Introduction
Structural brain changes during normal aging comprise decreases in gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM; Fjell and Walhovd
2010). Interestingly, some older individuals experience strong and early manifestations (accelerated brain aging), while others of comparable age do not experience changes expected at that age [decelerated brain aging; (Bartrés-Faz and Arenaza-Urquijo
2011; Ziegler et al.
2012)]. As this high interindividual variability cannot be fully explained by chronological age (Jockwitz et al.
2017; Stern
2009,
2012), other factors that provide potential explanatory insight have come into focus, one of them being lifestyle.
According to the seed and soil model (McDonough and Allen
2019) neurocognitive disorders are only developed, if pathological processes such as cell death and accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles (the seed) meet an unfavorable neuronal environment (the soil). Unfavorable neuronal environments can be health conditions accompanying normal aging such as cardiovascular disease or infections, but also behavior such as a risky lifestyle (McDonough and Allen
2019). In contrast, a protective environment, e.g., healthier lifestyle such as higher physical activity and social integration, may promote a more resilient soil and neuroprotection (Anaturk et al.
2018; Arenaza-Urquijo et al.
2015; Bittner et al.
2019; Fratiglioni et al.
2004; McDonough and Allen
2019). For example, socially integrated Alzheimer’s disease patients show higher cognitive stability compared to not integrated patients, even when suffering from a similar degree of pathology (Bennett et al.
2006). Furthermore, social network size correlates positively with amygdala volume in humans (Bickart et al.
2011). Likewise, higher physical activity, especially in older adults, has repeatedly been associated with better cognitive performance (Colcombe and Kramer
2003; Erickson et al.
2007; Hughes and Ganguli
2009; Kramer et al.
1999,
2003; Voelcker-Rehage et al.
2010) and preservation of GM volume (Colcombe et al.
2003; Erickson et al.
2011). Older adults engaging in physical activity training showed increased hippocampal volume (Erickson et al.
2011) and more efficient use of functional brain networks (Colcombe et al.
2004; Voelcker-Rehage et al.
2011). Recently, lifespan physical activity has been associated with favorable ratios of brain metabolism markers in magnetic resonance imaging (Engeroff et al.
2019). This hints even further at physical activity being a factor for promoting a more resilient neuronal environment. In contrast, smoking seems to be associated with cortical thinning in prefrontal and temporal regions (Karama et al.
2015) and decreased GM density within cingulum, precuneus, thalamus, and precentral gyrus (Almeida et al.
2008). In addition, excessive alcohol consumption can lead to serious neurological diseases, e.g., Korsakow syndrome (de la Monte and Kril
2014) and is associated with reduced GM and WM volume and density (Paul et al.
2008; Pfefferbaum et al.
1995; Topiwala et al.
2017) in alcohol-dependent as well as non-dependent individuals (Mukamal at al.
2001).
Most previous studies focused on specific effects of a single lifestyle variable on brain structure and function. In real life, however, individuals engage in a combination of lifestyle behaviors, e.g., physical exercise and afterwards meeting friends (social integration) while drinking a beer (alcohol consumption). Only few studies investigated the effects of lifestyle on brain structure and function or on cognition in a multidimensional way. For example, Floel et al. (
2008) found that the combination of exercise, dietary habits, BMI, smoking and alcohol intake was a better predictor for memory performance than the individual lifestyle behaviors. In a previous study, we developed a combined lifestyle risk score reflecting individual combinations of the above described daily lifestyle behaviors, with higher values reflecting more risky behavior (e.g., high smoking and alcohol consumption, low social integration and physical activity), whereas lower values indicate protective combinations (Bittner et al.
2019). We showed that higher combined lifestyle risk was associated with brain atrophy, e.g., more alcohol consumption combined with low physical activity was associated with structural decreases in the premotor region. From the perspective of the seed and soil theory, it may further be concluded that combination of several risky lifestyle factors may provide an even more unfavorable soil for the development of alterations in brain structure than the presence of one factor alone. Based on these findings, it might thus be assumed that combined risky lifestyle leads to accelerated brain aging, accompanied by decreases in cognitive performance. Non-linear effects and covariates such as sex or education as these affect not only brain phenotypes, but also lifestyle habits and the association between both are of additional relevance (Cullen et al.
