Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2011

Open Access 01.12.2011 | Research article

When does poor subjective financial position hurt the elderly? Testing the interaction with educational attainment using a national representative longitudinal survey

verfasst von: Christy Pu, Nicole Huang, Gao-Jun Tang, Yiing-Jenq Chou

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2011

Abstract

Background

Several studies have demonstrated that perceived financial status has a significant impact on health status among the elderly. However, little is known about whether such a subjective perception interacts with objective socioeconomic status (SES) measures such as education that affect the individual's health.

Methods

This research used data from the Survey of Health and Living Status of the Middle Age and Elderly in Taiwan (SHLS) conducted by the Bureau of Health Promotion, Department of Health in Taiwan. Waves 1996, 1999 and 2003 were used. The sample consisted of 2,387 elderly persons. The interactive effects of self-rated satisfaction with financial position and educational attainment were estimated. Self-rated health (SRH), depressive symptom (measured by CES-D) and mortality were used to measure health outcomes.

Results

Significant interaction effect was found for depressive symptoms. Among those who were dissatisfied with their financial position, those who were illiterate had an odds ratio (OR) of 8.3 (95% CI 4.9 to 14.0) for having depressive symptoms compared with those who were very satisfied with their financial position. The corresponding OR for those with college or above was only 2.7 (95% CI 1.0 to 7.3). No significant interaction effect was found for SRH and mortality.

Conclusions

Although poor financial satisfaction was found to be related to poorer health, the strongest association for this effect was observed among those with low educational attainment, and this is especially true for depressive symptoms. Subjective financial status among the elderly should be explored in conjunction with traditional measures of SES.
Hinweise

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

CP was responsible for data analysis and drafted the manuscript, NH and GT designed the study and helped in drafting the manuscript, YC helped in designing the study and finalized the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

The relationship between socioeconomic (SES) and health disparities has been well documented, with those having lower SES being less healthy. However, it has been found that medical care and health behavior do not sufficiently account for this relationship and the psychosocial stress caused by inequalities is one of the potential explanatory factors [1]. The relationship between SES and health disparities thus can result from two pathways, the material and the psychosocial [2]. While the importance of the former lies in social policies that deal with resource distribution, the latter provides information on the behavioral and psychological responses to these disparities [2]. Recently, there has been an increasing interest in exploring the relationship between subjective SES and health disparities [37].This branch of research suggests that ''psychological stress'' plays an important role in affecting health adversely.
Subjective SES consists of a complex measure of a person's relative standing in society, and is often defined as "an individual's perception of his/her place in the socioeconomic structure" (p.1321) [8], which may include both social and economic phenomena. Subjective SES is important in research on health disparities because it has been shown that SES may operate at the social level [9]. It has been argued that a person's relative standing may be more important than his absolute SES in terms of influencing health [10]. In addition, previous research has indicated that distribution of income and neighborhood characteristics are closely associated with health and sometimes even have stronger effects than an individual's SES [1113]. These inequality effects on health may manifest through subjective SES, for example through mechanisms such as psychological well-being and behavioral choices; importantly, the distress caused by such inequalities may not be captured by objective SES [14].
The above mentioned research on subjective SES suggests that if one wishes to reduce health disparities, then the relationship between subjective SES and health status should not be overlooked. Several studies have found a significant relationship between subjective SES and health (such as self-rated health, coronary heart disease and all cause mortality) [7, 1517]. For the elderly, subjective or perceived SES is of particular importance because traditional SES measures such as income, occupation and education are limited when measuring SES during old age. For older adults, income and occupation may not be relevant due to retirement, and the benefits reflected by education may not be the same as those obtained from other attainments [18].
Subjective SES is a multidimensional concept and many measures have been proposed in previous research. Each of these measures captures a different aspect of what is termed "subjective SES". One of the frequently used measure of subjective SES is a question involves a ten rung ladder asking the respondents to place themselves in the ladder, with the top of the ladder representing those who are best off in the society (who have most money, most education and best jobs) [19]. A modification has been made by asking the respondent to compare themselves with different reference groups (others in the society, others of the same race or ethnicity or neighbors and parents when they were at the same age) [15]. One study used individual perceptions of workplace status (whether the respondents perceive themselves as ''employees'', ''foremen'', or ''managers'') as the measure of subjective SES [17]. Relative comparison (to doctors and to garbage collectors) pertaining to occupational prestige as well as perceived job control have also been examined [16, 20].
Within different measures of subjective SES, subjective financial position has been shown to have separate effect on health independent of other SES measures. For example, Butterworth et al[21] has shown that financial hardship is particularly associated with depression independent of the effect of absolute income. Szanton et al. [18] found that women who reported financial strain had a higher risk of death, and that financial strain is a stronger predictor of mortality than annual income among older women. Similarly, Zimmerman and Katon [22] found that employment status and financial strain are causally related to depression, but that income is not. This suggests that how people perceive their financial position may be more important in terms of affecting their health than their actual income level.
Financial position has been measured differently in various studies. Other than income level, some have asked the respondent whether there is any money left over at the end of month [18], others have asked whether they are suffering from financial hardship [21], and yet others have asked whether they have difficulties meeting certain expenses [23]. However, it is possible that individuals with similar income or financial hardship levels have different measures of financial satisfaction due to, for example, expectations, and it may be difficult to determine what reference group the individuals use when trying to match such expectations. In this research, we use financial satisfaction as the measure for subjective financial status. One merit of using satisfaction as the measure of subjective financial status is that it (at least partially) mitigates the problem caused by people using different reference groups when assessing their social position. Wolff et al[15] demonstrated that the relationship between subjective SES and SRH may be sensitive to the reference group used when measuring a person's relative standing. A person may have a lower SES rank when a more distal reference group (such as "others in the society") is used, but he may not necessarily be dissatisfied with it since those at the top positions in the society may simply be perceived as unattainable by the majority of the population and hence this does not necessarily lead to distress. In other words, a low rank using a distal reference group may not necessarily mean a low rank when the reference group is narrowed. Using satisfaction on the other hand, may reflect more of such distress if it is present.
While many studies have noted that objective and subjective SES in themselves are not mutually exclusive pathways and may be inter-related [2, 24], few research studies have specifically tested the interactive effect between the two. Poor health conditions are normally more prevalent in the lower socioeconomic groups, and hence it may be hypothesized that a lower subjective SES (such as poorer perceived financial status) may act as either a mediating factor with respect to the SES-health gradient or interact with the SES variables to produce poor health. This phenomenon has been shown to occur with work stress, where the prevalence of poor health was found to be highest among those with high work stress and poor SES [1]. Similarly, for subjective financial status, it is possible that this measure does not work independently of objective SES measures such as educational attainment when affecting an individual's health. This issue is particularly important for the elderly since it is more difficult to obtain a suitable SES measure for this subgroup, and a complete measure may include both objective and subjective measures of SES. In this study, we explored the hypothesis that there is an interaction between subjective financial satisfaction and education and that this is associated with health outcomes. We tested this association with respect to three health measures, namely self-rated health (SRH), depressive symptoms, and mortality among the elderly in Taiwan. These three health measures have been shown to be related to traditional measures of SES; specifically, individuals with poor SES are more likely to have poorer SRH, higher depressive symptoms and a higher risk of mortality [2530].

