Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Study protocol

Baby’s first bites: a randomized controlled trial to assess the effects of vegetable-exposure and sensitive feeding on vegetable acceptance, eating behavior and weight gain in infants and toddlers

verfasst von: S. M. C. van der Veek, C. de Graaf, J. H. M. de Vries, G. Jager, C. M. J. L. Vereijken, H. Weenen, N. van Winden, M. S. van Vliet, J. M. Schultink, V. W. T. de Wild, S. Janssen, J. Mesman

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

The start of complementary feeding in infancy plays an essential role in promoting healthy eating habits. Evidence shows that it is important what infants are offered during this first introduction of solid foods: e.g. starting exclusively with vegetables is more successful for vegetable acceptance than starting with fruits. How infants are introduced to solid foods also matters: if parents are sensitive and responsive to infant cues during feeding, this may promote self-regulation of energy intake and a healthy weight. However, the effectiveness of the what and the how of complementary feeding has never been experimentally tested in the same study. In the current project the what and how (and their combination) are tested in one study to determine their relative importance for fostering vegetable acceptance and self-regulation of energy intake in infants.

Methods

A four-arm randomized controlled trial (Baby’s First Bites (BFB)) was designed for 240 first-time Dutch mothers and their infants, 60 per arm. In this trial, we compare the effectiveness of (a) a vegetable-exposure intervention focusing on the what in complementary feeding; (b) a sensitive feeding intervention focusing on the how in complementary feeding, (c) a combined intervention focusing on the what and how in complementary feeding; (d) an attention-control group. All mothers participate in five sessions spread over the first year of eating solid foods (child age 4–16 months). Primary outcomes are vegetable consumption, vegetable liking and self-regulation of energy intake. Secondary outcomes are child eating behaviors, child anthropometrics and maternal feeding behavior. Outcomes are assessed before, during and directly after the interventions (child age 18 months), and when children are 24 and 36 months old.

Discussion

The outcomes are expected to assess the impact of the interventions and provide new insights into the mechanisms underlying the development of vegetable acceptance, self-regulation and healthy eating patterns in infants and toddlers, as well as the prevention of overweight. The results may be used to improve current dietary advice given to parents of their young children on complementary feeding.

Trial registration

The trial was retrospectively registered during inclusion of participants at the Netherlands National Trial Register (identifier NTR6572) and at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT03348176).
Protocol issue date: 1 April 2018; version number 1.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AC
attention control group
BEBQ
Baby Eating Behavior Questionnaire
BFB
Baby’s First Bites
BMI
Body Mass Index
CEBQ
Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire
CEBQ-T
Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire-Toddler
COMBI
combined RVE + VIPP-FI
EAH
eating in the absence of hunger
EFSA
European Food and Safety Authority
ESPGHAN
European Society for Paediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition
IM
Intervention Mapping
MI
Motivational Interviewing
RCT
Randomized Controlled Trial
RVE
repeated vegetable exposure
VIPP-FI
Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Feeding Infants
VIPP-SD
Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Sensitive Discipline
WHO
World Health Organization

Background

In light of today’s global obesity epidemic and related diseases, promoting healthy eating habits is essential [1]. Children as young as 1–3 years of age already eat too much energy-dense food and too little fruit and vegetables [26]. In the Netherlands, based on surveys between 2006 and 2014, estimates for the percentage of preschoolers failing to meet daily recommendations for vegetable intake vary from 40% up to an alarming 80% [2, 3]. Moreover, a recent experimental study showed that almost 40% of 4 year-olds fail to effectively regulate their own energy intake, showing a tendency to eat even though they are not hungry [7]. Poor eating habits, such as consuming too little vegetables and eating in the absence of hunger increase the risk of developing overweight and obesity, and related diseases such as type II diabetes [812], cardiovascular disease [13], and certain cancers [14]. Both children’s food preferences and their ability to self-regulate their energy intake are influenced by their direct environment already in the first two years of life [1520]. Therefore, promoting a healthy diet and healthy eating habits and behavior from infancy is essential. At this young age, parents bear primary responsibility for the diet of their children. The present article describes the study protocol and sample of a randomized controlled trial under the acronym Baby’s First Bites (BFB), aimed at (a) promoting vegetable intake and liking, and (b) promoting child self-regulation of energy intake, by advising parents what and how to feed their infants from the very start of complementary feeding. The primary goals of promoting vegetable acceptance and self-regulation of energy intake serve the purpose of reducing the risk of developing overweight in early childhood – our secondary outcome. Three interventions will be compared to an attention-control condition (1): a repeated exposure intervention motivating parents to repeatedly expose their children to the taste of a variety of vegetables during the first year of complementary feeding (2); a parenting intervention promoting sensitive parental feeding; and (3) a combined intervention promoting both repeated exposure to vegetables and sensitive feeding.

Repeated exposure to a variety of vegetables from the start of complementary feeding

When parents start complementary feeding, they can choose from a variety of foods to introduce to their children, including (baby) cereals, grains, fruits or vegetables [21, 22]. Already in the 1970s it was theorized that to improve the acceptance of vegetables, these should be introduced before fruits or other sweet tastes during complementary feeding because infants’ inherent preference for sweet tastes will interfere with vegetable acceptance [23]. The effects of starting complementary feeding exclusively with vegetables on promoting vegetable acceptance has, however, not been studied often [24]. Two other methods of increasing vegetable intake and liking have been studied extensively. First, repeated exposure to the taste of vegetables has been shown effective in increasing its intake and liking in infants and preschoolers [2432], especially for bitter tastes [33]. Second, being exposed to a variety of vegetables increases vegetable acceptance in infants [23, 29, 34, 35]. However, whether it is indeed most effective to start with vegetables only was not tested until the trial by Barends et al. in 2013 [22]. This study showed that infants exposed to a variety of vegetables during the first three weeks of complementary feeding – including a target vegetable to which they were repeatedly exposed – nearly doubled their intake of the target vegetable, whereas children who only received fruits showed increased intake of fruits but not of vegetables [26]. Shortly after this trial, another intervention study found similar results: encouraging parents from the United Kingdom to start complementary feeding with a variety of vegetables significantly increased vegetable intake compared to a control group in which parents were allowed to start complementary feeding with whatever food they wanted [36].
Thus, there is preliminary evidence that starting complementary feeding by repeatedly exposing infants to a variety of vegetables is an effective way to increase vegetable intake and liking in the first year of a child’s life. However, the beneficial effects on vegetable acceptance do not seem to last when children grow older [27, 30, 37]. This is in line with the finding that children are open to trying a variety of different tastes in their first year of life, but tend to become more selective about their diet when they become older (especially in the ‘food neophobic phase’) [24, 38, 39]. Indeed, in the Barends et al. trial, starting complementary feeding with vegetables did not predict vegetable intake at age two, whereas how selective children were about what they wanted to eat did [27]. Continuing the active promotion of eating vegetables in the first and second year of the child’s life after exposing them to a variety of vegetables at the start of complementary feeding may counteract the negative effects of the food neophobic phase and effectively boost vegetable intake throughout childhood. However, most intervention studies have been conducted with infants in the early phases of complementary feeding or preschoolers older than 2 years; few studies focus on promoting vegetable acceptance in the difficult period between 12 and 24 months when children go through the major transition of eating the same meals as their family and enter the food neophobic phase [40, 41]. Therefore, we studied the effectiveness of a more prolonged vegetable-exposure intervention throughout the whole first year of complementary feeding, well into the more ‘difficult’ second year of the child’s life to promote vegetable intake in toddlers.