2012; Fratiglioni et al.
2004; Gur and Gur
2017; Kramer and Colcombe
2018; McKenna et al.
2003; Mukamal et al.
2001).
However, we still do not know enough about which factors or behaviors predict the size of the gap between a specific chronological age and the actual manifestation of the individual aging process. To capture this manifestation, biological age of the brain, estimated from structural brain scans, may be more informative and precise than chronological age. To measure these MRI-based brain-aging patterns, Franke et al. (
2010) developed a machine-learning framework, which uses the most relevant voxel-wise GM information to aggregate the complex underlying multidimensional alteration patterns of brain aging into one single value, the estimated brain age. The difference, i.e., the gap, between brain age as estimated from MR images and true chronological age (Franke et al.
2010) is then the Brain
Age
Gap
Estimation (BrainAGE) score. BrainAGE is positive if aging patterns observed via MRI appear older than expected based on chronological age (accelerated brain aging), and negative if they appear younger (decelerated brain aging). BrainAGE and several other MRI-based brain age prediction models have been established as a meaningful imaging biomarker to study the prediction of future brain aging patterns and their longitudinal trajectories (Cole and Franke
2017; Cole et al.
2019; Franke et al.
2012; Gaser et al.
2013), cognitive decline and disease severity (Franke et al.
2012). At the same time, BrainAGE is widely applicable and highly accurate to study the high interindividual variability in structural brain aging (Cole and Franke
2017). Recent studies found MRI-predicted brain age even to be associated with relevant genetic variants for cortical thickness and encoding for tau protein (Jónsson et al.
2019; Ning et al.
2020), even though these variants only explained 11% of the variance at most within the gap between estimated brain and chronological age. In contrast, other brain age predictions have been reported to be not associated to genetic variants in general, but only specific brain compartments, which in turn were partly related to modifiable risk behavior, e.g., smoking (Smith et al.
2020).
Hence, the current study aimed at examining whether highly complex, multivariate lifestyle behaviors can partly predict the deceleration or acceleration of brain aging (reflected in the BrainAGE score) in the 1000BRAINS cohort of “normal” aging older adults (Caspers et al.
2014). Here, BrainAGE allows conducting straightforward quantification of how much variance in brain aging can be predicted by a more risky lifestyle, and therefore, whether it may be a potential soil for neurocognitive alterations (McDonough and Allen
2019).
First, we examined the relation between our newly developed combined lifestyle risk score (Bittner et al.
2019) and BrainAGE. We hypothesized that higher combined lifestyle risk would generally predict accelerated brain aging, i.e., higher BrainAGE scores. Second, we investigated the association between each individual lifestyle variable and BrainAGE to further elucidate contributions of single lifestyle variables to this age acceleration prediction. We ran all analyses for the whole sample and subsequently for males and females separately (based on separate BrainAGE estimations) to account for sex-specific differences.
Discussion
The present study showed that lifestyle habits contribute to differences in brain aging in a cohort of “normal” aging older adults and gives promising insights into why people even without neurodegenerative diseases age so differently. We used two approaches: We used a novel lifestyle risk score (Bittner et al.
2019) combining different lifestyle variables into one value, while additionally investigating each lifestyle habit alone. In addition, we examined differences in brain structure using BrainAGE as a meaningful imaging biomarker (Franke et al.
2012,
2014; Gaser et al.
2013; Lowe et al.
2016), which showed a very good performance in our sample (Cole and Franke
2017). Further, the results of our subsequent analyses may provide first hints at sex differences in the association between lifestyle and BrainAGE that are often not examined in studies on lifestyle-associated differences in the brain, e.g., in terms of smoking (Karama et al.
