Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function 2/2018

Open Access 22.09.2017 | Original Article

When three is greater than five: EEG and fMRI signatures of errors in numerical and physical comparisons

verfasst von: Ewa Beldzik, Aleksandra Domagalik, Magda Gawlowska, Tadeusz Marek, Justyna Mojsa-Kaja

Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function | Ausgabe 2/2018

Abstract

Unravelling the neural mechanisms, which determine performance accuracy, is one of the key concepts in cognitive neuroscience. When compared to correct responses, shorter reaction times are commonly observed behavioural feature of errors committed in typical conflict tasks. Yet, little is known about the origins of this phenomenon. In this study, EEG and fMRI experiments were conducted using the numerical version of the Stroop paradigm, which yielded unique behavioural outcomes. Particularly, errors in numerical comparison had shorter reaction times than correct trials, whereas physical comparison resulted in the opposite pattern. This criss–crossing interaction effect was used as a marker when exploring time-courses of brain activity. Group independent component analysis was applied to neurophysiological data and event-related analysis was conducted on the components’ time-courses. Results revealed one centro-parietal EEG component and one temporo-parietal fMRI neural network, which exhibited significant task and accuracy interactions. Showing linear increase that peaked right after the response onset, the activity of centro-parietal EEG component was linked to the decision variable signal, which reflects a process of accumulating evidence until reaching an action-triggering threshold. Both amplitude measurements and linear fits to the signal provided evidence for distinctive characteristics between numerical and physical comparisons, thereby explaining the behavioural outcomes: errors are committed due to accumulation of evidence in favour of the other (wrong) task instruction. The architecture of the temporo-parietal network, which comprises bilateral inferior temporal and intraparietal regions, is highly consistent with the recently established core “number network”. These findings link perceptual decisions with the generalized magnitude system and impart novel insights into the neural determinants of errors in humans.

Introduction

In the numerical version of the Stroop paradigm, participants compare simultaneously presented Arabic digits based either on their numerical (numerical task) or physical (physical task) dimension (Besner and Coltheart 1979; Henik and Tzelgov 1982). Assessing the physical features of digits is significantly faster than assessing their value (e.g. Kadosh et al. 2007; Szucs and Soltész 2007). Regardless of the task instructions, an incongruent trial consists of a pair of digits in which the numerically larger digit has a smaller font size (Fig. 1). Although size and numerical information are processed automatically and in parallel, they interact and evoke response competition (Henik and Tzelgov 1982; Kadosh et al. 2007; Szucs and Soltész 2007; Szűcs and Soltész 2008; Santens and Verguts 2011). As with other conflict tasks, this interference manifests in slower reaction times (RTs) and error rates compared to neutral and congruent trials. While there is a large body of work on number–size interaction, very little is known about the origins of errors in numerical and physical comparisons. Notably, none of the aforementioned studies reported the RTs of erroneous responses.
As is commonly observed in conflict tasks, RTs of errors are significantly shorter than RTs of hits (e.g. Gratton et al. 1988; Falkenstein et al. 1991; White et al. 2011). A simple explanation of this phenomenon is that an erroneous response is executed before the stimulus is fully processed (Pailing and Segalowitz 2004; Danielmeier et al. 2009). An elucidation was provided by studies using electromyography and lateralized readiness potentials (Gratton et al. 1988; Szűcs et al. 2009). According to them, early processing of task-irrelevant stimulus information overwhelms the relevant feature of the target and triggers activation of the incorrect motor response, resulting in a high likelihood of error commission. If this response competition is reduced and overcome, a delayed correct reaction is executed. Considering that incongruent trials in both numerical and physical Stroop tasks elicit such interference (Kadosh et al. 2007; Szucs and Soltész 2007), one could speculate that errors in both tasks have shorter RTs than hits. As will be discussed later, it is not the case.
Computational and neuroimaging studies have provided substantial insights into numerical cognition. In general, number processing consists of two steps: the transformation of an external sensory number input to an internal number representation, and the transformation of a numerical representation to a task-relevant decision before the execution of a motor response (Verguts and Fias 2008). According to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, perceptual encoding into a numerical representation occurs in the inferior temporal gyrus, which selectively responds to written Arabic numerals (Shum et al. 2013; Amalric and Dehaene 2016). The intraparietal sulcus (IPS) acts as a cortical hub to extract quantitative information to estimate magnitude, space, or time (Hubbard et al. 2005; Nieder and Dehaene 2009), all of which are necessary when making decisions. On the other hand, electroencephalography (EEG) studies have contributed a comprehensive overview of the precise timing of each processing stage. The numerical representation is evaluated until 200 ms after the presentation of digits, as marked by early temporo-occipital P1/N1 visual potentials (Pinel et al. 2001; Szũcs et al. 2007). Number–size interference can be observed later (~ 450 ms after stimulus onset) within the centro-parietal positivity (CPP), also referred to as P3b (Kadosh et al. 2007; Szucs and Soltész 2007; Szűcs and Soltész 2008, 2012; Beldzik et al. 2015a).
In our previous study, in which only the numerical task was implemented (Beldzik et al. 2015a), we applied independent component analysis (ICA) to EEG data and successfully isolated several functionally distinct neural sources including two lateral temporo-occipital components that showed early visual P1/N1 potentials, a fronto-central component explaining all variance of error-related negativity, and a centro-parietal component showing pronounced CPP. The analysis of response-aligned event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed that the CPP increases linearly before the response until it reaches its maximum just after response onset. Moreover, the slope of this increase was sensitive to all experimental conditions that vary in RT.
This effect can be attributed to the recently established decision variable (DV) signal. DV was first introduced by sequential sampling models that focused on explaining two-choice perceptual decisions (Smith and Ratcliff 2004; Bogacz et al. 2006). According to the models, noisy sensory evidence is accumulated in favour of a particular outcome upon reaching an action-triggering threshold. DV represents the accrual of priors, evidence, and value into a quantity. The model has proven extremely successful at accounting for performance on a variety of different cognitive tasks (Ratcliff and McKoon 2008). It was soon employed by neurobiologists and supported by empirical data from single-cell recordings in monkeys (Platt and Glimcher 1999; Gold and Shadlen 2007). According to these studies, the build-to-threshold DV dynamics are represented by neurons in the lateral part of IPS. The DV signal was later isolated in human EEG studies (O’Connell et al. 2012). The authors showed that DV is reflected by the build-up rate of the response-aligned CPP as it exhibited every aspect of the dynamics observed in single-neuron counterparts. It increases steadily with incoming evidence, peaks at the time of response execution, scales with target detection difficulty, and accounts for trial-to-trial variance in RT. Recently, Twomey et al. (2015) indicated that the commonly observed centro-parietal P3b potential exhibits the same accumulation characteristics as CPP, thus it encodes the DV signal. The authors pointed out that these effects observed at the slope of response-aligned P3b have been overlooked in previous EEG studies, possibly due to the spatiotemporal overlap with distinct frontal potentials. Indeed, as demonstrated by our previous study (Beldzik et al. 2015a), the fronto-central and centro-parietal independent sources explained over 80% of variance of the P3b potential. Once separated using ICA, the effects on the slope of CPP revealed the classic build-to-threshold dynamic.
In this study, which was a part of broader research project, numerical and physical Stroop tasks were implemented in separate experiments using EEG and fMRI. Both experimental sessions revealed an interesting and novel behavioural pattern of RT measurements. That is, errors committed in the numerical task were, as expected, faster than correct responses, whereas errors in the physical task showed the opposite pattern of slower RTs than hits. Thus, the goal of this study was to reveal the neural underpinnings of erroneous responses in the numerical Stroop paradigm using the temporal and spatial advantages of EEG and fMRI, respectively. Group ICA was applied to both EEG and fMRI data to separate EEG independent sources and identify the neural networks involved in the task. The unique pattern of RT measurements was used as a marker when exploring the time-courses of independent components. Based on the results from our previous study (Beldzik et al. 2015a), we hypothesise that the response-locked CPP is sensitive to the task and accuracy interactions. Particularly, we expect to find greater CPP amplitude before the response and smaller linear slope coefficient. Considering the compelling involvement of the IPS in numerical cognition (Nieder and Dehaene 2009) and its alleged locus of DV signal (Gold and Shadlen 2007), we hypothesise that this brain structure shows greater involvement in the longer taking numerical and physical decisions, regardless of its accuracy.