Sensitive feeding

Apart from what parents should offer their children during complementary feeding, how they offer this food may also strongly influence a child’s acceptance of the offered food, as well as their ability to self-regulate their energy intake. Experimental studies show that pressuring a child to eat decreases children’s ability to self-regulate their energy intake and thereby to consume appropriate amounts of calories [42]. Similarly, pressuring a child to eat vegetables has a counterproductive effect and will make a child eat and like these vegetables less [43]. Even giving subtle prompts to eat, like moving food towards a child, may have a counterproductive effect [44]. However, if children start to decrease their vegetable intake when they enter the second year of life, parents are likely to use some sort of pressure to make their child eat. Indeed, an Australian study showed that more than half of the parents of 1–3 year-olds sometimes insist on their child eating a food, and 35% reported to pressure their child often or all the time [45]. As such, it is not surprising that many parents struggle with the question how to feed their infants effectively. Indeed, 25 to 40% report feeding problems with their infants and toddlers, including picky eating and strong food preferences [46, 47].
In contrast to pressuring children to eat, responsive feeding is often suggested to be the optimal way to feed infants and toddlers [4851]. Responsive feeding is generally defined as a style of feeding in which parents correctly perceive the hunger and satiety signals of the child, and respond promptly and appropriately [50, 52]. This feeding style is suggested to promote and reinforce young children’s ability to self-regulate their energy intake, because the parent who feeds responsively will not override a child’s satiety cues [50]. Indeed, promoting responsive feeding was shown to be associated with a reduced risk of overweight and of rapid weight gain during the first years of life [50, 53, 54]. However, although attending to hunger and satiety signals may promote child self-regulation of energy intake, it may not be sufficient to promote healthy food preferences including vegetable acceptance during the first years of the child’s life. As children from the age of 1.5 years become more and more autonomous and selective about their food preferences, parents have to manage that their child eats appropriate quantities, but also the specific (healthy) foods that are served. To promote healthy food preferences, parents will need to stimulate their child to eat vegetables in a non-pressuring way that is sensitive to the child’s autonomy-related needs and behaviors. This requires more than just responsiveness to hunger and satiety cues, but also sensitive discipline strategies to challenging child behavior (e.g. when a child throws their food on the ground) and sensitive responses to distracted behavior (e.g. when a child is more interested in what is happening around them than in its plate of food). Sensitive discipline strategies that parents may use entail positive encouragement (e.g. explicitly complimenting the child for positive behavior), appropriate pacing to allow the child sufficient time to adapt to the situation, granting the child appropriate amounts of autonomy (e.g. allowing the child to eat autonomously when the child is able to and shows it wants to) and showing understanding for the child’s point of view [55]. Using these sensitive discipline strategies has been shown to promote infant’s committed compliance, i.e. internally motivated and self-regulated adherence to parental rules [56]. In the current study we introduce the concept sensitive feeding to capture this broader set of sensitive parenting skills relevant to promoting children’s committed compliance to parental attempts to feed them healthy foods. Sensitive feeding thus includes the traditional concept of responsive feeding [50, 52], but with the addition of sensitive discipline as well as autonomy support, also in response to non-food related child behaviors during feeding. We hypothesize that parents showing sensitive feeding will be more successful in increasing their children’s vegetable acceptance.
In recent years a number of randomized controlled trials to promote responsive feeding have been performed, some of which incorporated the discipline component described above [5762] whereas others merely focused on teaching parents how to effectively respond to the hunger and satiety cues of their child [53, 54]. However, none of these interventions focused on promoting responsive or sensitive feeding alone. Instead, they incorporated a much broader range of topics such as dietary advice, advice on general feeding practices, guidelines for physical activity, or even more broad advice on how to manage the sleeping and crying behavior of the child. As such, it is impossible to isolate the specific effect of responsive feeding on the diet and eating behavior of the child, and whether this is in fact an element that should be targeted to promote healthy eating patterns. Moreover, all previous trials evaluated changes in parenting behavior via self-report questionnaires, whereas expert observations of parent-child interaction is considered the gold standard to measure parenting behavior [63]. An important disadvantage of self-reports of parenting behavior specifically is that it is questionable whether these data represent the actual parenting behavior parents show, or rather attitudes about what they think they are or should be doing. Indeed, the correlation between self-reported and observed parenting behavior is often low, both in the field of parental feeding [6466] and in other fields [67]. Therefore, we will test the effectiveness of an intervention focusing solely on the enhancement of sensitive feeding, by evaluating its outcomes using repeated observations of family meals at home in addition to self-reports.

Repeated exposure and sensitive feeding

Whether a combination of repeatedly exposing infants to vegetables and encouraging sensitive feeding may lead to a better vegetable intake and liking than each of the interventions alone, has never been tested before. However, there is evidence that presentation of beneficial food choices (succeeding at the what) in a non-responsive manner (failing at the how), and the presentation of unhealthy food choices (failing at the what) in a responsive manner (succeeding at the how) may lead to overweight and eating problems in children [43, 68]. For instance, an experimental study by Galloway and colleagues showed that pressuring a child to eat, even if this pressure is mild in nature, decreases the beneficial effects of repeated exposure to the taste of vegetables [43]. This suggests that an intervention aimed at both elements may be particularly powerful.

Aims and hypotheses

In summary, the Baby’s First Bites (BFB) study aims to test whether promoting the what and/or promoting the how of complementary feeding will result in increased vegetable consumption and liking and a better self-regulation of energy intake in infants and toddlers up until the age of 36 months. To this end, we will perform a superiority randomized controlled trial with parallel groups, comparing a) an intervention focusing on vegetable exposure (=what), b) an intervention focusing on sensitive feeding (=how), c) an intervention focusing on vegetable exposure and sensitive feeding (=what and how), and d) a control condition. The interventions will begin when the infant starts receiving complementary food (child age 4–6 months, as recommended by the Dutch Nutrition Center) and continue until the child is 16 months old. We hypothesize that a) all interventions are more effective in improving vegetable consumption and vegetable liking than the control condition without guidance on complementary feeding; b) the sensitive feeding and combined intervention will be more effective in supporting child self-regulation of energy intake than the vegetable exposure or control conditions; and c) the combined intervention is more effective than the other two interventions alone in promoting vegetable intake and vegetable liking. As the inclusion phase of the BFB study has already successfully been completed, the present article describes the characteristics of the sample of included participants as well as the design of this ongoing study.