2015), alcohol consumption (Vergara et al.
2017) and physical activity (Kramer and Colcombe
2018).
Associations between combined lifestyle risk and BrainAGE
Prior studies focused mostly on the effect of single lifestyle variables on the brain, considering co-occurrences of various lifestyle behaviors as nuisance factors rather than as effects of interest. In contrast, the current study considered four different lifestyle behaviors as a combined concept (Bittner et al.
2019) to examine if lifestyle explains variability in BrainAGE and which lifestyle behaviors contribute the most to this association. Regarding a phenotype as complex and multidimensional as lifestyle, it is reasonable to assume that one specific behavior can only account for parts of the variance in brain aging, providing necessity for investigation of different variables together (Bittner et al.
2019; Floel et al.
2008; Vergara et al.
2017). Considering several behaviors as well as composite scores seem to provide a better prediction of differences between older adults, e.g., in verbal memory, than the individual measures alone (Floel et al.
2008). Comparable approaches have also been used in imaging genetics, where polygenic risk scores (several genetic markers aggregated into one score) can explain more variability in neurological diseases and brain phenotypes than individual genetic markers alone (Dudbridge
2013; Harrison et al.
2016; Torkamani et al.
2018; Ursini et al.
2018). Considering the seed and soil model of neurocognitive disorders, it may be expected that a soil that promotes neuronal alterations gets even more toxic, the more risk factors come together (McDonough and Allen
2019). Therefore, we hypothesized that our combined lifestyle risk score would predict more variance than each single behavior alone.
To test this hypothesis, we conducted three different approaches: approach 1 including chronological age as a predictor, approach 2 ignoring chronological age and approach 3 regressing chronological age out of the BrainAGE score before the analyses. In general, the results of those three models were in agreement. Approach 1 was predictive of the highest amount of variance in BrainAGE, likely, since chronological age was still associated to this outcome, and therefore, contributed a small amount of prediction. The amount of variance in BrainAGE explained by the different lifestyle variables, i.e., the combined lifestyle risk score, physical activity and pack-years of smoking was greatest in approach 2. Therefore, the quantification in months of BrainAGE was also highest in this approach, but these differences were marginal. This is most likely due to the fact, that if no correction for chronological age is made, there is some variance in BrainAGE which would be explained by chronological age usually, but which will now be predicted by lifestyle behavior, which is slightly confounded with chronological age. The prediction accuracy measured in MAE was comparable between the approaches though. Before regressing chronological age out of BrainAGE in approach 3 it was still associated to BrainAGE with
p = 0.028, but only explaining 0.8% of the variance in the whole sample. This was not true in male (
R2 = 0.005;
p = 0.209), but in female (
R2 = 0.015,
p = 0.047) participants, which was expectable since a small correlation between chronological age and BrainAGE is visible in females (Fig.
2b). In fact, the association between chronological age and BrainAGE in the whole sample may be driven by the female participants.
However, from a theoretical perspective, it seems contra-intuitive to include chronological age into a model to predict an outcome, which is related to age itself, favoring approach 2. Since BrainAGE was still—at least in females—associated to chronological age, not correcting BrainAGE for chronological age would be slightly inaccurate though, from a statistical perspective. In our view, approach 3 seems, therefore, to be the most appropriate approach, since it does not include chronological age as a predictor (to predict BrainAGE), but the residual variance of chronological age, which is statistically there, is regressed out, nevertheless. However, there is no conclusive solution to this issue in the present literature (Smith et al.
2019).
As hypothesized, we found a lifestyle-dependent acceleration of structural brain aging, where higher lifestyle risk predicted higher BrainAGE scores, thus older looking brains. This observation is particularly important, since it helps explain a significant proportion of the large interindividual variability in structural brain aging of older adults (Dickie et al.
2013), which cannot be accounted for by age, sex, education or clinical markers, such as BMI or uric acid (Arenaza-Urquijo et al.
2015; Christie et al.