Methods

Data

This research uses data from the Survey of Health and Living Status of the Middle Age and Elderly in Taiwan (SHLS) conducted by the Bureau of Health Promotion, Department of Health in Taiwan. The SHLS is a 15-year longitudinal survey based on a national representative sample that was initiated in 1989. In 1989, the SHLS started with 4,049 respondents (response rate = 91.8%) aged 60 and above. The age range of this sample was 60 to 96 years, with those aged between 60 and 80 representing about 95% of the total sample. The SHLS was conducted by the Bureau of Health Promotion under the Department of Health in Taiwan. The sampling frame was designed to ensure that the sample is representative of the elderly in the country. A detailed description of the sampling method can be found elsewhere [31]. Information was collected by face-to-face interview, and the survey consists of a rich dataset on the elderly in Taiwan; it includes demographics, socioeconomics, life style, as well as health status. Starting in 1996, the survey included the CES-D (10 items) questions and hence information on depressive symptoms was available for waves 1996, 1999 and 2003. These three waves were used for the analysis. Since the initiation of the survey (n = 4049), 1,047 subjects had died by 1996 and of the remaining 3002 subjects in 1996, 333 did not complete the survey (response rate = 88.9%). For the remaining 2669 subjects, 282 had at least one missing value for the variables used (as described below), and this left us with 2,387 subjects for analysis. Among the 282 subjects, those with missing values for financial satisfaction tended to have lower education (p < 0.01). This should be taken into consideration when interpreting the results. The descriptions of the variables are given below.

Socioeconomic measures

Education

Educational attainment was used as a measure of objective SES. Since the respondents were the elderly persons, it is illogical to use income or occupation as an objective SES measure since a large proportion of the respondents would be retired. In terms of education variable, the respondents were grouped into "illiterate", "literate but with no formal education", "primary school", "junior high school", "senior high school and "college or above", and this assignment was assumed to be unchanged across the waves, which is be a reasonable assumption for the elderly.

Subjective financial status

For this research we use the following. The respondents were asked the question "generally speaking, are you satisfied with your financial status?" and the respondent was able to choose from "Very satisfied", "Satisfied", "Average", "Unsatisfied", "Very unsatisfied". To ensure that there were sufficient respondents within each group, we combine those who responded "Unsatisfied" with those who responded "Very unsatisfied". This is a time-varying variable such that the respondent may report different level of satisfaction at each wave and was treated as such during the various regression analyses.

Other control variables

All regressions were controlled for respondent's marital status and ethnicity (Fukien, Hakka, Mainlander, and other). Fukien, Hakka and Mainlander represent the three major ethnicities in Taiwan. Other control variables included whether the subject is a current smoker, and the number of persons living together in a household.

Health measures

Three health measures were included in the analysis, namely self-rated health (SRH), depression, and mortality. For SRH, each respondent was asked the question " How would you rate your general health?" and the respondent could choose from "very good", "good", "fair", poor" and "very poor". This question was asked for all waves and was treated as time-variant. For each wave, SRH was dichotomized into those who reported reduced health (SRH not equal to "very good" or "good") and those who did not; this approach is consistent with previous research [1, 32]. Sensitivity analysis was done using two other cut-off points to categorize those who reported reduced health, namely SRH not equal to "very good" alone and SRH not equal to either "very good", "good" or "average". Depression symptoms were measured using the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale (CES-D, 10-item) score. The presence of depressive symptom was identified when the score was equal to or greater than 10 [33, 34]. Finally, mortality was also used as one of the health outcomes.