Methods/design

Study design

The BFB study is a collaboration between Leiden University, Wageningen University and Research, Danone Nutricia Research and Nutricia Early Life Nutrition. The study is a multicenter trial that is currently being performed at Leiden University and Wageningen University and Research, using a superiority randomized controlled design. The protocol has been approved by the Ethical Review Board of Education and Child Studies, Leiden University (protocol number ECPW-2015/116) and the Medical Ethical Review Board of Wageningen University and Research (METC-WU protocol number NL54422.081.15). The inclusion phase started in May 2016 and ended successfully in November 2017. Mothers and their 4–6 month-old infant were randomly allocated to receive either repeated exposure to a variety of vegetables (RVE), the parenting intervention Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Feeding Infants (VIPP-FI), RVE and VIPP-FI combined, or an attention-control intervention (see Fig.1 and Table 1). Families receiving the RVE intervention were further randomly allocated to one of two types of vegetables the infant is repeatedly exposed to (see Interventions below): green beans or cauliflower. Two target vegetables were chosen as the current feeding schedule is based on the 19-day feeding schedule as described by Barends and colleagues [26, 27]. Green beans and cauliflower are commonly consumed in the Netherlands. Randomization into these conditions was done using the online program TenALEA, which assured that the exact same randomization procedure was used at both study sites [69]. To make the groups allocated to the different conditions as comparable as possible concerning relevant potential confounders, randomization was stratified by age of the child at the start of complementary feeding (4, 5 or 6 months), gender of the child and study location, using minimization procedures. The online randomization program TenALEA has been used previously in other clinical trials [70, 71]). Participants were allocated to a condition by one of the PhD-students or research assistants at each study location.
Table 1
Overview of conditions and intended N per condition
Name
Description of condition
N
RVE
Repeated vegetable exposure intervention:
- exposure to either green beans or cauliflower as target vegetable during the first 19 days of weaning
- five phone calls to motivate parents to expose children to vegetables at child age 4–6, 8, 13 and 16 months
60
VIPP-FI
VIPP-Feeding Infants:
- exposure to fruits and a sweet vegetable (carrots) during the first 19 days of weaning
- five home-visits using video-feedback to promote sensitive feeding at child age 4–6, 8, 13 and 16 months
60
COMBI
Combination of RVE and VIPP-FI
60
AC
Attention control group:
- exposure to fruits and a sweet vegetable (carrots) during the first 19 days of weaning
- five phone calls on development of child at age 4–6, 8, 13 and 16 months
60
Note. RE Repeated exposure, VIPP-FI VIPP-Feeding infants, COMBI Repeated exposure and VIPP-Feeding infants combined, AC Attention-control condition
Intervention effects are assessed both during and after conclusion of the interventions by performing a pre-test at the first two days of complementary feeding (child age 4–6 months; t0), two assessments during the interventions (at the end of the 19-day feeding schedule (child age 5–7 months; t1) and when the child is 12 months old (t12)), a post-test at the age of 18 months (t18) and two follow-ups when the child is 24 (t24) and 36 months old (t36). T0 and t1 are not scheduled at a standard, fixed child age but rather within a certain age range because we wanted to allow parents to start complementary feeding when they thought their child was ready. The other measurements are scheduled at set child ages because the intervention sessions following the very first start of complementary feeding are scheduled at fixed time points (see Timing of intervention sessions below). The timeline for participants is depicted in Table 2. Participants are allowed to stop at any point during the study if they no longer want to participate. If participants decide to withdraw from the study, discontinue an intervention or are unable to complete a specific assessment, they will be asked once whether they would still be willing to complete (parts of) the intervention, the post-test and/or follow-up assessments to come.
Table 2
Timeline for participants
 
Enrolment
Intervention-period
Post-test
Follow-up
Child age (in months)
2–4
4–7
8
12
13
16
18
24
36
Time point
 
t 0
t 1
 
t 12
  
t 18
t 24
t 36
Enrolment & allocation
 1. Invitation e-mail
x
         
 2. Information and informed consent
x
         
 3. Screening
x
         
 4. Allocation
x
         
 5. Rice-flour porridge
x
         
Interventions
 RVE
  Feeding schedule
 
days 1–19
       
  Phone-call
 
Twice in period days 3–17
x
 
x
x
   
  Provision of vegetable purees
 
X
x
x
     
 VIPP-FI
  Feeding schedule
 
days 1–19
       
  Home-visit
 
Twice in period days 3–17
x
 
x
x
   
  Provision of fruit and carrot purees
 
X
x
x
     
 Combined RVE + VIPP-FI
  Feeding schedule
 
days 1–19
       
  Phone-call + home-visit
 
Twice in period days 3–17
x
 
x
x
   
  Provision of vegetable purees
 
X
x
x
     
 Attention-control
  Feeding schedule
 
days 1–19
       
  Phone-call
 
Twice in period days 3–17
x
 
x
x
   
  Provision of fruit and carrot purees
 
X
x
x
     
Assessment of study outcomesa
 
Days 1 + 2
Days 18 + 19
 
x
  
x
x
x
Note. aPrimary outcomes: vegetable intake and liking, child self-regulation of energy intake. Secondary outcomes: child anthropometrics, child eating behavior and maternal feeding behavior. RVE = repeated exposure to vegetables. VIPP-FI=Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Feeding Infants

Calculation of sample size

A power analysis was conducted to calculate the sample size necessary to detect a moderate effect size of .50, which is based on previous studies of the effects of repeated exposure to vegetables [27] and the effects of VIPP [72]. Given a power of .80 and an alpha of .05 the analysis showed that a sample size of 51 participants per group would be sufficient. Taking attrition into account, we aimed to include a total of 240 mothers, 60 per group (see Fig. 1 and Table 1).

Recruitment and participants

We decided to focus all interventions on mothers, because in Dutch households women most often fulfil the role of primary caregiver. Participants were recruited from the general population in four Dutch provinces (Zuid-Holland, Noord-Holland, Gelderland and Utrecht) that are closest to the two universities performing the trial, Leiden University and Wageningen University and Research. Participants were recruited by sending emails with information about the study and a link to the website of the study to mothers of 2–4 month-old infants. Addressees included parents who had signed up for the ‘Nutricia for parents group’ or were parents who had ordered a free gift box containing baby merchandise from ‘WIJ Special Media’. All addressees had indicated that they were interested in receiving information on additional opportunities and/or activities. Names and e-mail addresses were available to only a limited number of researchers, ensuring the privacy of the addressees. Finally, we approached potential participants through handing out brochures at youth health care centers within the vicinity of Wageningen University and Research. We cannot ascertain how many families were invited at the youth health care centers, but the total number of families invited through the two e-mail lists was 5565. A total of 409 families expressed interest in our study, 255 of which fulfilled in- and exclusion criteria (see below) and were randomly allocated to the groups (62.3%; see Fig. 2).
Families that showed interest in our study received a phone-call from one of our trained researchers/students, explaining the study in detail. Families still expressing interest in the study at the end of the call received a detailed information brochure as well as consent forms. Both mothers and fathers were asked to sign and return the consent forms. After receiving the signed consent forms, mothers were asked to fill out an online screening questionnaire which assessed inclusion criteria. Families had to fulfil the following inclusion criteria: a) first-time mothers; b) healthy term infants (37–42 weeks of gestation); c) planning to start complementary feeding at child age of 4–6 months (families that already started complementary feeding were excluded) and d) sufficient knowledge of the Dutch language to receive advice on complementary feeding in Dutch and to be able to fill out Dutch questionnaires. Mothers with major psychiatric diagnoses (e.g., depression, schizophrenia or borderline personality disorder) were excluded, as these may affect parenting [73]. Following the study protocol of Barends and colleagues [26], families were also excluded when the first-borns were twins or in the case of medical problems in the infants that influence the ability to eat, such as food allergies, swallowing or digestion problems. Finally, for standardization purposes, mothers who were not willing to commit to the outcome of the randomization procedure were excluded, e.g. the child was assigned to a VIPP-FI group, but the mother was objecting to being video-taped. A flowchart of the inclusion phase can be found in Fig. 2.
In total, 255 first-time mothers and their babies were randomly allocated to the various conditions. Directly after randomization, prior to starting the intervention-phase, 12 mother-infant dyads dropped out (for reasons, see Fig. 2). A total of 243 families successfully started the intervention-phase. Mean age of the mothers was 30.4 years (sd = 4.7, range 18–44). Concerning educational level, 41.6% of mothers had a lower education (finished high school or vocational school), 38.7% finished higher education (higher vocational school) and 19.8% finished university. The trial was thus successful in including a large group with lower education, which is generally considered a risk factor for having less healthy eating habits [74] and less beneficial parental feeding styles [75]. About 18% of mothers worked fulltime, and 63 worked part-time, and 19% did not have paid work Gender of the child was roughly equally distributed (47.3% boys); mean age of the children at the start of the intervention-phase was 4.68 months (sd = .42, range 3.98–6.38 months); median age was 4.57 months.

