol community health. 2010;64:161–6.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Evan T, Whitehead M, Diderichsen F, Bhuiya A, Wirth M. Introduction. In: Evans, Whitehead, Diderichsen, Bhuiya, Wirth, editors. Challenging inequities in health care: from ethics to action. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. p. 3–11.CrossRef Evan T, Whitehead M, Diderichsen F, Bhuiya A, Wirth M. Introduction. In: Evans, Whitehead, Diderichsen, Bhuiya, Wirth, editors. Challenging inequities in health care: from ethics to action. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. p. 3–11.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Garzon DL, Huang H, Todd RD. Do attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder influence preschool unintentional injury risk? Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 2008;22(5):288–96.CrossRef Garzon DL, Huang H, Todd RD. Do attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder influence preschool unintentional injury risk? Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 2008;22(5):288–96.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Blakely T, Mckenzie S, Carter K. Misclassification of the mediator matters when estimating indirect effects. J Epidemiol Community Heal. 2013;67:458–66 Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.CrossRef Blakely T, Mckenzie S, Carter K. Misclassification of the mediator matters when estimating indirect effects. J Epidemiol Community Heal. 2013;67:458–66 Available from: http://​dx.​doi.​org/​10.​1136/​jech.​CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Mock C, Acheampong F, Adjei S, Koepsell T. The effect of recall on estimation of incidence rates for injury in Ghana. Int J Epidemiol. 1999;28(4):750–5.CrossRef Mock C, Acheampong F, Adjei S, Koepsell T. The effect of recall on estimation of incidence rates for injury in Ghana. Int J Epidemiol. 1999;28(4):750–5.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Understanding pathways to social inequalities in childhood unintentional injuries: findings from the UK millennium cohort study
verfasst von
M. Campbell
E. T. C. Lai
A. Pearce
E. Orton
D. Kendrick
S. Wickham
D. C. Taylor-Robinson
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1514-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Pediatrics 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.