Statistical analysis

Sample characteristics are presented as percentages for categorical variables and as means for continuous variables (Table 1). Chi-square and ANOVA were used to determine significant differences in trends across the waves for the categorical and continuous variables, respectively. Bivariate analysis for SES and each of the three measures of health are presented in Table 2. The generalized estimation equation (GEE) was used to estimate the models for SRH and depressive symptoms, and Cox regression was used to estimate the impact of SES on mortality. The main effects of the three SES measures on each of the health outcome are presented in Table 3. Finally, we estimate the interactive effect between financial satisfaction and the education, which is shown in Table 4. We also performed statistical tests for the interaction effects, as well as the simple effect for financial satisfaction at each level of education.
Table 1
Sample characteristics for the survived (mean or % by wave)
Year
1996
1999
2003
p a
N
2387
2022
1482
**
Age (mean)
73.8+-5.0
76.4+-4.8
79.6+-4.1
 
Sex (male, %)
56.1
55.1
54.5
 
Education (%)
    
   Illiterate
36.0
36.6
34.0
 
   Literate with no formal education
8.4
8.4
8.2
 
   Primary school
34.0
34.0
34.9
 
   Junior high school
9.5
9.5
9.8
 
   Senior high school
6.0
6.0
7.0
 
   College or above
5.5
5.5
6.2
 
Subjective financial status (%)
   
**
   Very satisfied
7.5
6.9
5.7
 
   Satisfied
33.4
32.0
41.4
 
   Average
43.3
40.9
33.5
 
   Dissatisfied
15.9
20.3
19.3
 
Self-rated health
   
**
   Very good
12.2
9.2
7.9
 
   Good
19.4
19.4
21.0
 
   Faire
34.2
34.4
32.1
 
   Poor
28.9
29.9
32.5
 
   Very poor
5.4
7.1
6.6
 
CESD-10 score
6.5+-6.5
11.0+-3.7
10.5+-3.5
**
a Chi-squared (ANOVA)test for count (continuous) variables for difference in trends across the waves, ** = p < 0.01
Table 2
Distribution of poor self-rated health, CES-D score and mortality by indicators of SES and financial satisfaction
 
Poor self-rated health (%)
Depression (mean)
Mortality (%)
 
1996
1999
2003
1996
1999
2003
1996
1999
2003
Education (%)
         
   Illiterate
76.8
81.0
78.6
8.4
11.6
11.3
17.75
13.8
24.5
   Literate with no formal education
68.7
70.9
64.5
6.9
11.1
10.8
12.0
12.9
20.6
   Primary school
70.0
70.5
70.8
5.6
10.7
10.4
11.26
11.9
23.0
   Junior high school
55.3
57.7
66.2
4.8
10.3
9.7
8.23
6.8
15.7
   Senior high school
49.7
53.7
59.2
4.1
10.2
9.6
6.13
11.9
17.2
   College or above
42.0
55.7
62.0
4.4
10.0
9.1
10.2
9.9
19.6
Subjective financial status (%)
         
   Very satisfied
46.1
40.0
47.1
3.8
9.4
9.8
9.4
9.6
15.1
   Satisfied
58.2
60.4
65.3
4.5
10.1
9.7
10.4
11.3
21.2
   Average
74.1
77.8
76.7
6.5
11.2
10.8
12.4
12.5
22.8
   Dissatisfied
85.2
85.9
81.1
12.1
12.4
12.1
13.7
14.3
24.6
Mean CES-D score
         
Number of subjects: 1996 = 2387, 1999 = 2022, 2003 = 1482 (decreases due to death or loss during follow-up)
Table 3
Main effects of education and financial satisfaction on health.
 
SRH
Depressive symptoms
Mortality
 
OR
95 C.I.
OR
95 C.I.
OR
95 C.I.
Education
      
   Illiterate (base)
      
   Literate with no formal education
0.7
(0.5 1.0)
0.8
(0.6 1.0)
0.8
(0.6 1.1)
   Primary school
0.8
(0.6 1.0)
0.7
(0.6 0.9)
0.9
(0.7 1.0)
   Junior high school
0.6
(0.5 0.8)
0.7
(0.5 0.8)
0.6
(0.4 0.9)
   Senior high school
0.5
(0.3 0.6)
0.7
(0.5 0.9)
0.8
(0.6 1.2)
   College or above
0.5
(0.3 0.7)
0.6
(0.4 0.8)
0.8
(0.6 1.2)
Financial satisfaction
      
   Very satisfied (base)
      
   Satisfied
2.2
(1.7 2.8)
1.1
(0.9 1.4)
1.4
(1.0 2.0)
   Average
4.0
(3.1 5.2)
1.7
(1.3 2.1)
1.5
(1.0 2.1)
   Dissatisfied
5.3
(3.8 7.2)
4.2
(3.2 5.4)
1.6
(1.1 2.3)
All models were adjusted for baseline age, sex, marital status, ethnicity, smoking, and number of people living together.
Table 4
Interaction between education and financial satisfactiona
  
SRH
 
Depression
 
Mortality
 
  
OR
95 C.I.
p
OR
95 C.I.
p
HR
95 C.I.
p
Education*FS
          
Education
Financial satisfaction
         
College or above
   Very satisfied (1.1)
1.0
 
*
1.0
  
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (2.6)
4.2
(2.0 8.8)
 
1.7
(0.8 3.8)
 
1.1
(0.4 2.9)
 
 
   Average(1.5)
4.9
(2.2 10.9)
 
1.9
(0.8 4.4)
 
1.0
(0.3 3.1)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(0.6)
6.4
(2.4 17.1)
 
2.7
(1.0 7.3)
 