Materials and methods

Participants

Thirty-four participants (mean age of 23.4 ± 2.4 years, 17 females) met the inclusion criteria for MRI scanning and the experiment requirements: right-handedness, normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no physical and psychiatric disorders, being drug-free. They were informed about the procedure and goals of the study and gave their written consent. The study was approved by the Bioethics Commission at Jagiellonian University.

Experimental task

The numerical version of the Stroop paradigm, also known as the size congruity paradigm (Besner and Coltheart 1979), was prepared and generated using E-Prime 2.0 (©Psychology Software Tools). In the EEG experiment, the task was presented on a 17-inch screen (luminance 300 cd/m2, brightness level 70%) located approximately 80 cm from the eyes of participants. In the fMRI experiment, a mirror was placed on the head coil so participants could see the 32-inch screen (luminance 400 cd/m2, brightness level 50%) situated behind the scanner approximately 100 cm from the mirror. In the former experiment, a keypad was provided in front of the participants, whilst in the latter experiment two keypads were placed in the participants’ hands.
In the task, the stimuli were a pair of Arabic digits (from 1 to 9) of varying size (from 20 to 52 pt in steps of 4 pt) in a black Arial font on a light grey background (Fig. 1). In the numerical task, subjects were instructed to press a button with the left/right index finger if the digit on the left/right side had higher magnitude. In the physical task, they were instructed to indicate the digit with the greater font size. There were three congruence conditions: (1) congruent, when the numerically larger digit was physically larger; (2) neutral, when the two digits were of the same physical size (numerical task) or the same two digits had a different size (physical task); (3) incongruent, when the numerically larger digit was physically smaller. One block comprised 160 trials, which were equally distributed between congruence condition as well as left/right correct responses. Stimuli were presented in a pseudorandom order to avoid carryover effects. At the beginning of each block, the instruction “numerical task” was presented. After 80 trials, the instruction “physical task” informed participants about the change of task. There were four blocks, each differing in the type of feedback provided after the response (words ‘Good!’, ‘Bad!’, ‘You win!’, ‘You lose!’ after correct or erroneous responses, respectively). However, feedback types were not the concern of this study, thus blocks were treated as equivalent sessions.
The stimulus was preceded by a blank screen for 300 ms. The stimulus then appeared for 350 ms and was followed by a blank screen during which the response was registered for 200 ms. Next, feedback or a black screen was presented for 900 ms and was followed by the fixation point (a hash symbol, #). The only difference in the task protocols between the EEG and fMRI experiments was the time intervals between the events. In the EEG experiment, the stimulus-feedback interval was on average 1000 ms (varying between 800 and 1200 ms), whereas the interval between feedback and the next stimulus was on average 1600 ms (varying between 1100 and 2100 ms). Together, there were 4150 ms on average between trials. In the fMRI experiment, the stimulus–feedback interval was set to a constant 250 ms, whereas the interval between feedback and next stimulus was on average 3400 ms (varying between 2000 and 4800 ms). Together, there were 5200 ms on average between trials.

Behavioural data analysis

The same behavioural analysis was applied to data from both experiments. Accuracy and reaction times (RT) were analysed in Statistica 12.0 (StatSoft Inc., OK, US). These measures were subjected to a 2 × 3 way repeated measure ANOVA with task and congruency conditions. To ensure a clear comparison between correct and erroneous responses, all further time course analyses were performed solely on incongruent trials. RTs for correct and erroneous responses in both tasks underwent a 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA.