2017; Eavani et al.
2018; Fjell and Walhovd
2010; Franke et al.
2014; Jagust
2013). In the present sample, the combined lifestyle risk score predicted 2.4% (approach 3) of the variance beyond sex, education, BMI and cognitive status, which was comparable to the amount predicted by physical activity (2.1%) and smoking (2.9%), also in approach 3. This is comparable to the amount of explained variance reported for lifestyle behaviors or health markers in other large epidemiological studies (Franke et al.
2014; Jockwitz et al.
2019; Miller et al.
2016) and also within reasonable range for a large sample from a population-based cohort (Bzdok and Ioannidis
2019). It is important to note, that even though each variable only predicted a small proportion of variance, this variance is calculated after correction for those factors usually showing a large influence on brain structure: age, sex, education and general cognitive status. Even after correction, the general association between riskier lifestyle and older looking brains remained stable. However, other influences, such as genetic variations may have an additional impact on age-related differences in brain structure and age predictions (Ning et al.
2020; Smith et al.
2020).
In a former study, individual lifestyle variables did not show a significant effect on cortical surface measures, whereas the combined lifestyle risk score did (Bittner et al.
2019). However, it is important to note, that the former study was a vertex-wise whole brain approach, sensitive to lifestyle-related regional differences, whereas the present study examined the association between lifestyle and BrainAGE, reflecting the multidimensional pattern of aging aggregated into one marker. Importantly, we quantified the effect of combined lifestyle risk on structural brain aging in terms of years. This was inspired by Franke et al. (
2014), who estimated mean BrainAGE in a “risky” and a “healthy” group in terms of clinical markers, such as BMI or uric acid. Instead of quantifying group differences in terms of years, as done by Franke et al. (
2014), we estimated the linear increase in BrainAGE for each increase in lifestyle risk of the specific variable. In consequence, with each increase in combined lifestyle risk, brains appear 5.04 months older than the “normal” age-related difference in brain structure, which the statistical model corrected for. In comparison, brains appear 0.6 months older with each pack-year and 0.55 months younger with each increase in metabolic equivalent (MET) per week (with, e.g., 4 MET reflecting one hour of 10-mph bicycling). Hence, the combined lifestyle risk score explained more than 3 months in Brain AGE “in addition” to smoking in pack-years, as hypothesized. Therefore, the combined lifestyle risk score may have higher explanatory power, presumably via consideration of over-additive and interacting effects between the individual factors when quantifying the harmful and protective effects of lifestyle. Considering different behaviors of interest simultaneously may thus be a fruitful way to explain additional variance in brain aging by investigating their cumulative effects.
Associations between individual lifestyle variables and BrainAGE
Investigating the four lifestyle behaviors individually revealed more smoking and lower physical activity to be the strongest contributors to the prediction capacity of lifestyle risk on BrainAGE.
Smoking
One of the compelling results of the current study were the negative effects of smoking on the aged brain quantified in months, which were also not corroborated after correcting for education or cognitive status. Prior studies already hinted at an association between smoking and changes in GM. Regionally lower GM volume and density for smokers compared to non-smoking control participants have been reported in the prefrontal cortex and the cerebellum (Brody et al.
2004), the posterior cingulum, precuneus, right thalamus, and bilateral frontal cortex (Almeida et al.
2008), as well as the substantia nigra (Gallinat et al.
2006). Importantly, all of these studies had a fairly small sample size that either included younger adults only [
n = 45, age range 22.4–38.3 years, Gallinat et al. (
2006)], older adults only [
n = 78, age range 71.6–78.9 years; Almeida et al. (
2008)], or a large age range [
n = 36, 21–65 years, Brody et al. (
2004)]. In addition, results on the association of smoking with other brain metrics are quite heterogenous. Higher numbers of white matter hyperintensities (Longstreth et al.
2005), lower microstructural integrity (Gons et al.
2011), or infarcts (Howard et al.