1.5
(0.5 5.0)
 
Senior high school
   Very satisfied (0.9)
1.0
 
*
1.0
  
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (2.8)
1.8
(1.0 3.4)
 
1.3
(0.6 2.6)
 
2.1
(0.6 7.2)
 
 
   Average(1.7)
2.5
(1.3 5.0)
 
1.9
(0.9 4.0)
 
2.2
(0.6 7.7)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(0.8)
3.2
(1.4 7.4)
 
2.4
(1.0 5.7)
 
1.7
(0.4 7.7)
 
Junior high school
   Very satisfied (0.8)
1.0
 
**
1.0
 
*
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (4.0)
1.8
(0.9 3.3)
 
0.8
(0.4 1.6)
 
2.2
(0.7 7.2)
 
 
   Average (3.4)
3.4
(1.8 6.4)
 
0.8
(0.4 1.5)
 
1.4
(0.4 4.8)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(1.0)
4.4
(1.9 10.0)
 
2.1
(0.9 4.5)
 
1.7
(0.4 7.4)
 
Primary school
   Very satisfied (2.3)
1.0
 
**
1.0
 
**
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (12.6)
1.8
(1.2 2.6)
 
0.8
(0.5 1.2)
 
1.5
(0.8 2.7)
*
 
   Average(13.6)
3.2
(2.2 4.6)
 
1.3
(0.9 1.9)
 
1.7
(0.9 3.1)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(5.5)
5.1
(3.2 7.9)
 
3.3
(2.1 5.0)
 
2.2
(1.2 4.1)
 
Literate with no formal education
   Very satisfied (0.48)
1.0
 
**
1.0
 
**
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (2.4)
1.3
(0.6 3.0)
 
1.1
(0.5 2.8)
 
2.6
(0.6 11.4)
 
 
   Average(3.8)
3.3
(1.5 7.3)
 
2.0
(0.9 4.9)
 
2.0
(0.5 8.6)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(1.7)
5.5
(2.2 13.9)
 
5.2
(2.0 13.1)
 
3.1
(0.7 13.7)
 
Illiterate
   Very satisfied (1.5)
1.0
 
**
1.0
 
**
1.0
  
 
   Satisfied (10.4)
1.7
(1.0 2.6)
 
1.9
(1.1 3.1)
 
1.0
(0.5 1.8)
 
 
   Average(15.9)
3.3
(2.1 5.2)
 
3.0
(1.8 5.0)
 
1.1
(0.6 1.9)
 
 
   Dissatisfied(8.6)
6.0
(3.6 10.1)
 
8.3
(4.9 14.0)
 
1.0
(0.6 1.8)
 
Overall test of interaction (p-value)
 
>0.05
 
<0.01
 
>0.05
 
aAll models were adjusted for baseline age, sex, marital status, ethnicity, smoking, and number of people living together. All regressions also include the main effect of education.
bPercentage of observations per category.
**p<0.01, *p < 0.05 for tests of simple effects of financial satisfaction at each level of education.

Results

Table 1 shows the sample characteristics at each wave. The distribution of SRH, CES-D score and mortality relative to the three SES measures are presented in Table 2. A gradient of SES can be observed, such that, for all waves, those with a higher education level had a lower rate of reduced health response, a lower CES-D score and a lower mortality. A similar trend was observed for the financial satisfaction variable.
The results from the regression analyses are presented in Table 3. First, education has a positive effect on all health measures. For the base models (models without interactions), those with higher education (compared with illiterate) had a lower odds ratio for reporting reduced health. Those with a college degree or above, for example, had an odds ratio of 0.5 (p < 0.01) for reporting reduced health. A similar effect was observed for depressive symptoms. Compared with those with the lowest level of education, all other groups had lower odds ratios of depressive symptoms, ranging from OR = 0.6 ~ 0.8 (p < 0.05). For mortality, again, those in the higher educational level groups had lower odds ratio for mortality compared with those who were illiterate, with a slight U-shaped hazard ratios (HRs) across the groups, although this was only statistically significant for the junior high school group (HR = 0.6, p < 0.01).
Turning now to financial satisfaction, the effect of this variable on the health measures was statistically significant for all health measures. For the base models, those who reported poorer financial satisfaction had higher odds ratios for reporting reduced health compared with those who reported "very satisfied" (OR ranging from 2.2~5.3, p < 0.01). For all SRH models, the results were robust in terms of the different cut-off points for categorizing those who reported reduced health (data not shown). Similarly, those who reported "average" and "dissatisfied" for financial satisfaction were much more likely to report depressive symptoms (OR = 1.7 and 4.2, p < 0.01, respectively) compared with those who reported "very satisfied". A similar pattern was found for mortality, with those who reported poorer financial satisfaction (relative to those reported "very satisfied") having a higher HR for mortality, though only marginal statistical significance was observed.
The interaction between the subjective financial satisfaction and education are reported in Table 4. Statistical tests revealed the presence of interactions between the two SES variables for depressive symptom (p < 0.01). In terms of the effect on financial satisfaction, for SRH, the odds ratios of poor SRH were found to be consistently higher among those characterized as having lower financial satisfaction for all education groups, however, with a slight U-shaped ORs observed along the education groups. Among those who were dissatisfied with their financial position, the highest odds ratios were observed for the highest (OR = 6.4, p < 0.01) and lowest (OR = 6.0, p < 0.01) education categories compared with those in the "very satisfied" group. For depressive symptoms, higher ORs were observed consistently for those who were dissatisfied with their financial position among the lower education groups. For example, compared with those who were "very satisfied" within the highest education group, those who were dissatisfied with their financial position has an OR of 2.7 for having depressive symptoms within the same education group, and is statistically insignificant (p > 0.05). The corresponding OR for those who were illiterate were 8.3 (p < 0.01). Statistical tests also revealed that significant interaction effects were present for depressive symptoms. No significant simple effect for financial satisfaction was observed for mortality.