EEG data acquisition and analysis

Dense-array EEG data (HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net, EGI System 300; Electrical Geodesic Inc., OR, USA) was collected from 256 channels (band-pass filtered at 0.01–100 Hz with a vertex electrode as a reference) at a sampling rate of 250 Hz and recorded with Net Station Software (Version 4.5.1, Electrical Geodesic Inc., OR, USA). Further offline data analysis was conducted with the open-source EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig 2004). Data were digitally filtered to remove frequencies below 0.5 Hz and above 35 Hz. Average reference was recomputed and bad channels were automatically removed by kurtosis measures with a threshold value of 20 standard deviations. The choice of the threshold was adjusted based on our previous study (Beldzik et al. 2015a) to ensure that only true outliers (usually 5% of channels) were automatically removed; for instance, electrodes not adhering to the scalp. Next, continuous data were visually inspected to remove channels or time epochs containing high amplitude, high-frequency muscle noise and other irregular artefacts. The removed channels were interpolated.
Independent component analysis at the individual level was used to remove artefacts from the data. Due to the large number of channels, decomposition of EEG data with the Infomax algorithm was preceded by Principle Component Analysis. Fifty ICs were obtained and visually inspected for each subject. According to the characteristic spatiotemporal pattern (Jung et al. 2000; Delorme and Makeig 2004), components recognized as blinks, heart rate, saccades, muscle artefacts, or bad channels were removed. Next, epochs were extracted from − 400 to 1200 ms with regard to stimulus presentation and were baseline corrected from − 100 to 0 ms. Thus, both stimulus-locked and response-locked ERPs had the same prestimulus baseline. This choice was determined by the fact that the responses in the numerical Stroop task occur within the same time range (300–700 ms) as the effects on ERP amplitudes (Szűcs and Soltész 2012; Beldzik et al. 2015a). In such cases, a pre-response baseline could considerably reduce the effects.
To obtain reliable and stable group ICA decomposition, all incongruent trials entered the group ICA using the EEGIFT toolbox (Eichele et al. 2011). Since the toolbox requires the same number of trials for each subject, we chose to remove the rearmost correct trials (as they were substantially more numbered than incorrect trials), so that the final trial count would be evened out to the lowest number in the sample (i.e. 191). The number of independent sources to extract was set to seven based on our previous study with the numerical task (Beldzik et al. 2015a). The stability of ICA decomposition was validated with ICASSO (Himberg et al. 2004), with 50 random initializations of the Infomax algorithm. Data were back-reconstructed using the moo-icar option, i.e. the multivariate objective optimization ICA with reference (Du and Fan, 2013). Since the reconstruction adds some noise to component activity, a 35 Hz low pass filter was reapplied to the time courses with the use of the EEGLab iirfilt() function. To estimate the location in the brain of each independent source, the scalp maps were imported to BESA 5.3 (MEGIS Software GmbH, Gräfelfing, Germany) and underwent Classical LORETA Analysis Recursively Applied (CLARA).
Component ERPs were extracted from − 100 to 700 ms with regard to stimulus presentation and from − 400 to 400 ms with regard to reaction occurrence. ERPs were obtained for 2 × 2 (accuracy × task) conditions to search for a neural measure that accounted for the behavioural pattern found here. Thus, besides a significant interaction, a significant post hoc test between correct and erroneous responses within each task was required. A repeated measures ANOVA was applied to point-by-point amplitudes and peak latencies within the time range of recognized ERP components. Specifically, the N1 was verified within windows ranging from 0 to 300 ms with regard to stimulus onset. The error-related negativity (ERN) was 0–150 ms with regard to response onset. Finally, the CPP was verified in the stimulus-locked window from 200 to 600 ms and the response-locked window from − 300 to 100 ms. To protect against Type-I errors, multiple time point comparisons were corrected with the false discovery rate (FDR) method.
To establish the build-up rate of CPP, which represents the accumulation, we measured the temporal slope of each condition in each subject’s response-locked waveforms (Murphy et al. 2015). Build-up rate was computed as the slope of a straight line fitted to the signal within the temporal window of extending length. That is, fitting was conducted in a stepwise fashion for time windows, which started at − 100, − 104, − 108,…, until − 400 ms and ended at the response onset. The coefficient of determinant, R 2, was obtained for each fit and averaged across subjects. Since R 2 represents the goodness of the fit, this way (1) the linearity of the slope can be verified (2) the start of the CPP build-up can be establish for each condition using maximal R 2 value measured among extending temporal windows. Thus, maximal R 2 value defined the final length of the window for each condition and final fit to the statistical testing. Both the slope coefficients and constant terms of each fit underwent statistical testing using a 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA.

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed using a 3 T scanner (Magnetom Skyra, Siemens) with a 20-channel head/neck coil. High-resolution, whole-brain anatomical images were acquired using a T1-MPRAGE sequence. A total of 176 sagittal slices were obtained (voxel size 1 × 1 × 1.1 mm3; TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, flip angle = 9°) for coregistration with the fMRI data. Functional T2*-weighted images were acquired using a whole-brain echo planar pulse sequence (EPI) with the following parameters: 3 mm isotropic voxel, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV 192 × 192 mm2, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, phase encoding A/P. The whole-brain image (cerebellum excluded) was covered with 35 axial slices, taken in an interleaved, ascending fashion. The acquisition time for each session was 15 min. Due to magnetic saturation effects, the first four volumes (dummy scans) of each session were discarded instantly.
The standard pre-processing procedure was applied using Analysis of Functional NeuroImage (AFNI) software (Cox 1996). Each 3D image was time-shifted so that the slices were aligned temporally. After head motion correction, the functional EPI data sets were coregistered to structural scans and smoothed using a full-width at a half maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel of 4 mm. Voxels with low-signal intensity located outside the brain were excluded from the functional images by a clipping function. During the scaling procedure, the percent signal change was calculated for each voxel. Anatomical and functional images were transformed into a coordinate system of Talairach space (Talairach and Tournoux 1988).
Next, group ICA was conducted using the GIFT toolbox (Calhoun et al. 2001). The estimation determining the number of components was performed using the minimum description length (MDL) criteria implemented in the software (Li et al. 2007). The stability of ICA decomposition was validated with ICASSO (Himberg et al. 2004), with 50 random initializations of the Infomax algorithm. In the case of insufficient stability scores, ICA was performed with the numbers neighbouring the number estimated with the MDL criteria. Data were back-reconstructed using the gica template option. The group average maps were inspected to identify and discard those ICs primarily associated with artefacts that represented signals from large vessels, ventricles, motion, and susceptibility. From the remaining ICs, time-courses were interpolated to 100 ms resolution and epochs were extracted − 1 to 12 s prior to the stimulus. These epochs were baseline corrected by subtracting the value at zero. Epochs were divided into 2 × 2 (accuracy × task) conditions and subjected to statistical testing. Specifically, values at the peak of hemodynamic responses underwent a 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA for each neural network. As with EEG data analyses, besides the significant interaction effect, a significant post hoc test between correct and erroneous responses within each task was required.