1998) in smokers compared to non-smokers were reported. With population-based cohort imaging available, the sample sizes have substantially increased (Bamberg et al.
2015; Caspers et al.
2014; Miller et al.
2016; Van Essen et al.
2013), thus increasing generalizability of results to the general population. For example, Karama et al. (
2015) showed that smoking was associated with widespread cortical thinning in a sample of 504 older adults particularly in prefrontal cortex, mostly omitting primary sensory areas. Still, none of the studies provided a quantification of the effect of smoking on the brain. We were able to predict an increase of 0.36 months of BrainAGE with each pack-year, with 2.9% of BrainAGE being predicted just by smoking (approach 3). Translating this result to our older adult study sample taking into account the average smoking behavior of 13.61 pack-years, an overall increase of 4.9 years of BrainAGE (13.61 × 0.36 month of BrainAGE) only by smoking can be stated.
There was a high variance in BrainAGE in individuals, who never smoked (139 female, 114 male, Fig.
4). Within this group, BrainAGE scores were very high, as well as very low, aggregating to a mean BrainAGE of almost zero. This finding is comparable to the considerable variance in cortical thickness in the rarely smoking participants observed by Karama et al. (
2015). In the current study, the more the participants smoked, the stronger was the relationship between higher pack-years and higher BrainAGE, suggesting that this effect was mostly driven by high lifetime smoking (Fig.
4). This was also revealed when comparing never, moderate and severe smokers, where the brains of severe smokers (BrainAGE = 1.61) appeared significantly older than those of never (BrainAGE = − 0.05) and moderate (BrainAGE = − 0.03) smokers. It is particularly important to note that this observation cannot be translated simply to the assumption that rare smoking has no effect on the brain. Rather, rare smoking may manifest in other metrics for healthy brain aging, even if alterations in brain structure would not be present: For example, in our previous study, we found no association between smoking and cortical surface measures in older adults. Instead, more smoking was associated with higher resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC), which may be a compensation mechanism for accelerated brain aging (Bittner et al.
2019). In addition, activity differences in task-based fMRI (Lawrence et al.
2002; Tanabe et al.
2011), as well as receptor differences between smokers and non-smokers (Feduccia et al.
2012; Mukhin et al.
2008) were described. Studies on RSFC in relation to long-term effects of smoking and not acute effects of nicotine are rather rare. It may thus be of particular interest to investigate general differences in brain function, e.g., in RSFC, associated with light smoking (Janes et al.
2012; Pariyadath et al.
2014; Zhou et al.
2017), even though light smoking seems not to be heavily associated to differences in brain structure.
The underlying mechanisms driving the association between smoking and changes in brain structure are still unclear. Smoking could potentially act via atherosclerotic processes, which may impact the aging brain and thus accelerate brain aging (Freund et al.
1993; Mucha et al.
2006; Prescott et al.
1998; Pujades-Rodriguez et al.
2015). Possibly, the measured increase in BrainAGE might also be attributable to the direct toxic effects of tobacco smoke onto the cerebro-vascular system, which includes oxidative stress within the cells and results in apoptosis (Swan and Lessov-Schlaggar
2007). However, since BrainAGE takes the whole GM volume of an individual into account, drawing inferences about any molecular mechanisms or disentangle regional differences that drive the association between stronger smoking and accelerated brain aging as reflected by higher BrainAGE remains for future studies. Still, our results provide evidence, that smoking may be one of the detrimental factors contributing to an unfavorable soil, which then together with additional risk factors, promotes alterations within the brain or accelerated already existing processes of structural decline.
Sex differences in the associations between smoking and BrainAGE
Most prior studies assessing the effect of smoking on brain structure did not examine sex differences or the interaction of sex and smoking (Almeida et al.
2008; Brody et al.
2004; Gallinat et al.
2006; Karama et al.
2015; Longstreth et al.
2005). To our knowledge, there are only two studies addressing this issue, showing that structural differences associated to smoking may regionally differ depending on sex (Duriez et al.