Interventions

The specific content and timing of the RVE and VIPP-FI interventions are specified in Table 1. To control for possible placebo-effects due to receiving attention from researchers/interveners, the number of contacts with researchers/interveners and time in between contacts are the same for all conditions. The interventions in all groups as well as the attention control condition is performed by trained researchers or Master’s students in the fields of nutrition or child and family studies. Participants in all conditions are allowed to seek any type of concomitant advice on infant feeding during the trial; to control for potential co-intervention bias we ask participants after the interventions are completed whether they sought advice concerning feeding elsewhere, and if so, where and how often.

All groups/conditions

Feeding schedule and provision of foods in all groups

Prior to the start of each intervention, all mothers are instructed to give their infant rice-flour porridge with a spoon for at least five days, to accustom the infant to eating food from a spoon [26]. Each intervention starts with providing infants their first bites of complementary foods according to a specific 19-day feeding schedule (see Table 3). The infants in the repeated exposure and combined conditions receive a variety of commercially available jars of vegetable purees, whereas the infants in the VIPP-FI and attention-control condition receive similar jars containing both fruits and a sweet vegetable puree (carrots). During the first two days and the last two days of the feeding schedule, the target and control vegetables (cauliflower and green beans) are provided to infants in all conditions. During these days, families are visited at home by the research team and the feed is videotaped; researchers measure at home how much the child has eaten (see Measures). During the other days of the feeding schedule, the mother feeds her child at home without the presence of the researchers. To facilitate compliance to the feeding schedule, mothers receive a printed overview of the feeding schedule indicating which puree to feed their child on each of the 19 days. In addition, each jar of food is labelled with a sticker indicating the day of the feeding schedule.
Table 3
Feeding schedules used within each intervention group and the control group
 
Day
Condition
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
RE and COMBI
TV
CV
TV
V1
TV
V2
TV
V1
TV
V2
TV
V1
TV
V2
TV
V1
TV
CV
TV
VIPP-FI and AC
CF
GB
F1
F2
F3
V3
F1
F2
F3
V3
F1
F2
F3
V3
F1
F2
F3
GB
CF
Note. RE Repeated exposure, COMBI Repeated exposure and VIPP-Feeding infants combined, VIPP-FI VIPP-Feeding infants, AC Attention-control, TV Target vegetable (either green beans or cauliflower), CV Control vegetable (either green beans or cauliflower); V1 Spinach, V2 Broccoli, CF Cauliflower, GB Green beans, F1 Apple, F2 Pear, F3 Banana; V3 Carrot
After this feeding schedule has been completed, all families are provided with a total of 100 jars of age-appropriate fruits and/or meals with vegetables, depending on the condition they are in, up until the child is approximately 12 months of age (distributed on five different occasions; 20 jars per occasion). Parents are free to decide whether they want to feed their baby using homemade foods or the jars provided to them. The provision of these foods serves as a means to facilitate prolonged exposure to vegetables in the repeated vegetable exposure conditions by making sure age-appropriate meals containing vegetables are available to the families. Whether or not families use these jars and how much the child eats of these jars is reported by the mother.

Timing of intervention sessions

The five sessions of each intervention and the phone calls in the control condition are timed to take place when the infant goes through major transitions in eating (see Table 2). It was decided to give advice specifically during these major transitions to optimize the potential effectiveness of the interventions. The first two sessions are scheduled when the infant has just started eating complementary foods (approximately one and two weeks after the start). The third session is scheduled when the child reaches the age of 8 months and parents should start introducing their child to more lumpy foods to facilitate their infants’ acceptance of different food textures [76]. The fourth session is scheduled when the child is approximately 13 months and is allowed to eat the same foods as the rest of the family. Finally, the fifth session is scheduled when the child is 16 months of age to prepare parents for the potentially difficult ‘food neophobic phase’ that infants tend to reach in their second year [39, 77].

Repeated exposure to a variation of vegetables (RVE)