Results

Behavioural results

In general, participants committed 7.8% errors and 0.5% omissions in the EEG experiment, and 11.4% errors and 5.3% omissions in the fMRI experiment. The main effect of the congruence condition on both error rates and RTs confirmed the occurrence of conflict (Table 1). Since most of the errors were committed on incongruent trials, it was possible to base further behavioural, EEG, and fMRI data analyses solely on incongruent trials. The numerical task evoked more errors than the physical task (Table 2). RTs of correct and erroneous responses showed a significant interaction in both experiments (Fig. 2; EEG: F (1,33) = 161; p < 0.001; fMRI: F (1,33) = 295, p < 0.001). Particularly, errors in the physical task had longer RTs than correct trials, whereas errors in the numerical task showed the opposite pattern (Table 3). Moreover, significant and opposite pattern in RTs was observed when comparing hits to errors within each task.
Table 1
Behavioural results—all trials
Measure
Experiment
Congruent
Neutral
Incongruent
F (2,66)
p value
Error rate (% of all)
EEG
0.48 (0.08)
1.59 (0.13)
5.74 (0.24)
410.6
< 0.001
fMRI
1.24 (0.15)
2.50 (0.19)
7.62 (0.35)
466.3
< 0.001
RT of correct (ms)
EEG
326.2 (3.4)
345.4 (3.5)
365.7 (3.6)
394.5
< 0.001
fMRI
411.2 (3.5)
422.2 (3.8)
436.2 (3.9)
357.7
< 0.001
SEs across participants are provided in parentheses
Table 2
Error rate of incongruent trials (% of all)
Experiment
Num
Phys
t (33)
p value
EEG
3.9 (1.1)
1.8 (0.7)
10.3
< 0.001
fMRI
5.1 (1.8)
2.5 (0.9)
7.6
< 0.001
SEs across participants are provided in parentheses
Table 3
Reaction time of incongruent trials in ms
Experiment
Type
Numerical
Physical
Post hoc
EEG
Correct
400.7 (3.4)
330.6 (2.8)
p < 0.001
Errors
334.8 (3.3)
351.2 (5.7)
p < 0.001
Post hoc
p < 0.001
p < 0.001
 
fMRI
Correct
460.8 (2.7)
411.6 (3.2)
p < 0.001
Errors
411.8 (3.2)
422.6 (5.6)
p < 0.001
Post hoc
p < 0.001
p < 0.001
 
SEs across participants are provided in parentheses

EEG results

Seven independent sources were reliably obtained (component stability was above 0.98) with the group ICA. Each of the estimated EEG sources can be characterized by its topography (Fig. 3a), the constellation of underlying brain sources (Fig. 3b), and event-related time courses (Fig. 3c). Local maxima of independent component source reconstructions are listed in Table 4. Three of the components had clearly identifiable ERPs and matched the independent components obtained previously (Beldzik et al. 2015a). The fronto-central component with the source estimated in the medial frontal cortex showed early (~ 100 ms after the stimulus onset) negativity, which can be linked to the visual anterior N1 potential (Vogel and Luck 2000). The activity of this component in response-locked data revealed pronounced ERN and followed it positivity (Holroyd and Coles 2002). The inferior temporal component with the source estimated approximately in the right inferior temporal cortex and left insula manifested in early visual activity of temporo-occipital N1 potential (Pinel et al. 2001). No significant task and accuracy interactions were found within these aforementioned ERP components.
Table 4
Local maxima of EEG independent components’ source reconstructions reported in Talairach coordinates
EEG component
Region
Site
x
y
z
nAm/cm3
Fronto-central
Cingulate
R
3.5
− 23.8
24.9
0.55
Cerebellum
R
3.5
− 57.7
− 31.7
0.64
Inferior temporal
Posterior cingulate
R
3.5
− 64.5
30.7
0.55
Inferior temporal
R
38.5
− 38.1
− 10.6
0.55
Insula
L
− 38.5
− 10.1
2.6
0.28
Centro-parietal
Inferior parietal
R
31.5
− 38.1
17.5
0.39
Inferior parietal
L
− 31.5
− 38.1
16.9
0.22
Insula
L
− 38.5
− 10.6
− 3.2
0.25
Superior temporal
Superior temporal
R
38.5
− 51.8
16.4
0.34
Superior temporal
L
− 31.5
− 52.8
23.8
0.1
Occipital
M
− 3.5
− 79.4
2.6
0.18
Anterior cingulate
M
3.5
52.4
9.5
0.15
Middle temporal
Middle temporal
R
38.5
− 58.2
3.7
0.46
Medial frontal
M
− 3.5
53.4
9.5
0.36
Insular
Insula
L
− 31.5
− 30.7
10.1
0.23
Insula
R
38.5
− 23.8
10.1
0.24
Anterior cingulate
M
− 3.5
25.9
38.1
0.19
central
Thalamus
R
24.5
− 23.8
2.6
0.16
Insula
L
− 31.5
− 30.7
24.3
0.13
The centro-parietal component (Fig. 4a) with the source estimated in the bilateral inferior parietal cortices showed pronounced P3b/CPP potential (Polich 2007; Twomey et al. 2015). A significant interaction effect was found on the amplitudes within both stimulus-locked and response-locked ERPs (around 300 ms post stimuli and around −110 and − 18 ms prior to response; see Table 5 for details; Fig. 4a). Average R 2 values of the linear fit to the signal before the response determined the time window in which CPP increases linearly for each condition (Supp. Figure 1). Maximal R2 was found for windows beginning at −352 ms (R 2 = 0.78) for numerical correct, − 142 ms (R 2 = 0.91) for physical correct, − 164 ms (R 2 = 0.80) for numerical error, and − 344 ms (R 2 = 0.59) for physical error. Lower R 2 values for erroneous trials in comparison to correct ones reflect lower number of trials, which contributed to ERPs. The obtained slope coefficients and constant terms showed significant interaction effect between accuracy and task instructions (Table 5; Fig. 4b).
Table 5
Significant task and accuracy interactions of the centro-parietal EEG component
Measurement
ERP type
Time range (ms)
F (1,33)
p value
\(\eta_{p}^{2}\)
LSD Post hoc
Num: Cor vs. Err
Phys: Cor vs. Err
Cor: Num vs. Phys
Err: Num vs. Phys
Amp
Stim
276–376
22.3
0.001
0.40
0.025
0.001
0.001
0.021
Amp
Resp
− 132 to − 88
77.1
0.001
0.70
0.001
0.011
0.001
0.035
Amp
Resp
− 32 to − 4
19.6
0.001
0.37
0.018
0.001
0.009
0.002
a
Resp
157.3
0.001
0.83
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.001
b
Resp
57.0
0.001
0.63
0.011
0.001
0.015
0.001
Amp amplitude, a slope coefficient of the linear fit, b constant term of the linear fit, resp response-locked, stim stimulus-locked