2014; Franklin et al.
2014). After examining lifestyle effects in the whole study sample, we addressed this issue and found no interaction between sex and smoking, hinting at a comparable direction and strength of association in both sexes. With imaging research focusing more on sex differences (Franke et al.
2014; Gur and Gur
2017; Ritchie et al.
2018; Ruigrok et al.
2014; Wierenga et al.
2018), it may nevertheless be of interest to identify lifestyle behaviors that differentially affect male and female brains, such that interventions that slow or delay manifestations of aging can be tailored for sex.
Physical activity and BrainAGE
The protective effect of physical activity on GM volume has been discussed to be regionally specific [see review by Erickson et al. (
2014)]. Our results support that higher physical activity is predictive of lower BrainAGE, thus younger looking brains. Physical activity, therefore, does not only seem to affect specific brain regions, but also the multidimensional pattern of brain aging itself. Most previous studies comprised intervention trainings, where training was systematic, regular, and highly controlled (Erickson et al.
2014). The present study adds to this by demonstrating that higher physical activity is associated with decelerated brain aging (lower BrainAGE scores) in a large sample of “normal” aging older adults using a comprehensive, epidemiologically motivated measurement of physical activity, i.e., the metabolic equivalent (Ainsworth et al.
2000; Bus et al.
2011; Floel et al.
2010; Milanovic et al.
2013; Pierce et al.
2007; Ruscheweyh et al.
2011; Wagner et al.
2012). This measurement is drawn from self-reports that summarize all sorts of sports older adults engage in and is likely to reflect the average daily physical activity, in contrast to highly controlled intervention settings. The present association between self-reported physical activity and BrainAGE is, therefore, not as strong as reported effects of fitness training on, e.g., the hippocampus (Erickson et al.
2011), but is likely reflecting a natural, and therefore, more generalizable relationship.
Several mechanisms how higher physical activity or fitness levels may act protectively on the aging brain have been discussed, such as the upregulation of neurotrophic factors, including brain-derived-neurotrophic factor [BDNF, (de Melo Coelho et al.
2013; Neeper et al.
1996; Piepmeier and Etnier
2015) Ruscheweyh et al.
2011] and granulocyte-colony stimulating factor (G-CSF; Flöel et al.
2010), which significantly impact synaptic efficacy, neuronal connectivity, and use-dependent plasticity. Here, use-dependent plasticity may play a crucial role in the sense that physical activity as one kind of training would lead to better preservation of those brain structures needed to perform the activity engaged in (Bittner et al.
2019; Colcombe et al.
2003; Vaynman and Gomez-Pinilla
2005; Vaynman et al.
2004). Several studies have shown that training-induced preservation or even adaptation of brain regions is possible in adults (Churchill et al.
2002; Draganski et al.
2004; Erickson et al.
2011; Kramer and Erickson
2007) or older adults in particular (Boyke et al.
2008). Nevertheless, other studies have discussed, that the most important factor for preservation of brain structure is effortful learning (Shors
2014). During physical activity, both processes are possible, but could not be controlled in the epidemiological setting of the present study. However, physical activity is widely known for its benefitting effects on the general health of the human organism (World Health Organization
2019). Possibly enhancing the resilience of the organism, as well as higher neuronal integrity (Engeroff et al.
2019), it may, therefore, contribute to a more protective soil, where pathological processes may have lesser effect (McDonough and Allen
2019). Future studies examining specific mechanisms are needed, though.
Sex differences in the associations between physical activity and BrainAGE
Most previous studies on lifestyle did not examine interaction effects of sex, as done in classic psychological research or specifically conducted sex-stratified analyses, as done in epidemiological research (Erickson et al.
2014; Floel et al.
2010,
2008; Ho et al.
2011). Yet, a recent review concluded that the sex proportion in physical activity intervention studies may impact the effect sizes (Kramer and Colcombe
2018). A potential reason may be expression of BDNF and its effect on physical activity, which has been shown to differ between the sexes in mice with lower expression in females (Venezia et al.