The repeated vegetable exposure (RVE) intervention focuses on what to feed infants. The RVE intervention starts with vegetables only according to a 19-day feeding schedule as described by Barends and colleagues [26, 27], and further promotes vegetable exposure in the first year of complementary feeding until 16 months of age using a protocol developed specifically for the current study. We conducted a needs assessment and applied the Intervention Mapping (IM) process [7880] to develop this protocol.
In short, to promote vegetable exposure in the first year of eating complementary foods the method of repeated exposure to vegetables is used because it has been found to be the most effective way to increase vegetable intake and liking in infants [40, 81]. To support this method, we motivate mothers both during and after the feeding schedule to offer their child vegetables daily. From an analysis of risk factors and determinants that may influence children’s vegetable consumption we selected the determinants knowledge, attitude, self-efficacy, skills, modelling, availability of vegetables, beliefs of the parent, positive reinforcement, and costs to target in the intervention.
The main goal of the RVE intervention is for mothers to increase the child’s acceptance and liking of vegetables by a) starting the first 19 days of complementary feeding with vegetables only and b) offer vegetables to their child daily after this first period. The risk factors and determinants described above are targeted with the feeding schedule and the five telephone calls. Each phone call focuses on a different theme (Table 4) and discusses basic information material and optional additional information material that is sent to mothers by post. Mothers are asked to read the basic information before the scheduled telephone call with the researcher. Conversations are structured according to the general principles of Motivational Interviewing (MI) [82]. Interveners are instructed to act as a coach and guide mothers through the feeding schedule and – during later sessions – the family meal. The telephone protocol contains guidelines with questions mothers might ask and possible responses.
Table 4
Content of each of the RVE and VIPP-FI intervention sessions
Session
Child age
RVE
VIPP-FI
Theme
Topics discussed
Optional information
Situation filmed
Topics discussed
1
4–6 m
Discovering vegetables
• Why should children learn to eat vegetables?
• Keep offering, also if child rejects (at least 10 times)
• Benefits of eating vegetables
• Development of taste in young children
Mother feeding infant pureed vegetables/fruits
Learn to observe and interpret child feeding cues (hunger, satiation, liking)
2
4–6 m
Keep on offering vegetables
• How long should I persist? (at least 10 times)
• Daily variation, steady increase of portion size
• Tips about offering vegetables to children on a daily basis and the preparation of age appropriate vegetable meals
Mother feeding infant pureed vegetables/fruits
Five tips: Timing, routine, adequate pacing, stop at the right time, enjoy
3
8 m
Being creative with vegetables
• Increase level and variety of texture
• Set a good example
• Additional information about introducing more lumpy foods to children.
• Tips about preparing and storing age appropriate vegetable meals
• Tips to cut costs
Child eating sandwich with mother; new topping on sandwich
What to do when infants a) want more autonomy during mealtimes and b) don’t want to eat
4
13 m
Vegetables are part of a balanced diet
• Eating with the whole family
• Recommendations for vegetable intake
• Achieving the recommended intake for vegetables
Dinner with whole family; child is served a new vegetable
Positive ways of dealing with negative behavior during dinner
5
16 m
Keep eating vegetables
• Inform parents on possible food neophobia phase, and how to respond
• Involving children in the preparation of vegetables
Dinner with whole family; child is served something new
Inform parents on possible food neophobia phase, and how to respond to that
Note. m = months
The Stages of Change Model [83] is used to achieve behavior change. The model identifies five stages that people move through when modifying behavior; 1) pre-contemplation; 2) contemplation; 3) preparation; 4) action; 5) maintenance. During the first two sessions (during the 19 day feeding schedule) it is assumed that mothers are motivated to offer their child a vegetable puree daily (preparation/action phase). For session three to five, the stage of change is monitored based on the conversation with the mother. When the mother appears not to be motivated to offer vegetables or encounters barriers in doing so, the protocol contains a series of possible questions and arguments to be discussed to motivate or come up with solutions for the encountered barriers.
Interveners are explicitly not allowed to give advice on how to feed the infant to avoid overlap with the VIPP-FI intervention. If mothers have any specific questions about feeding issues, they are referred to their youth health care center or the website of the Dutch? Nutrition Centre where parents get standard advice available for the general public.
In summary, the standardized telephone protocol for each intervention session contains the following elements:
  • General part with standardized questions about adherence of mother and child to the vegetable guidelines
  • Classifying the stage of change
  • Testing the extent to which goals (e.g. knowledge of the topics discussed) of the previous session were achieved by asking questions and repeating information when necessary (sessions 2, 3, 4, 5)
  • Discussing the basic information material that mothers receive per post and presenting the option to tailor the conversation by addressing the optional information and questions the mother might have
  • Discussing continuation and goal setting with regard to vegetable consumption (sessions 2, 3, 4, 5)
To optimize adherence of interveners to the intervention protocol, interveners familiarize themselves with all the information in the protocol and are trained on how to approach the mothers during the telephone calls. In addition, the interveners have regular meetings to discuss the RVE intervention, exchange experiences and discuss difficulties that may arise. To allow further monitoring of adherence and achievement of the intervention goals, notes are made of each interaction with the parent. In addition, important individual details and information discussed are noted.

VIPP-feeding infants (VIPP-FI)

The VIPP-Feeding Infants intervention focuses on how to feed an infant. The intervention is based on an existing parenting intervention that has repeatedly been proven effective in enhancing both parental sensitivity in general and sensitive discipline in particular: the Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD) [84]. For the present study, the VIPP-SD was adapted to the specific situation of feeding infants (VIPP-FI) and aims to enhance sensitive parenting during feeding. The intervention consists of five sessions that take place at home and makes use of a detailed protocol that can be requested from the first author, SV. To avoid overlap with the RVE intervention, interveners are explicitly not allowed to give any advice on what type of food to give the infant. If mothers have any specific questions about this, they are referred to their youth health care center or the Dutch Nutrition Centre.
The goal of VIPP-FI is to increase mothers’ sensitive reactions to her child’s hunger and satiety cues and to increase sensitive discipline and autonomy support during feeding. To reach this goal, mothers are shown videotapes of their own feeding-interaction with their infant and receive feedback on these tapes by a trained intervener. For each session a different type of meal-setting is filmed. The videos also include potentially challenging situations like introducing the child to a new taste. The mealtimes are filmed approximately one week before the session takes place, to allow the intervener to prepare the feedback they want to give mothers. The different settings that are filmed and topics that are discussed during each session are displayed in Table 4.
One of the core principles of VIPP is to always provide positive feedback to a mother [72]. Every moment where a mother shows sensitive ways of responding to infant cues of hunger, satiety, or other cues are pointed out during the sessions. Instances of insensitive behavior by the mother during the video are also discussed but the intervener always provides the mother with an alternative by referring to a more sensitive response that the mother showed during the video. In doing so, the mother becomes her own role model for showing sensitive reactions to the infant’s needs. Another core principle of VIPP is that to improve maternal sensitivity, mothers need to be trained in observing and interpreting the behavior of their child (in essence, how does my child signal hunger, satiety, interest in their surroundings, etc. [72]). Therefore, during the first sessions mothers do not get direct feedback on their own behavior, as this likely distracts them from observing the behavior of their infant while watching the video. In the standard VIPP protocol mothers do not get specific feedback on their own behavior until the third session. However, in VIPP-FI we allow interveners to do this from the second half of the second session. We made this alteration as there is a relatively long time gap between the second and third session (2 to 4 months) and we wanted to give mothers as many pointers as possible to practice sensitive feeding in the months between the sessions. Examples of techniques used for providing feedback to mothers are speaking for the child (i.e. the intervener stops the video and talks with a mother about what the infant is trying to communicate at that point in the video) and corrective messages (i.e. the intervener stops the video after an example of insensitive behavior of the mother and gives an example of a more sensitive approach she could have used and showed at another point during the video).
To ensure the adherence of interveners to the intervention protocol, interveners receive five days of training in VIPP-SD and a one-day training in VIPP-FI. Moreover, they perform the VIPP-FI in one pilot-family before performing the intervention for the present trial. The progress of the intervention in this pilot-family is discussed extensively with interveners who have experience with the VIPP-FI protocol. Adherence is further optimized by scheduling regular meetings with all interveners at each study location, where the progress of each family receiving the intervention is discussed, as well as any issues that may arise while providing the interventions. Finally, the interveners from both study sites have regular meetings to make sure that adherence is similar at both sites. Similar to the procedure in the RVE intervention, notes are made of each interaction with the parent to allow further monitoring of adherence and achievement of the intervention goals. In addition, important individual details and information discussed are noted.

Vegetable exposure + VIPP-feeding infants (COMBI)

Participants randomly allocated to the combined intervention receive both the RVE intervention and the VIPP-FI as described above. Similar to these interventions, families receive five phone calls for the RVE intervention and five home visits for VIPP-FI, at the same moments as in the two separate interventions.

Attention control condition (AC)

Participants in the attention control condition receive five phone calls, scheduled at the same time that the intervention sessions in the RVE, VIPP-FI and COMBI conditions take place. The researchers/students that make the phone calls are explicitly not allowed to give any advice on the what and how of complementary feeding; instead, they are instructed to simply inquire after the development of the child, using a semi-structured interview, listen to mothers and show interest and empathy. Topics that are discussed concern the general development of the child (e.g., sleeping behavior, motor development, language development) as well as what the mother’s experiences are with the complementary feeding of her child. If mothers have any specific questions about complementary feeding, they are referred to their youth health care center or the Dutch Nutrition Centre.