fMRI results

Although MDL criteria estimated 25 components on average, ICA decomposition with a total of 29 proved most reliable (component stability was above 0.97). All activations are reported at the FDR-corrected threshold, p < 0.001. Based on the literature (Kelly et al. 2010; Varoquaux et al. 2010), 11 components were classified as neural networks (Fig. 5; Table 6). Three maps corresponded to visual areas. Two of them are commonly observed in resting state studies (Beckmann et al. 2005; Smith et al. 2009; Varoquaux et al. 2010): the medial visual network, which comprised extensive activation in the lingual gyrus, and the lateral visual network ranging from the bilateral middle occipital to the superior parietal cortices. Less commonly observed is the temporo-parietal network involving bilateral inferior temporal gyri and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). The executive control network usually includes the bilateral prefrontal cortex and the core regions of anterior cingulate and insular cortices (Smith et al. 2009; Domagalik et al. 2012). Here, the executive control and prefrontal networks were separated. In line with the study of Damoiseaux et al. (2008), the default mode network was divided into anterior and posterior parts. The left and right frontoparietal and the auditory networks are highly reproducible, as indicated by all the referenced studies. Lastly, the network comprising the precuneus (Domagalik et al. 2012) is less often observed. Only the temporo-parietal network achieved the established criteria of significant interaction [Fig. 6; F (1,33) = 44.1; p < 0.001; LSD post hocs: numerical (Cor vs. Err) p = 0.032, physical (Cor vs. Err) p < 0.001, correct (Num vs. Phys) p = 0.011, error (Num vs. Err) p < 0.001].
Table 6
Local maxima of fMRI independent components reported in Talairach coordinates
Network
Region
Site
x
y
z
T
Medial visual
Lingual
M
− 3
− 63
9
13.61
Lateral visual
Middle occipital
R
33
− 87
15
15.42
Middle occipital
L
− 24
− 93
15
11.75
Temporo-parietal
Intraparietal sulcus
R
42
39
60
9.61
Intraparietal sulcus
L
− 54
− 24
45
11.86
Inferior temporal
R
57
− 57
− 3
11.81
Inferior temporal
L
− 51
− 63
− 3
11.11
Posterior cingulate
M
0
− 54
33
13.82
Executive control
Medial frontal
M
0
57
15
13.96
Anterior insula
L
− 42
21
− 3
13.04
Anterior insula
R
51
24
0
12.52
Prefrontal
Middle frontal
R
51
42
18
17.03
Middle frontal
L
− 48
42
21
14.65
Default mode—anterior
Medial frontal
M
0
63
6
16.06
Posterior cingulate
M
0
− 51
18
14.87
Default mode—posterior
Superior frontal
M
− 3
36
57
10.60
Posterior midline
M
− 3
− 51
36
13.43
Middle temporal
L
− 54
− 60
30
13.36
Middle temporal
R
57
− 60
30
10.64
Frontoparietal left
Middle frontal
L
− 45
54
12
9.78
Superior parietal
L
− 21
− 75
51
8.50
Superior parietal
R
36
− 69
51
9.53
Frontoparietal right
Middle frontal
R
45
57
6
12.87
Superior parietal
R
39
− 66
51
9.53
Auditory
Insula
L
− 63
− 15
15
13.10
Insula
R
66
− 9
15
11.53
Precuneus
Precuneus
M
3
− 72
39
14.21
Inferior parietal
L
− 33
− 57
39
10.08