Discussion

While the relationship between traditional SES and health has a much longer history, subjective SES has begun to gain ground when explaining health disparities over the past decade. In many cases, the effect of subjective SES persists even after controlling for objective SES[10]. We have demonstrated here that the poorest health effect occurred in the lower financial satisfaction and lower education groups for depressive symptoms. The results from this research suggest that, while subjective financial position may contribute to the SES-health gradient, its effect may be particularly harmful in terms of having higher depressive symptoms among those who have a lower education level.
We found that the interaction effect was only statistically significant for depressive symptoms and not for SRH and mortality. First, it is possible that those in the lower educational rank had fewer resources when coping with the stress from poor financial satisfaction, and this may lead to depressive symptoms for the elderly as financial stress is one of the important factors leading to depression for this group[33]. It has also been found that those with lower education experience lower life satisfaction in general [35], and thus this may enhance the psychological stress associated with lower education. Secondly, it is also possible that what comprises "financial satisfaction" may differ between those with high and low educational attainment, and the factors associated with financial dissatisfaction for the lower educated individuals may be associated with higher depressive symptoms. For example, those with lower education may be dissatisfied with their financial positions due to actual financial hardship, and financial hardship is closely associated with depressive symptoms[21]. Another possible explanation is that there may be different time lags associated with the impact of SES on different health measures. More specifically, the impact of SES on depressive symptoms may be quicker than SRH and mortality. Future studies can test whether this mechanism is true for the elderly.
This research has several merits and limitations. Studies investigating the relationship between subjective financial position and health rarely compared results among different health measures. One of the merits of this research is that we have incorporated three health measures and we found different interactive effects among these different measures. The results of this research should also be viewed in light of a number of limitations. Firstly, the insignificance of the interaction terms for the SRH and mortality model may be due to sample attrition and because those who would/could not complete the survey or those who had missing values were excluded. In addition, if assuming those belonged to lower SES groups had higher probability of missing responses, then underestimation of the negative impact of lower SES may be suspected. Secondly, we could not include various other factors that may cause poor financial satisfaction. For example, the amount of savings that an individual has will be important when determining a person's financial satisfaction, especially during old age.
In contrast to Macleod et al's [17] results, where they found that perceived work-place status was only weakly associated with mortality, we found that a perceived poor financial satisfaction had a (marginal) significant main effect on mortality, but had no interaction effect with education, and this may be caused by the different measures of subjective socioeconomic position for the two studies as well as the difference in the mean age of the samples. In our case, financial satisfaction may be correlated with actual financial resource availability and hence medical care utilization, which is particularly important for the elderly. The interaction effects we found for depressive symptoms in this research are in line with the argument that objective and subjective SES do not involve mutually exclusive pathways and may be interrelated [2, 24]. Recently, a number of studies have investigated whether SES disparities in health reduce as people become older [3639]. In this context, we found that the effect of both SES measures persists in our sample (as measured by the main effects of the SES variables), which is a sample of elderly persons, and this is true, for all three health outcomes.
We found a U-shaped gradient in mortality across education groups, with the lowest HRs observed for the junior high school group. This probably was due to the fact that the majority of the elderly investigated in this study had a low level of education, and those with an educational level higher than junior high school may have had special characteristics that were not taken into account during the analysis (for example, working extremely hard and hence had a slightly poorer health).
Previous research has found depressive symptoms to be associated with self-rated health (15). To test whether having depressive symptom acted as a mediating factor in explaining the relationship between subjective financial status and SRH, we re-estimated the models for SRH including depressive symptom (CES-D score > = 10) as an independent variable (table not shown). The results showed that in both the baseline and interaction models, the ORs for having depressive symptoms turned out to be larger than 1 and statistically significant. In the baseline and interaction models, the ORs for having depressive symptoms on reporting reduced health were 1.54 (p < 0.01) and 2.1 (p < 0.01), respectively. In both models, the pattern of the relationship between financial satisfaction and SRH remain unchanged. Having depressive symptom thus may partially, though not fully, act as a mediating factor for the relationship between financial satisfaction and SRH.
It should be noted that subjective financial satisfaction captures only a specific part of what is termed "subjective SES", and there are many other aspects of subjective SES that have not been investigated in this research. For example, the previously mentioned ten rung ladder question (asking respondent to place themselves in their social position) may capture very different information from the subjective financial satisfaction variable used in this research.
The financial satisfaction used in this research may be influenced by many factors not explored in this study. A future question thus is what are the factors affecting a person's subjective financial satisfaction and to what degree does this related to the person's actual income level. In addition, it is important to determine the mechanisms through which the subjective financial position of a person affects their health. It should also be noted that financial satisfaction may or may not be directly related to a person's education. Obviously, it is very possible that those with higher education earn more and hence are more likely to be financially satisfied. On the other hand, it is also possible that a person with high education being dissatisfied with his financial position simply due to the fact that he is relatively less well off financially, rather than being actually deprived in any material way. If the assessment of subjective position systematically differs across objective SES measures, than the differences in the distributions of objective SES measures across samples could lead to differences in the impact of subjective assessments.