Measures

Primary outcome measures

Vegetable intake. For the duration of the 19-day weaning schedule the child’s consumption of the purees is assessed. On days 1, 2, 18, and 19 of the feeding schedule researchers visit the families’ homes and measure the amount of the vegetables the infants eat in grams (maximum of 125 g per day, as this is the amount available per day). This is done by weighing the jar of food, bowl, spoon, bib and the cloth mother plans to clean the baby with both before and after the meal by using a standard small kitchen scale (Soehnle, Fiesta 65106). For the other days of the feeding schedule, mothers are asked to put all the leftover puree back in the jar as precisely as possible and store it in the fridge until the researchers collect the jars of food at day 18. The researchers determine the amount of puree eaten on these days by weighing the jars.
At t12, t18, t24, and t36 vegetable intake is measured by asking mothers to fill out web-based 24-h recalls on three randomly assigned, non-consecutive days using the online program, Compl-eat, developed by Wageningen University and Research. Compl-eat is based on the multiple pass method [85] to increase accuracy of dietary recalls and uses the Dutch food composition table [86] to calculate energy and nutrient intake. The program was adapted to assess the diets of infants and young children for this study (e.g., inclusion of smaller portion sizes, and special baby foods). The recall days are scheduled in advance. The parent is provided with a paper food diary to be filled out throughout the day if the child is not in the parents care, but for instance with a babysitter or at a day-care center, making it possible for the parent to enter the data in Compl-eat afterwards. In addition, the parent is asked to weigh all vegetables consumed by the child on a digital scale. Instructions on how to fill out Compl-eat are given during the home visits of t12, t18, t24, and t36; invitations to fill out the recalls are sent after the home visits.
Vegetable liking is measured every day of the feeding schedule by asking mothers to note their infants liking of the vegetables in a diary. Using the same scale as used in the trial by Barends and colleagues (2013), mothers are asked to rate their infant’s liking on a 9-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (dislikes very much) to 9 (likes very much). At t12, t18, t24, and t36, liking of the target and control vegetables (cauliflower and green beans) is measured using the same scale, filled out by the mother.
Child self-regulation of energy-intake is measured using questionnaires and observation. Mothers are asked to fill out the Baby Eating Behavior Questionnaire (BEBQ [87]) at t0 and the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire – Toddler (CEBQ-T [88]) at all other t’s. The BEBQ and CEBQ-T assess several aspects of eating behavior including satiety responsiveness and food responsiveness. These scales are used as indicators of the infant’s self-regulation of energy-intake.
In addition, at t18, t24 and t36, a home-based eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) paradigm is used. This is done according to the free-access procedure, which is considered the gold-standard for this type of measurement [8992]. During the home visit the researcher carefully assesses what and how much the child eats during dinner to determine the weight, energy and macronutrient content of the meal. In addition, the mother is asked to indicate how satiated she thinks her child is after consuming dinner. Directly after dinner an 8-min free play session takes place after which the researcher provides a plate with savory and sweet age-appropriate snacks and the child is told that these are for him/her to eat. The mother is asked not to interfere with the child’s behavior during this time. Using these data, the EAH-score, the percentage of energy intake from the snacks relative to the energy intake from the dinner, is calculated.

Secondary outcome measures

Child anthropometrics are measured at all t’s. Infants’ body weight is measured by asking mothers to first stand on a calibrated electronic personal scale (KERN MPC/SECA robusta 813) themselves, and then again while holding their infant. The difference between these two weights produces the child’s weight. As of t24, children are invited to stand on the scales themselves. Weight is measured in 0.1 kg. Infants’ length is measured by lying them down on a small mat with an indication of centimeters printed on top of it. As of t24 child length is measured using a stadiometer (SECA 213, Chino, USA/Garant).
Child eating behavior is measured by the mother-reported Baby Eating Behavior Questionnaire at t0 (BEBQ [87]) and the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire – Toddler (CEBQ-T [88]) at all other t’s. The BEBQ and CEBQ-T are both derived from the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), a well-validated, reliable and widely used questionnaire that assesses different aspects of child eating behavior [93, 94]. We use the CEBQ-T as of t1 as it is more appropriate for assessing children’s eating behavior in relation to eating solid foods. However, since the scale ‘emotional over-eating’ is largely inapplicable for infants under the age of 2 years (e.g., “My child eats more when upset”) this scale is only added to t18, t24 and t36.
Maternal feeding behavior is measured using both observations of family meals at home and questionnaires. When the child is 4–7 months of age (t0 and t1), a videotape is made of the mother feeding the child one of the pureed foods of the feeding schedule. At all other time points, a family dinner is videotaped. These videos are coded by trained researchers/students for maternal sensitive feeding using the Ainsworth scale [95]. In addition, maternal responsiveness to child satiety cues is coded using a scale based on the Responsiveness to Child Feeding Cues Scale [96], and maternal pressure to eat is coded using a scale based on a large Dutch study that observed family meals in 4–6 year-olds [66].
In addition, at each time point the Infant Feeding Style Questionnaire [97] is administered. This questionnaire has shown adequate internal consistency and validity and measures the following parental feeding styles: laissez-faire, restrictive, pressuring, responsive and indulgent. As of t18 the following scales from the validated Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire [98, 99] are added which are appropriate at that age: restriction, monitoring, modelling, encourage balance and variety, pressure to eat, child control, emotion regulation and food as reward. Scales from the Feeding Practices and Structures Questionnaire [100] are also added as of t18 (reward for eating, overt restriction) and t24 (reward for behavior, persuasive feeding, structured meal setting, structured meal timing).

Other measures

The following potential covariates will be assessed: demographic variables such as maternal and paternal education and job status, family income, cultural background (t0); type of milk feeding (breast/formula: t0-t18); maternal depression (t0-t36: Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale [101]); maternal vegetable intake (t12 and t36: Food frequency questionnaire [102]); maternal anthropometrics (t0-t36); use and amount of purée consumed of the 100 distributed vegetable- and fruit jars in the 5 months after the feeding schedule (t12); maternal self-efficacy related to feeding their child (t0-t36: Parental Feeding Self-Efficacy Questionnaire [103]); maternal emotions during feeding the child (t0-t36: measure designed for this study); structure of family meals (t0-t36: Meals in our Household [104]); maternal perception of feeding (t0-t36: Five Minute Speech Sample [105]); child temperament (t0-t12: Infant Behavior Questionnaire-Revised [106]; t18-t36: Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire [107]); general parenting styles (t0-t36: observed maternal intrusiveness during mealtimes and observed maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness during free-play situations [95]; t18-t36: Comprehensive General Parenting Questionnaire [108]).

Blinding

Researchers coding video data are blinded for intervention-allocation. It is impossible to blind participants for intervention-allocation, because they will be informed prior to randomization about what types of advice they can receive in the study and it will be clear after randomization what type of advice they are receiving.