2016). Further, estrogens or hormone replacement therapy seem to be related to levels of neurotrophins such as BDNF (Garcia-Segura et al.
2000) and longer periods of hormone therapy may corroborate the positive effect of high physical activity in women (Erickson et al.
2007). There may also be further differences between the two sexes that co-occur with physical activity, such as dietary habits (Kramer and Colcombe
2018), the specific kind of activity (Churchill et al.
2002; Colcombe et al.
2003; Floel et al.
2010; Hayes et al.
2013; Kramer and Colcombe
2018), as well as differences in metabolism (Burd et al.
2009; Wu and O'Sullivan
2011) to be considered in future studies. Therefore, we specifically addressed sex differences in subsequent analyses. We did not find a significant interaction effect of sex and physical activity on BrainAGE. Post hoc stratification of the sample for the two sexes revealed that physical activity was significantly predictive for BrainAGE in male (
p = 0.005), but not in female participants (
p = 0.169). This, however, may be due to the smaller proportion of females in the whole sample. Hence, it is possible, that the effect is present in females, but even larger sample sizes are needed to detect it in females as well (i.e., more power). The statistical significance is non-conclusive, hence. The picture gets more unambiguous, when inspecting the amount of predicted variance in BrainAGE (
R2). Here, physical activity was predictive of 3.4% of variance in BrainAGE in males, but of a strikingly different 0.8% of variance in females. This hints at physical activity contributing more to BrainAGE prediction in males.
Interestingly, the (in-sample) prediction accuracy measured in MAE for any of the models examining lifestyle was highest in females. In females, the lowest MAE was 3.70 years in the fully adjusted model examining physical activity. For comparison, the lowest MAE in males was MAE = 3.99 years (fully adjusted model for the combined lifestyle risk score) and in the whole subsample MAE = 3.88 years (fully adjusted model examining pack-years) showing that factors contributing to prediction capacity may differ between males and females (Jiang et al.
2020).
Interestingly, the most accurate model for the prediction of BrainAGE in females was also the model, where physical activity did not significantly contribute to prediction, but which was mostly driven by cognitive status (fully adjusted model examining physical activity). Note that all models including cognitive status (measured using the DemTect) predicted a relatively high proportion of variance in BrainAGE in females (up to 7.4% in approach (i) in combination with the combined lifestyle risk score). This proportion was considerably higher in females than in males (3.9% explained variance within the same model). Hence, cognitive status may in females be a more important mediator when it comes to the prediction of structural brain aging from lifestyle behavior, whereas lifestyle behavior may contribute more to prediction in males.
Thus, the current study found hints for sex differences within this association, but larger samples are needed. Taken together, higher physical activity seems to be one lifestyle behavior that may be predictive of decelerated brain aging, in line with previous studies. As Kramer and Colcombe (
2018) state in their recent review, it can be of great help to disentangle the association between physical activity and BrainAGE to facilitate, e.g., large exercise programs within the communities.
Alcohol consumption and social integration
We did not find an association between BrainAGE and social integration or alcohol consumption. Regional differences in brain structure associated to social integration, as well as alcohol consumption could be present, but might not have been identifiable with the specific approach of the current study. Several explanations might hold for these observations. To date, the number of studies investigating social integration in relation to structural brain decline in older adults is relatively small (for a recent review, see Anaturk et al.
2018). In addition, most studies report effects for composite measurements of cognitive and social activities, which do not clearly differentiate between social and cognitive components (Gow et al.
2012; Hafsteinsdottir et al.
2012; Vaughan et al.
2014). Further, composite measures for social activities when investigated in combination with additional lifestyle behaviors (Bittner et al.
2019) have been assessed. Therefore, future studies could shed light on effects of social activities with low cognitive versus high cognitive demands to answer the question whether the cognitive or the social component of social integration contributes to brain reserve, the amount to which age-related GM loss can be tolerated without showing deficiencies (Stern,
2012). In addition, we used a quantitative measurement of social integration. Studies have shown that older adults engage in relationships with a focus on quality rather than quantity (Carstensen et al.