Conclusions

Previous research has shown that a subjective perception of an individual's financial position is important to determining health status. However, although poor financial satisfaction was found to be related to poorer health, the strongest association for this effect was observed among those with low educational attainment, and this is particularly significant for depressive symptoms. Subjective financial status among the elderly thus should not be explored separately from traditional measures of SES. Future research can test whether other aspects of subjective SES (apart from subjective financial satisfaction) also interact with objective SES.

Acknowledgements

This research is partially sponsored by a grant from Ministry of Education, aim for the Top University Plan
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

CP was responsible for data analysis and drafted the manuscript, NH and GT designed the study and helped in drafting the manuscript, YC helped in designing the study and finalized the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Wege N, Dragano N, Erbel R, Jockel KH, Moebus S, Stang A, et al: When does work stress hurt? Testing the interaction with socioeconomic position in the Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008, 62 (4): 338-41. 10.1136/jech.2007.062315.CrossRefPubMed Wege N, Dragano N, Erbel R, Jockel KH, Moebus S, Stang A, et al: When does work stress hurt? Testing the interaction with socioeconomic position in the Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008, 62 (4): 338-41. 10.1136/jech.2007.062315.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Adler N: When one's main effect is another's error: material vs. psychosocial explanations of health disparities. A commentary on Macleod et al., "is subjective social status a more important determinant of health than objective social status? Evidence from a prospective observational study of Scottish men". Soc Sci Med. 2005, 61 (9): 1916-1929. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.009. Soc Sci Med. 2006 Aug;63(4):846-50; discussion 51-7CrossRef Adler N: When one's main effect is another's error: material vs. psychosocial explanations of health disparities. A commentary on Macleod et al., "is subjective social status a more important determinant of health than objective social status? Evidence from a prospective observational study of Scottish men". Soc Sci Med. 2005, 61 (9): 1916-1929. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.009. Soc Sci Med. 2006 Aug;63(4):846-50; discussion 51-7CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Manuck SB, Phillips JE, Gianaros PJ, Flory JD, Muldoon MF: Subjective Socioeconomic Status and Presence of the Metabolic Syndrome in Midlife Community Volunteers. Psychosom Med. 2009 Manuck SB, Phillips JE, Gianaros PJ, Flory JD, Muldoon MF: Subjective Socioeconomic Status and Presence of the Metabolic Syndrome in Midlife Community Volunteers. Psychosom Med. 2009
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Epel O, Kaplan G: Can subjective and objective socioeconomic status explain minority health disparities in Israel?. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 69 (10): 1460-7. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.028.CrossRefPubMed Baron-Epel O, Kaplan G: Can subjective and objective socioeconomic status explain minority health disparities in Israel?. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 69 (10): 1460-7. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.028.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Suchday S, Chhabra R, Wylie-Rosett J, Almeida M: Subjective and objective measures of socioeconomic status: predictors of cardiovascular risk in college students in Mumbai, India. Ethn Dis. 2008, 18 (2 Suppl 2): S2-235-7. SpringPubMed Suchday S, Chhabra R, Wylie-Rosett J, Almeida M: Subjective and objective measures of socioeconomic status: predictors of cardiovascular risk in college students in Mumbai, India. Ethn Dis. 2008, 18 (2 Suppl 2): S2-235-7. SpringPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Demakakos P, Nazroo J, Breeze E, Marmot M: Socioeconomic status and health: the role of subjective social status. Soc Sci Med. 2008, 67 (2): 330-40. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.038.CrossRefPubMed Demakakos P, Nazroo J, Breeze E, Marmot M: Socioeconomic status and health: the role of subjective social status. Soc Sci Med. 2008, 67 (2): 330-40. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.038.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ostrove JM, Adler NE, Kuppermann M, Washington AE: Objective and subjective assessments of socioeconomic status and their relationship to self-rated health in an ethnically diverse sample of pregnant women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19 (6): 613-8. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.613.CrossRefPubMed Ostrove JM, Adler NE, Kuppermann M, Washington AE: Objective and subjective assessments of socioeconomic status and their relationship to self-rated health in an ethnically diverse sample of pregnant women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19 (6): 613-8. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.613.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE, Marmot MG: Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 56 (6): 1321-33. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00131-4.CrossRefPubMed Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE, Marmot MG: Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 56 (6): 1321-33. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00131-4.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Adler NE, Ostrove JM: Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999, 896: 3-15. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08101.x.CrossRefPubMed Adler NE, Ostrove JM: Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999, 896: 3-15. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08101.x.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR: Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19 (6): 586-92. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586.CrossRefPubMed Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR: Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19 (6): 586-92. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan GA, Pamuk ER, Lynch JW, Cohen RD, Balfour JL: Inequality in income and mortality in the United States: analysis of mortality and potential pathways. BMJ. 1996, 312 (7037): 999-1003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kaplan GA, Pamuk ER, Lynch JW, Cohen RD, Balfour JL: Inequality in income and mortality in the United States: analysis of mortality and potential pathways. BMJ. 1996, 312 (7037): 999-1003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Diez-Roux AV, Nieto FJ, Muntaner C, Tyroler HA, Comstock GW, Shahar E, et al: Neighborhood environments and coronary heart disease: a multilevel analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1997, 146 (1): 48-63.CrossRefPubMed Diez-Roux AV, Nieto FJ, Muntaner C, Tyroler HA, Comstock GW, Shahar E, et al: Neighborhood environments and coronary heart disease: a multilevel analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1997, 146 (1): 48-63.