Participant reimbursement and efforts to prevent drop-out

As a compensation for the time and effort participants invest in our study, families receive several compensations. Apart from the pureed vegetables or fruits during the feeding schedule and the 100 jars of baby foods until the infant is 12 months of age, families receive gift tokens of 25 euros and a gift for the child of approximately 5 euros at t18, t24, and t36. Additionally, all videos made throughout the study are shared with the families at completion of the study, and families randomly allocated to receive VIPP-FI receive the videos used for the intervention during the last session of the intervention.
To involve participants in the study we will send families biannual newsletters about the study, mentioning interesting facts (e.g., inclusion rates, presentations at symposia, pictures of researchers/students involved in the project). Also, we aim to stimulate a pleasant relationship between researchers and participating mothers by for example sending birthday cards to the family when the child will have its birthday. In a similar effort, and to diminish any additional burden for the participating families, we will strive to provide continuity in the researchers/students that are in direct contact with a family (e.g., at home visits or telephone calls). Moreover, we will make sure during every home visit to check whether participants have any questions about the measurements and/or interventions and to provide assistance in filling out questionnaires or dietary recalls whenever needed.

Confidentiality, data management and access

All data will be stored using numbers to identify participants at the secured databases of Leiden University and Wageningen University and Research. Only one document exists that links participant numbers to personal data, and this file is only available to the main researchers performing data collection at Leiden University and Wageningen University. Data that need to be entered manually (e.g., measured weight and height during home visits, codes of video material) will be entered in the latest version of the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics by trained researchers/students. The quality of this data entry will be checked regularly by another (independent) trained researcher/student.
As detailed in the consortium agreement-contract of the project, only researchers and students involved in the project working at any of the academic parties (Leiden University, Wageningen University and Research) will be allowed access to the data. With the exception of the video-recordings (VIPP-FI), which contain privacy-sensitive information, research data will be open access where possible (e.g. when a peer-reviewed journal requests or offers the uploading of anonymized datasets into an open access database. In these cases, all personal information will be removed from data files and replaced by participant identification numbers. The file linking these numbers to personal information will be stored digitally in a separate password protected file that will only be accessible to the researchers). Large video-files will be shared between the two universities by making copies on external pass-word protected hard-drives and personally exchanging these hard-drives.

Analyses

The intention-to-treat principle will be applied to all analyses. Whether the interventions differentially affect primary and secondary outcomes over time will be analyzed using linear mixed models analyses, a technique that makes use of every data point for every participant, irrespective of their missing data. The three intervention groups will be compared to the control group, and the combined group will be compared to the repeated exposure and the VIPP-FI group. A significance level of α = .05 will be used. The analyses will be corrected for relevant covariates such as family socioeconomic status, maternal consumption of vegetables, parental body mass index (BMI), child temperament, etc.

Monitoring of interventions and trial progression

Participants will be asked to fill out an evaluation form concerning the interventions following the last session. These forms will assess participants’ satisfaction with the intervention as well as with the person delivering the intervention. In addition, participants will be asked to note any other comments about the interventions, allowing for spontaneously reported adverse events. As the interventions are not invasive and merely provide parents support, advice and commercially available foods with a history of safe use, no adverse events are expected and no stopping guidelines are formulated. For the same reasons, a data management committee is not needed. Principle investigators at each study site (i.e. JM, SV, KG, JV and GJ) will supervise data collection and data management. We will not perform any interim analyses as we want to avoid the risk of the results of such analyses influencing the overall results of the trial. No explicit trial conduct audit is planned; however, yearly reports on the progress of the project will be sent to the major funder of the trial (The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research). If any major changes will occur in the study protocol (e.g., changes to outcomes or assessment periods) the ethical review boards that approved the study as well as the funder of the trial will be notified of these changes.

Dissemination policy

It is planned to publish the results of our trial in peer-reviewed journals, as well as present the results at (inter) national conferences. Also, participants will receive a report of the results of our study after completion of the study. Publication in magazines for healthcare professionals and the general public are also intended. Authorship to any publications will be granted to those who fulfil the ICMJE recommendations [109]. We will not hire any professional writers.

Discussion

Baby’s First Bites will be the first trial explicitly testing the separate and combined effects of promoting the what and how of complementary feeding. By comparing three prolonged, intensive interventions, we will be able to draw firm conclusions on what is most important to focus on when promoting vegetable acceptance and children’s self-regulation of energy intake in early childhood; what food to offer, how to offer this food, or a combination of the two. Moreover, this will be the first trial to include an intervention specifically manipulating sensitive feeding practices without manipulating any other variables, evaluating its effects using both self-report and observational measures. This allows conclusions on whether this parenting practice will indeed promote healthier food preferences in children and will foster children’s ability to self-regulate their energy intake, as is often suggested in the literature.
The planned study also provides some points of discussion to be considered. First, the channels of recruitment we have chosen pose the risk that participating families are not representative of the general population, as they are partly recruited from a database of pregnant women who showed interest in information about infant nutrition. Thus, these families may be more motivated to provide a healthy eating environment for their infant than the general public. However, it should be noted that time-consuming randomized controlled trials (RCTs) like the present study will always elicit this potential selection bias, irrespective of the channels of recruitment chosen. Also, this drawback is negated somewhat by the fact that this study succeeded in including participants at all educational levels. Nevertheless, this potential selection bias should be taken into account when considering the implementation of the results of this study. Second, we chose to give parents the opportunity to start complementary feeding from the age of 4 months, thereby making sure that we followed parental preferences in starting complementary feeding. There is still some discussion in the literature about when to start complementary feeding. The general recommendation from the World Health Organization (WHO) is to exclusively breastfeed until the age of 6 months and introduce complementary foods from 6 months [1]. For the European Region, WHO [110] recommend that all infants should be exclusively breastfed from birth to about 6 months of age, and at least for the first 4 months of life, but that some infants may need complementary foods before 6 months of age, and that these should not be introduced before 4 months. The European Food and Safety Authority (EFSA) panel [111], the European Society for Paediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition (ESPGHAN) [112] recommend that complementary foods including allergens are introduced between 4 and 6 months, and this has been shown to be associated with a reduced risk of food allergies [113]. Starting complementary feeding between 4 and 6 months is also in accordance with recommendations from the Dutch Nutrition Centre [114] and the Dutch youth health care centers [115] and thus reflects official Dutch guidelines and probably the daily practice of parents in the Netherlands.
Third, we chose to deliver the combined intervention by simply following the same procedures as used in each separate intervention, and the intervention was provided by two different researchers/students (one delivering RVE, and one delivering VIPP-FI). As such, it can be debated whether this really constitutes a combined intervention or simply two interventions. Also, from the families’ point of view, receiving advice from two different persons might not be ideal. An alternative approach would have been to incorporate all information of both interventions in the home visits. However, we decided against this as the VIPP-FI home visits already took up 60 to 90 min. Including the information of the RVE intervention in this session would result in too much information for the mother to properly process in one sitting, increasing the risk that the effects of the intervention would diminish. Fourth, considering the time-consuming nature of this study for families, there will be a considerable risk of drop-out during the study. This risk is even higher in the selected sample of first-time mothers, as it is likely that many families will expand their family during the study period, making the time they have available for participating in this study more limited. We plan to accommodate families as much as possible to make sure that they will be able to finish the study, for instance by offering assistance where necessary (e.g., filling out questionnaires together or sending personal reminders) and by being flexible in planning the home-visits.
Finally, if the proposed RCT will prove the interventions effective, the labor intensiveness of the tested interventions may pose problems for their implementation to the general public. Although this is not so much a limitation of the current study, it is a drawback for implementing its results, as it will be necessary to translate the interventions to scalable prevention programs before the interventions can be implemented for a larger group.
In conclusion, the planned trial has the potential to provide valid evidence on the question how parents may promote healthy eating habits from the very first start of eating solid foods. If proven effective, these interventions could be useful to large scale effective prevention of childhood obesity.
Baby’s First Bites has been approved by the Medical Ethical Review Board of Wageningen University (METC-WU protocol number NL54422.081.15) and the Ethical Review Board of Education and Child Studies, Leiden University (protocol number ECPW-2015/116). Written informed consent will be obtained from both parents before families enter the study.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