1999). Hence, future studies would be needed to address the association between quality of relationships and brain aging. Further, differences in brain structure related to social integration may be regional or subtle (James et al.
2012), which also seems to be the case for alcohol consumption (Topiwala et al.
2017). Even though accelerated brain aging has been shown in patients with alcoholism (Pfefferbaum et al.
1995), differences related to alcohol consumption in the normal population may not be as strong or only identifiable if several risk behaviors co-occur (Bittner et al.
2019). Further, alcohol consumption may affect other brain parameters earlier such as WM lesions (den Heijer et al.
2004) or RSFC (Vergara et al.
2017). In addition, effects of alcohol consumption may also be non-linear (Mukamal et al.
2001), which we could not identify in the present study, but might be interesting for future studies to further investigate into.
Strengths and limitations
Strengths of the present study include the large sample size, the older age range of the sample, and the use of BrainAGE as a state-of-the art imaging biomarker. By conceptualizing brain aging in this biomarker, the multivariate dataset and the statistical analyses could be reformulated into intuitive and straightforward results, i.e., the quantification of lifestyle effects in meaningful months of additional brain age.
The present study has a cross-sectional design which does not allow conclusions about directionality of effects. Even without a longitudinal design, though, our approach hints at individuals with higher risk for accelerated brain aging: Comparing the apparent image-based age of individuals’ brains enables a measure of whether a participant’s brain appears to be older (or younger) than the average age-matched data of the sample and captures deviation from expected, i.e., typical development (Kaufmann et al.
2019). For future studies, regional specificity, i.e., disentangling the relevance of different regions for machine-learning-based brain age prediction would be highly desirable (Cole and Franke
2017). Further, investigating the relevance of different brain features, e.g., functional activation patterns, as well as functional and structural connectivity, will be interesting in the future. As BrainAGE enables the identification of individuals at higher risk, together with the straightforward statistics and intuitive quantification of lifestyle effects in meaningful months, it still provides a useful framework to capture relevant aspects of variability in structural brain aging and to examine the high variability in brain reserve (Stern,
2017).
Further, it is important to mention that, based on established approaches in epidemiological research (Schmermund et al.
2002), the lifestyle variables included in our combined lifestyle risk score were measured using different time windows (e.g., physical activity was assessed for the last 4 weeks, smoking as the number of cigarettes smoked over the whole lifetime). Assessments that refer to specifically defined short time frames (e.g., a month, a week) seem to be more reliable indicators of long-term behavior than self-reports referring to longer time frames, e.g., a whole year (Del Boca and Darkes
2003).
In addition, all lifestyle habits were assessed using self-reports, which makes it impossible to rule out memory effects or social desirability bias. Self-report measurements have nevertheless been shown to be valid and reliable (Del Boca and Darkes
2003) and thus suitable in such an epidemiological cohort setting.
Conclusion
Higher lifestyle risk, represented by a combined lifestyle risk score, contributes to accelerated brain aging as revealed by BrainAGE, a meaningful imaging biomarker. Higher lifetime smoking, as well as lower physical activity contributed most to this association. We found hints at a stronger relation between physical activity and BrainAGE for males than females, but future studies are needed to further evaluate the potentially differential relevance of this influencing factor for brain health between the sexes. Further, more research is needed to elucidate the relation between alcohol consumption and brain structure, as well as social integration and brain health, e.g., by disentangling the cognitive and social components. In summary, lifestyle seems to be a fruitful target for identifying behaviors that may contribute to a more resilient organism, and therefore, slow neuronal changes and related or resulting cognitive impairment. Future studies are warranted to examine the underlying mechanisms. Considering co-occurrences between several lifestyle behaviors as effects of interests, rather than as a nuisance may enable us to better understand individual trajectories of brain aging in the older population and why people age differently.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.