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Haan M, Kaplan GA, Camacho T: Poverty and health. Prospective evidence from the Alameda County Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1987, 125 (6): 989-98.PubMed Haan M, Kaplan GA, Camacho T: Poverty and health. Prospective evidence from the Alameda County Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1987, 125 (6): 989-98.PubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Weden MM, Carpiano RM, Robert SA: Subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics and adult health. Soc Sci Med. 2008, 66 (6): 1256-70. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.041.CrossRefPubMed Weden MM, Carpiano RM, Robert SA: Subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics and adult health. Soc Sci Med. 2008, 66 (6): 1256-70. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.041.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolff LS, Subramanian SV, Acevedo-Garcia D, Weber D, Kawachi I: Compared to whom? Subjective social status, self-rated health, and referent group sensitivity in a diverse US sample. Soc Sci Med. 2010, 70 (12): 2019-28. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.02.033.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wolff LS, Subramanian SV, Acevedo-Garcia D, Weber D, Kawachi I: Compared to whom? Subjective social status, self-rated health, and referent group sensitivity in a diverse US sample. Soc Sci Med. 2010, 70 (12): 2019-28. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.02.033.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Marmot MG, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Brunner E, Stansfeld S: Contribution of job control and other risk factors to social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet. 1997, 350 (9073): 235-9. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)04244-X.CrossRefPubMed Marmot MG, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Brunner E, Stansfeld S: Contribution of job control and other risk factors to social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet. 1997, 350 (9073): 235-9. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)04244-X.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Macleod J, Davey Smith G, Metcalfe C, Hart C: Is subjective social status a more important determinant of health than objective social status? Evidence from a prospective observational study of Scottish men. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 61 (9): 1916-29. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.009.CrossRefPubMed Macleod J, Davey Smith G, Metcalfe C, Hart C: Is subjective social status a more important determinant of health than objective social status? Evidence from a prospective observational study of Scottish men. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 61 (9): 1916-29. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.009.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Szanton SL, Allen JK, Thorpe RJ, Seeman T, Bandeen-Roche K, Fried LP: Effect of financial strain on mortality in community-dwelling older women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2008, 63 (6): S369-74.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Szanton SL, Allen JK, Thorpe RJ, Seeman T, Bandeen-Roche K, Fried LP: Effect of financial strain on mortality in community-dwelling older women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2008, 63 (6): S369-74.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Cantril H: The pattern of human concerns. 1965, New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press Cantril H: The pattern of human concerns. 1965, New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Veenstra G: Social status and health: absolute deprivation or relative comparison, or both?. Health Sociology Review. 2005, 14 (2): 121-34.CrossRef Veenstra G: Social status and health: absolute deprivation or relative comparison, or both?. Health Sociology Review. 2005, 14 (2): 121-34.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Butterworth P, Rodgers B, Windsor TD: Financial hardship, socio-economic position and depression: results from the PATH Through Life Survey. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 69 (2): 229-37. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.008.CrossRefPubMed Butterworth P, Rodgers B, Windsor TD: Financial hardship, socio-economic position and depression: results from the PATH Through Life Survey. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 69 (2): 229-37. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.008.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmerman FJ, Katon W: Socioeconomic status, depression disparities, and financial strain: what lies behind the income-depression relationship?. Health Econ. 2005, 14 (12): 1197-215. 10.1002/hec.1011.CrossRefPubMed Zimmerman FJ, Katon W: Socioeconomic status, depression disparities, and financial strain: what lies behind the income-depression relationship?. Health Econ. 2005, 14 (12): 1197-215. 10.1002/hec.1011.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Chou KL, Chi I: Financial strain and life satisfaction in Hong Kong elderly Chinese: moderating effect of life management strategies including selection, optimization, and compensation. Aging Ment Health. 2002, 6 (2): 172-7. 10.1080/13607860220126781.CrossRefPubMed Chou KL, Chi I: Financial strain and life satisfaction in Hong Kong elderly Chinese: moderating effect of life management strategies including selection, optimization, and compensation. Aging Ment Health. 2002, 6 (2): 172-7. 10.1080/13607860220126781.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE: Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status?. Psychosom Med. 2005, 67 (6): 855-61. 10.1097/01.psy.0000188434.52941.a0.CrossRefPubMed Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE: Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status?. Psychosom Med. 2005, 67 (6): 855-61. 10.1097/01.psy.0000188434.52941.a0.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Laaksonen M, Rahkonen O, Martikainen P, Lahelma E: Socioeconomic position and self-rated health: the contribution of childhood socioeconomic circumstances, adult socioeconomic status, and material resources. Am J Public Health. 2005, 95 (8): 1403-9. 10.2105/AJPH.2004.047969.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Laaksonen M, Rahkonen O, Martikainen P, Lahelma E: Socioeconomic position and self-rated health: the contribution of childhood socioeconomic circumstances, adult socioeconomic status, and material resources. Am J Public Health. 2005, 95 (8): 1403-9. 10.2105/AJPH.2004.047969.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Parna K, Ringmets I: Comparison of socioeconomic differences in self-perceived health in Estonia and Finland. Scand J Public Health. 