Discussion

In the study, we aimed to verify the neural substrates of correct and erroneous decisions in numerical and physical comparisons. To achieve this goal, we focused solely on incongruent trials as they yield the size congruity effect (Besner and Coltheart 1979) and trigger errors. The size congruity effect originates from the parallel processing of physical and semantic information in the brain, which interact and evoke response competition (Henik and Tzelgov 1982; Kadosh et al. 2007; Szucs and Soltész 2007; Szűcs et al. 2009). Here, we replicated a common finding that assessing the physical feature of the digits is faster than assessing their value (Fig. 2). Errors in the numerical task showed a typical-for-conflict task effect of shortened RTs in comparison with correct trials (Gratton et al. 1988; White et al. 2011). An interesting finding that has not been reported by any previous conflict study is the effect of longer RTs of errors in physical tasks compared to correct trials. This pattern of cross-shaped interaction was used as a marker for neural measurements to explore the origins of this phenomenon.
The EEG data were analysed using group ICA (Eichele et al. 2011) to separate functionally distinct neural sources. Decomposition of EEG data into functionally distinct independent sources was similar to our previous study (Beldzik et al. 2015a), including a centro-parietal component with a robust CPP potential. Although there was no effect on the latency of stimulus-locked CPP, the abrupt slope and sharp peak around the timing of response execution suggest that CPP is more accurately represented by an ERP average aligned to subject responses (Makeig and Onton 2009). In agreement to common findings of decreased P3b amplitudes for more difficult and longer-to-react conditions (West and Alain 2000; Szűcs and Soltész 2012), we found significant task and accuracy interaction within the stimulus-locked P3b/CPP potential (Fig. 4a). However, in light of the aforementioned methodological aspects, P3b/CPP amplitude decreases for more difficult conditions due to the temporal variability of the peak onset (Twomey et al. 2015). Thus, analysing the response-locked CPP is more accurate and informative approach.
As hypothesised, a significant interaction among task and accuracy conditions was found on the slope of response-aligned CPP around 110 ms before the response (Fig. 4a). In light of the interpretation of Twomey et al. (2015), this slope represents the build-up rate of accumulating evidence in favour of a particular outcome. To strengthen this association, we measured the build-up rate by fitting a straight line to the signal before the response onset separately for each condition (Murphy et al. 2015). The significant accuracy and task interaction of the slope coefficients confirmed that the build-up rates tracked the RT pattern of each condition (Fig. 4b). In addition, the constant terms showed the interaction effect, i.e. the value of CPP amplitude at the onset of the response. Due to the shared topography of the CPP signal, we cannot definitely distinguish between correct numerical and physical responses; however, these results indicate that each response has a characteristic build-up rate and action-triggering threshold. Following that logic, errors in the numerical task have the same build-up characteristic as hits in the physical task. This suggests that errors in the numerical task were committed due to accumulation of the wrong evidence. In other words, evidence was accumulated towards the decision of which digit was bigger in size rather than magnitude. Finally, the CPP build-up rate parameters of errors in the physical task indicate that, at least partially, subjects accumulate wrong evidence in favour of the numerically, instead of physically, greater digit. This potentially explains why errors in the physical task have longer RTs than hits.
The fMRI data were analysed in a similar manner to the EEG data. Group ICA was used to separate neural sources, event-related averaging was performed on components’ time-courses, and peaks of hemodynamic responses were searched for significant task and accuracy interaction. Eleven neural networks were obtained (Fig. 5), but only one showed the expected pattern of activity: greater activation for correct vs. error trials in the numerical task and greater activation for error vs. correct in the physical task. This network involves the bilateral inferior temporal gyrus as well as a bilateral cluster extending along the IPS to the postcentral gyrus; therefore, it was labelled temporo-parietal (Fig. 6a). The architecture of this component shows some similarities to the dorsal attention network (Fox et al. 2005; Visintin et al. 2015); however, the lack of frontal eye field inclusion speaks against this association.
More evident is a link to the recently established ‘number network’ (Amalric and Dehaene 2016; Nieder 2016), which comprises bilateral intraparietal, inferior temporal, and dorsal prefrontal sites. The network is activated by all domains of mathematics such as analysis, algebra, topology, and geometry (Amalric and Dehaene 2016). As indicated by studies using intracranial electrophysiological recordings in humans, the inferior temporal gyrus selectively responds to written Arabic numerals, but not to morphologically, semantically, or phonologically similar stimuli (Shum et al. 2013). IPS was activated during a variety of number-processing and calculation tasks implemented in fMRI (Pinel et al. 2004; Kadosh et al. 2005; Kaufmann et al. 2005) and is considered a key node for the representation of the semantic aspect of numerical quantity (Nieder and Dehaene 2009). Moreover, studies using single-neuron recordings in the IPS of monkeys have identified ‘number neurons’ which encode the number of elements in a set and its cardinality or numerosity, irrespective of stimulus modality (see a review by Nieder 2016). In brief, the involvement of the temporo-parietal network in the numerical Stroop paradigm is highly consistent with the literature.
Due to separate EEG and fMRI experiments, we were unable to couple independent components in the single-trial levels; however, partial consistency might be observed between EEG source location measurements (Fig. 3) and fMRI results (Fig. 6a). Particularly, the inferior temporal EEG component had the highest source probability in the right inferior temporal gyrus, whilst the centro-parietal EEG component had bilateral parietal distribution of the estimated source. Together, these results combine when and where the numerical processing occurs (Verguts and Fias 2008). The inferior temporal component with a source in the inferior temporal gyrus encodes numerical/physical sensory information into numerical representation (Shum et al. 2013; Amalric and Dehaene 2016), whereas the centro-parietal component with a source in IPS accumulates quantitative information into the DV until reaching an action-triggering threshold (Nieder and Dehaene 2009; O’Connell et al. 2012). The similar pattern of amplitude modulations by task and accuracy conditions in EEG and fMRI measurements supports this association. Interestingly, topography of the inferior temporal EEG component indicates right hemispheric dominance, whereas fMRI data show bilateral activations. The same discrepancy can be observed in the literature as some studies point to the right inferior temporal supremacy in response to selective numbers (Park et al. 2012; Shum et al. 2013), whereas others report bilateral activations in that brain region during numerical cognitive tasks (Pinel et al. 2004; Amalric and Dehaene 2016). Future studies, which implement simultaneous EEG and fMRI recordings, might consider verifying the link between these components and clarify the hemispheric inconsistency of the inferior temporal activations.
An interesting question remains regarding the nature of the DV signal. Does it reflect accumulation of the numerical quantity information (“seven is greater”) or spatial aim-of-reach information (“press left button”)? Several recent findings speak in favour of the latter perspective. First, Twomey et al. (2015) indicated that the DV signal is supramodal as it was detected during auditory and visual oddball tasks. Second, an fMRI study implementing a saccadic task revealed that, as opposed to regions involved in conflict resolution, IPS exhibits strong trial-by-trial variations with RTs (Domagalik et al. 2014; Beldzik et al. 2015b). Third, de Lafuente et al. (2015) showed using single-neuron recordings in monkeys that when decisions are communicated by hand movements, the reach-related neurons from the medial IPS exhibit a gradual modulation of their firing rates consistent with the representation of an evolving DV. When decisions are communicated by eye movements, neurons in the lateral IPS represent the accumulation of evidence bearing on the potential targets of saccades.
On the other hand, quantity and space are part of a generalized magnitude system in which “two kinds of quantity must be combined to determine behaviourally important decision variables” (Gallistel and Gelman 2000; Walsh 2003). The numerical–spatial interactions arise from common parietal circuits for attention to external space and internal representations of numbers (Hubbard et al. 2005). This perspective is also supported with studies applying single-neuron recordings in monkeys. Tudusciuc and Nieder (2007) found anatomically intermingled single neurons in the IPS of monkeys which encode continuous spatial, discrete numerical, or both types of quantities. This and several subsequent studies have provided evidence that the IPS encodes numerosity as a perceptual category, regardless of behavioural relevance (see review by Nieder 2016).