2010, 38 (2): 129-34. 10.1177/1403494809357259.CrossRefPubMed Parna K, Ringmets I: Comparison of socioeconomic differences in self-perceived health in Estonia and Finland. Scand J Public Health. 2010, 38 (2): 129-34. 10.1177/1403494809357259.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang JL, Schmitz N, Dewa C: Socioeconomic status and the risk of major depression: The Canadian National Populatoin Health Survey. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009 Wang JL, Schmitz N, Dewa C: Socioeconomic status and the risk of major depression: The Canadian National Populatoin Health Survey. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Mete C: Predictors of elderly mortality: health status, socioeconomic characteristics and social determinants of health. Health Econ. 2005, 14 (2): 135-48. 10.1002/hec.892.CrossRefPubMed Mete C: Predictors of elderly mortality: health status, socioeconomic characteristics and social determinants of health. Health Econ. 2005, 14 (2): 135-48. 10.1002/hec.892.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Bassuk SS, Berkman LF, Amick BC: Socioeconomic status and mortality among the elderly: findings from four US communities. Am J Epidemiol. 2002, 155 (6): 520-33. 10.1093/aje/155.6.520.CrossRefPubMed Bassuk SS, Berkman LF, Amick BC: Socioeconomic status and mortality among the elderly: findings from four US communities. Am J Epidemiol. 2002, 155 (6): 520-33. 10.1093/aje/155.6.520.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Martelin T: Mortality by indicators of socioeconomic status among the Finnish elderly. Soc Sci Med. 1994, 38 (9): 1257-78. 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90190-2.CrossRefPubMed Martelin T: Mortality by indicators of socioeconomic status among the Finnish elderly. Soc Sci Med. 1994, 38 (9): 1257-78. 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90190-2.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang CHA: The 1989 Survey of Health and Living Status in Taiwan: questionnaire and survey design. 1989, Ann Arbor, MI: Population Studies Center, University of Michigan.(Comparative study of the elderly in four Asian countries, research report no. 1) Chang CHA: The 1989 Survey of Health and Living Status in Taiwan: questionnaire and survey design. 1989, Ann Arbor, MI: Population Studies Center, University of Michigan.(Comparative study of the elderly in four Asian countries, research report no. 1)
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Monden CW, van Lenthe F, de Graaf ND, Kraaykamp G: Partner's and own education: does who you live with matter for self-assessed health, smoking and excessive alcohol consumption?. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57 (10): 1901-12. 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00055-8.CrossRefPubMed Monden CW, van Lenthe F, de Graaf ND, Kraaykamp G: Partner's and own education: does who you live with matter for self-assessed health, smoking and excessive alcohol consumption?. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57 (10): 1901-12. 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00055-8.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Lue BH, Chen LJ, Wu SC: Health, financial stresses, and life satisfaction affecting late-life depression among older adults: a nationwide, longitudinal survey in Taiwan. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2010, 50 (Suppl 1): S34-8. 10.1016/S0167-4943(10)70010-8.CrossRefPubMed Lue BH, Chen LJ, Wu SC: Health, financial stresses, and life satisfaction affecting late-life depression among older adults: a nationwide, longitudinal survey in Taiwan. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2010, 50 (Suppl 1): S34-8. 10.1016/S0167-4943(10)70010-8.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL: Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med. 1994, 10 (2): 77-84.PubMed Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL: Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med. 1994, 10 (2): 77-84.PubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Melin R, Fugl-Meyer KS, Fugl-Meyer AR: Life satisfaction in 18- to 64-year-old Swedes: in relation to education, employment situation, health and physical activity. J Rehabil Med. 2003, 35 (2): 84-90. 10.1080/16501970306119.CrossRefPubMed Melin R, Fugl-Meyer KS, Fugl-Meyer AR: Life satisfaction in 18- to 64-year-old Swedes: in relation to education, employment situation, health and physical activity. J Rehabil Med. 2003, 35 (2): 84-90. 10.1080/16501970306119.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandola T, Ferrie J, Sacker A, Marmot M: Social inequalities in self reported health in early old age: follow-up of prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2007, 334 (7601): 990-10.1136/bmj.39167.439792.55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Chandola T, Ferrie J, Sacker A, Marmot M: Social inequalities in self reported health in early old age: follow-up of prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2007, 334 (7601): 990-10.1136/bmj.39167.439792.55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Marmot MG, Shipley MJ: Do socioeconomic differences in mortality persist after retirement? 25 year follow up of civil servants from the first Whitehall study. BMJ. 1996, 313 (7066): 1177-80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marmot MG, Shipley MJ: Do socioeconomic differences in mortality persist after retirement? 25 year follow up of civil servants from the first Whitehall study. BMJ. 1996, 313 (7066): 1177-80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Huisman M, Kunst AE, Mackenbach JP: Socioeconomic inequalities in morbidity among the elderly; a European overview. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57 (5): 861-73. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00454-9.CrossRefPubMed Huisman M, Kunst AE, Mackenbach JP: Socioeconomic inequalities in morbidity among the elderly; a European overview. Soc Sci Med. 2003, 57 (5): 861-73. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00454-9.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat House JS, Lepkowski JM, Kinney AM, Mero RP, Kessler RC, Herzog AR: The social stratification of aging and health. J Health Soc Behav. 1994, 35 (3): 213-34. 10.2307/2137277.CrossRefPubMed House JS, Lepkowski JM, Kinney AM, Mero RP, Kessler RC, Herzog AR: The social stratification of aging and health. J Health Soc Behav. 1994, 35 (3): 213-34. 10.2307/2137277.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
When does poor subjective financial position hurt the elderly? Testing the interaction with educational attainment using a national representative longitudinal survey
verfasst von
Christy Pu
Nicole Huang
Gao-Jun Tang
Yiing-Jenq Chou
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2011
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2011
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-166

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2011

BMC Public Health 1/2011 Zur Ausgabe