In summary, when one is presented with a pair of digits that are incongruent in size and numerical value, this sensory information is encoded in parallel in the bilateral inferior temporal gyri, which manifests in posterior N1 activity of the inferior temporal component. This information is then integrated in the IPS, and the DV is accumulated in favour of a particular outcome until reaching an action-triggering threshold, which manifests in CPP potential of the centro-parietal component. These brain regions are functionally connected into one coherent neural network. Analysis of the DV signal for each condition revealed characteristic build-up rates and action-triggering thresholds for the two tasks, which differ only in the instructions given by the experimenter. Our results suggest that errors are committed due to accumulation of evidence in favour of the other (i.e. wrong) task instruction. This interpretation fully explains the behavioural pattern observed here: errors in numerical comparisons have shorter RTs than hits (and similar RTs to hits in physical comparisons), whereas errors in physical comparisons show longer RTs than hits. On a final note, our study highlights the advantages of implementing group ICA in both EEG and fMRI data.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by National Science Centre, Poland (Grant number 2012/05/E/HS6/03553).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Amalric M, Dehaene S (2016) Origins of the brain networks for advanced mathematics in expert mathematicians. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:4909–4917CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amalric M, Dehaene S (2016) Origins of the brain networks for advanced mathematics in expert mathematicians. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:4909–4917CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM (2005) Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:1001–1013CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM (2005) Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:1001–1013CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Froncisz W, Marek T (2015a) Dissociating EEG sources linked to stimulus and response evaluation in numerical Stroop task using independent component analysis. Clin Neurophysiol 126:914–926CrossRefPubMed Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Froncisz W, Marek T (2015a) Dissociating EEG sources linked to stimulus and response evaluation in numerical Stroop task using independent component analysis. Clin Neurophysiol 126:914–926CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Oginska H, Marek T, Fafrowicz M (2015b) Brain activations related to saccadic response conflict are not sensitive to time on task. Front Hum Neurosci 9:664CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Oginska H, Marek T, Fafrowicz M (2015b) Brain activations related to saccadic response conflict are not sensitive to time on task. Front Hum Neurosci 9:664CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Besner D, Coltheart M (1979) Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English. Neuropsychologia 17:467–472CrossRefPubMed Besner D, Coltheart M (1979) Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English. Neuropsychologia 17:467–472CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD (2006) The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol Rev 113:700–765CrossRefPubMed Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD (2006) The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol Rev 113:700–765CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 151:140–151CrossRef Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 151:140–151CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173CrossRefPubMed Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJS, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Rombouts SARB (2008) Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex 18:1856–1864CrossRefPubMed Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJS, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Rombouts SARB (2008) Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex 18:1856–1864CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Danielmeier C, Wessel JR, Steinhauser M, Ullsperger M (2009) Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology 46:1288–1298CrossRefPubMed Danielmeier C, Wessel JR, Steinhauser M, Ullsperger M (2009) Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology 46:1288–1298CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat de Lafuente V, Jazayeri M, Shadlen MN (2015) Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areas. J Neurosci 35:4306–4318CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral de Lafuente V, Jazayeri M, Shadlen MN (2015) Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areas. J Neurosci 35:4306–4318CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21CrossRefPubMed Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Domagalik A, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Oginska H, Marek T (2012) Neural networks related to pro-saccades and anti-saccades revealed by independent component analysis. Neuroimage 62:1325–1333CrossRefPubMed Domagalik A, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Oginska H, Marek T (2012) Neural networks related to pro-saccades and anti-saccades revealed by independent component analysis. Neuroimage 62:1325–1333CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Domagalik A, Beldzik E, Oginska H, Marek T, Fafrowicz M (2014) Inconvenient correlation—RT-BOLD relationship for homogeneous and fast reactions. Neuroscience 278:211–221CrossRefPubMed Domagalik A, Beldzik E, Oginska H, Marek T, Fafrowicz M (2014) Inconvenient correlation—RT-BOLD relationship for homogeneous and fast reactions. Neuroscience 278:211–221CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Du Y, Fan Y (2013) Group information guided ICA for fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 69:157–197CrossRefPubMed Du Y, Fan Y (2013) Group information guided ICA for fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 69:157–197CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Eichele T, Rachakonda S, Brakedal B, Eikeland R, Calhoun VD (2011) EEGIFT: group independent component analysis for event-related EEG data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:9CrossRef Eichele T, Rachakonda S, Brakedal B, Eikeland R, Calhoun VD (2011) EEGIFT: group independent component analysis for event-related EEG data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:9CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78:447–455CrossRefPubMed Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78:447–455CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Gallistel, Gelman (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59–65CrossRefPubMed Gallistel, Gelman (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59–65CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gold JI, Shadlen MN (2007) The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:535–574CrossRefPubMed Gold JI, Shadlen MN (2007) The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:535–574CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gratton G, Coles MG, Sirevaag EJ, Eriksen CW, Donchin E (1988) Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:331–344CrossRefPubMed Gratton G, Coles MG, Sirevaag EJ, Eriksen CW, Donchin E (1988) Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:331–344CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Henik A, Tzelgov J (1982) Is three greater than five: the relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Mem Cognit 10:389–395CrossRefPubMed Henik A, Tzelgov J (1982) Is three greater than five: the relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Mem Cognit 10:389–395CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004) Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22:1214–1222CrossRefPubMed Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004) Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22:1214–1222CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Holroyd CB, Coles M (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109(4):679–709CrossRefPubMed Holroyd CB, Coles M (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109(4):679–709CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:435–448CrossRefPubMed Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:435–448CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, Sejnowski TJ (2000) Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology 37:163–178CrossRefPubMed Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, Sejnowski TJ (2000) Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology 37:163–178CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kadosh RC, Henik A, Rubinsten O, Mohr H, Dori H, van de Ven V, Zorzi M, Hendler T, Goebel R, Linden DEJ (2005) Are numbers special? Neuropsychologia 43:1238–1248CrossRef Kadosh RC, Henik A, Rubinsten O, Mohr H, Dori H, van de Ven V, Zorzi M, Hendler T, Goebel R, Linden DEJ (2005) Are numbers special? Neuropsychologia 43:1238–1248CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kadosh RC, Kadosh KC, Linden DEJ, Gevers W, Berger A, Henik A (2007) The brain locus of interaction between number and size: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potential study. J Cogn Neurosci 19:957–970CrossRef Kadosh RC, Kadosh KC, Linden DEJ, Gevers W, Berger A, Henik A (2007) The brain locus of interaction between number and size: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potential study. J Cogn Neurosci 19:957–970CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufmann L, Koppelstaetter F, Delazer M, Siedentopf C, Rhomberg P, Golaszewski S, Felber S, Ischebeck A (2005) Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 25:888–898CrossRefPubMed Kaufmann L, Koppelstaetter F, Delazer M, Siedentopf C, Rhomberg P, Golaszewski S, Felber S, Ischebeck A (2005) Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 25:888–898CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly RE, Alexopoulos GS, Wang Z, Gunning FM, Murphy CF, Morimoto SS, Kanellopoulos D, Jia Z, Lim KO, Hoptman MJ (2010) Visual inspection of independent components: defining a procedure for artifact removal from fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods 189:233–245CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kelly RE, Alexopoulos GS, Wang Z, Gunning FM, Murphy CF, Morimoto SS, Kanellopoulos D, Jia Z, Lim KO, Hoptman MJ (2010) Visual inspection of independent components: defining a procedure for artifact removal from fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods 189:233–245CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Li Y-O, Adali T, Calhoun VD (2007) Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Hum Brain Mapp 28:1251–1266CrossRefPubMed Li Y-O, Adali T, Calhoun VD (2007) Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Hum Brain Mapp 28:1251–1266CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Makeig S, Onton J (2009) ERP features and EEG dynamics: an ICA perspective. In: Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. New York, Oxford Makeig S, Onton J (2009) ERP features and EEG dynamics: an ICA perspective. In: Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. New York, Oxford
Zurück zum Zitat Murphy PR, Robertson IH, Harty S, O’Connell RG, Ochsner K, Dougherty D, Bush G, Eskandar E (2015) Neural evidence accumulation persists after choice to inform metacognitive judgments. Elife 4:489–510CrossRef Murphy PR, Robertson IH, Harty S, O’Connell RG, Ochsner K, Dougherty D, Bush G, Eskandar E (2015) Neural evidence accumulation persists after choice to inform metacognitive judgments. Elife 4:489–510CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nieder A, Dehaene S (2009) Representation of number in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 32:185–208CrossRefPubMed Nieder A, Dehaene S (2009) Representation of number in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 32:185–208CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connell RG, Dockree PM, Kelly SP (2012) A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nat Neurosci 15:1729–1735CrossRefPubMed O’Connell RG, Dockree PM, Kelly SP (2012) A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nat Neurosci 15:1729–1735CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pailing PE, Segalowitz SJ (2004) The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure: motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. Psychophysiology 41:84–95CrossRefPubMed Pailing PE, Segalowitz SJ (2004) The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure: motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. Psychophysiology 41:84–95CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Park J, Hebrank A, Polk TA, Park DC (2012) Neural dissociation of number from letter recognition and its relationship to parietal numerical processing. J Cogn Neurosci 24:39–50CrossRefPubMed Park J, Hebrank A, Polk TA, Park DC (2012) Neural dissociation of number from letter recognition and its relationship to parietal numerical processing. J Cogn Neurosci 24:39–50CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pinel P, Dehaene S, Rivière D, LeBihan D (2001) Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage 14:1013–1026CrossRefPubMed Pinel P, Dehaene S, Rivière D, LeBihan D (2001) Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage 14:1013–1026CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S (2004) Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron 41:983–993CrossRefPubMed Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S (2004) Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron 41:983–993CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Platt ML, Glimcher PW (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233–238CrossRefPubMed Platt ML, Glimcher PW (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233–238CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Santens S, Verguts T (2011) The size congruity effect: is bigger always more? Cognition 118:94–110CrossRefPubMed Santens S, Verguts T (2011) The size congruity effect: is bigger always more? Cognition 118:94–110CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Shum J, Hermes D, Foster BL, Dastjerdi M, Rangarajan V, Winawer J, Miller KJ, Parvizi J (2013) A brain area for visual numerals. J Neurosci 33:6709–6715CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Shum J, Hermes D, Foster BL, Dastjerdi M, Rangarajan V, Winawer J, Miller KJ, Parvizi J (2013) A brain area for visual numerals. J Neurosci 33:6709–6715CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Smith PL, Ratcliff R (2004) Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends Neurosci 27:161–168CrossRefPubMed Smith PL, Ratcliff R (2004) Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends Neurosci 27:161–168CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:13040–13045CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:13040–13045CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Szucs D, Soltész F (2007) Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm. Neuropsychologia 45:3190–3202CrossRefPubMed Szucs D, Soltész F (2007) Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm. Neuropsychologia 45:3190–3202CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Szűcs D, Soltész F (2008) The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: an ERP study. Brain Res 1190:143–158CrossRefPubMed Szűcs D, Soltész F (2008) The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: an ERP study. Brain Res 1190:143–158CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Szűcs D, Soltész F (2012) Functional definition of the N450 event-related brain potential marker of conflict processing: a numerical stroop study. BMC Neurosci 13:35CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Szűcs D, Soltész F (2012) Functional definition of the N450 event-related brain potential marker of conflict processing: a numerical stroop study. BMC Neurosci 13:35CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Szũcs D, Soltész F, Jármi E, Csépe V (2007) The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study. Behav Brain Funct 3:23CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Szũcs D, Soltész F, Jármi E, Csépe V (2007) The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study. Behav Brain Funct 3:23CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Szűcs D, Soltész F, White S (2009) Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography. Neuroimage 47:1960–1973CrossRefPubMed Szűcs D, Soltész F, White S (2009) Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography. Neuroimage 47:1960–1973CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) A co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme, New York Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) A co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme, New York
Zurück zum Zitat Tudusciuc O, Nieder A (2007) Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:14513–14518CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tudusciuc O, Nieder A (2007) Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:14513–14518CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Twomey DM, Murphy PR, Kelly SP, O’Connell RG (2015) The classic P300 encodes a build-to-threshold decision variable. Eur J Neurosci 42:1636–1643CrossRefPubMed Twomey DM, Murphy PR, Kelly SP, O’Connell RG (2015) The classic P300 encodes a build-to-threshold decision variable. Eur J Neurosci 42:1636–1643CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Varoquaux G, Sadaghiani S, Pinel P, Kleinschmidt A, Poline JB, Thirion B (2010) A group model for stable multi-subject ICA on fMRI datasets. Neuroimage 51:288–299CrossRefPubMed Varoquaux G, Sadaghiani S, Pinel P, Kleinschmidt A, Poline JB, Thirion B (2010) A group model for stable multi-subject ICA on fMRI datasets. Neuroimage 51:288–299CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Verguts T, Fias W (2008) Symbolic and nonsymbolic pathways of number processing. Philos Psychol 21:539–554CrossRef Verguts T, Fias W (2008) Symbolic and nonsymbolic pathways of number processing. Philos Psychol 21:539–554CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Visintin E, De Panfilis C, Antonucci C, Capecci C, Marchesi C, Sambataro F (2015) Parsing the intrinsic networks underlying attention: a resting state study. Behav Brain Res 278:315–322CrossRefPubMed Visintin E, De Panfilis C, Antonucci C, Capecci C, Marchesi C, Sambataro F (2015) Parsing the intrinsic networks underlying attention: a resting state study. Behav Brain Res 278:315–322CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Vogel EK, Luck SJ (2000) The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology 37:190–203CrossRefPubMed Vogel EK, Luck SJ (2000) The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology 37:190–203CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7:483–488CrossRefPubMed Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7:483–488CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat West R, Alain C (2000) Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology 37:179–189CrossRefPubMed West R, Alain C (2000) Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology 37:179–189CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat White CN, Ratcliff R, Starns JJ (2011) Diffusion models of the flanker task: discrete versus gradual attentional selection. Cogn Psychol 63:210–238CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral White CN, Ratcliff R, Starns JJ (2011) Diffusion models of the flanker task: discrete versus gradual attentional selection. Cogn Psychol 63:210–238CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
When three is greater than five: EEG and fMRI signatures of errors in numerical and physical comparisons
verfasst von
Ewa Beldzik
Aleksandra Domagalik
Magda Gawlowska
Tadeusz Marek
Justyna Mojsa-Kaja
Publikationsdatum
22.09.2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Brain Structure and Function / Ausgabe 2/2018
Print ISSN: 1863-2653
Elektronische ISSN: 1863-2661
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1527-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2018

Brain Structure and Function 2/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.