Danone Nutricia Research and Nutricia Early Life Nutrition has provided additional funding for Baby’s First Bites (see Funding).
C. Vereijken (CV) and H. Weenen (HW) are employees of Danone Nutricia Research, N. van Winden (NW) is employee of Nutricia Early Life Nutrition, The authors declare that they have no other competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Ocké MC, van Rossum CTM, Fransen HP, Buurma EJM, de Boer EJ, Brants HAM, et al. Dutch national food consumption survey - young children 2005/2006. Bilthoven; 2008. Ocké MC, van Rossum CTM, Fransen HP, Buurma EJM, de Boer EJ, Brants HAM, et al. Dutch national food consumption survey - young children 2005/2006. Bilthoven; 2008.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldbohm RA, Rubingh CM, Lanting CI, Joosten KFM. Food consumption and nutrient intake by children aged 10 to 48 months attending day Care in the Netherlands. Nutrients. 2016;8(7):13.CrossRef Goldbohm RA, Rubingh CM, Lanting CI, Joosten KFM. Food consumption and nutrient intake by children aged 10 to 48 months attending day Care in the Netherlands. Nutrients. 2016;8(7):13.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Emmett PM, Jones LR. Diet, growth, and obesity development throughout childhood in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Nutr Rev. 2015;73:175–206.PubMedCrossRef Emmett PM, Jones LR. Diet, growth, and obesity development throughout childhood in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Nutr Rev. 2015;73:175–206.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox MK, Pac S, Devaney B, Jankowski L. Feeding infants and toddlers study: what foods are infants and toddlers eating? J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(1):S22–30.PubMedCrossRef Fox MK, Pac S, Devaney B, Jankowski L. Feeding infants and toddlers study: what foods are infants and toddlers eating? J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(1):S22–30.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Denney LA, Afeiche MC, Eldridge AL, Villalpando-Carrión S. Food sources of energy and nutrients in infants, toddlers, and young children from the Mexican national health and nutrition survey 2012. Nutrients. 2017;9:1–27.CrossRef Denney LA, Afeiche MC, Eldridge AL, Villalpando-Carrión S. Food sources of energy and nutrients in infants, toddlers, and young children from the Mexican national health and nutrition survey 2012. Nutrients. 2017;9:1–27.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Fogel A, Mccrickerd K, Fries LR, Goh AT, Quah PL, Chang MJ, et al. Eating in the absence of hunger: stability over time and associations with eating behaviours and body composition in children. Physiol Behav. 2018;192:82–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Fogel A, Mccrickerd K, Fries LR, Goh AT, Quah PL, Chang MJ, et al. Eating in the absence of hunger: stability over time and associations with eating behaviours and body composition in children. Physiol Behav. 2018;192:82–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hesketh K, Wake M, Waters E, Carlin J, Crawford D. Stability of body mass index in Australian children: a prospective cohort study across the middle childhood years. Public Health Nutr. 2003;7(2):303–9.CrossRef Hesketh K, Wake M, Waters E, Carlin J, Crawford D. Stability of body mass index in Australian children: a prospective cohort study across the middle childhood years. Public Health Nutr. 2003;7(2):303–9.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Katzmarzyk PT, Pérusse L, Malina RM, Bouchard C. Seven-year stability of indicators of obesity and adipose tissue distribution in the Canadian population. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;69:1123–9.PubMedCrossRef Katzmarzyk PT, Pérusse L, Malina RM, Bouchard C. Seven-year stability of indicators of obesity and adipose tissue distribution in the Canadian population. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;69:1123–9.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Camfferman R, Jansen P, Rippe R, Mesman J, Derkx IP, Tiemeier H, et al. The association between overweight and internalizing and externalizing behavior in early childhood. Soc Sci Med. 2016;168:35–42.PubMedCrossRef Camfferman R, Jansen P, Rippe R, Mesman J, Derkx IP, Tiemeier H, et al. The association between overweight and internalizing and externalizing behavior in early childhood. Soc Sci Med. 2016;168:35–42.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Rotteveel J, Felius A, Van Weissenburch MM. Delemarre-Van de Waal HA. Insulin resistance and the metabolic syndrome in obese children referred to an obesity center. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab. 2010;23(9):943–51.PubMedCrossRef Rotteveel J, Felius A, Van Weissenburch MM. Delemarre-Van de Waal HA. Insulin resistance and the metabolic syndrome in obese children referred to an obesity center. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab. 2010;23(9):943–51.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Sinha R, Fisch G, Teague B, Tamborlane WV, Banyas B, Allen K, et al. Prevalence of impaired glucose tolerance among children and adolescents with marked obesity. N Engl J Med. 2002;346(11):802–10.PubMedCrossRef Sinha R, Fisch G, Teague B, Tamborlane WV, Banyas B, Allen K, et al. Prevalence of impaired glucose tolerance among children and adolescents with marked obesity. N Engl J Med. 2002;346(11):802–10.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ajala O, Mold F, Boughton C, Cooke D, Whyte M. Childhood predictors of cardiovascular disease in adulthood. Obes Rev: A systematic review and meta-analysis; 2017. Ajala O, Mold F, Boughton C, Cooke D, Whyte M. Childhood predictors of cardiovascular disease in adulthood. Obes Rev: A systematic review and meta-analysis; 2017.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Maynard M, Gunnell D, Emmett PM, Frankel S, Davey SG. Fruit, vegetables, and antioxidants in childhood and risk of adult cancer: the Boyd Orr cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:218–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maynard M, Gunnell D, Emmett PM, Frankel S, Davey SG. Fruit, vegetables, and antioxidants in childhood and risk of adult cancer: the Boyd Orr cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:218–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cashdan E. A sensitive period for learning about food. Human Nutrition. 1994;5(3):279–91. Cashdan E. A sensitive period for learning about food. Human Nutrition. 1994;5(3):279–91.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34(6):310–5.PubMedCrossRef Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34(6):310–5.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issancou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite. 2005;44(3):289–97.PubMedCrossRef Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issancou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite. 2005;44(3):289–97.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Mallan KM, Fildes A, Magarey AM, Daniels LA. The relationship between number of fruits, vegetables, and noncore foods tried at age 14 months and food preferences, dietary intake patterns, fussy eating behavior, and weight status at age 3.7 years. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(4):630–7.PubMedCrossRef Mallan KM, Fildes A, Magarey AM, Daniels LA. The relationship between number of fruits, vegetables, and noncore foods tried at age 14 months and food preferences, dietary intake patterns, fussy eating behavior, and weight status at age 3.7 years. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(4):630–7.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Birch LL, Davison KK. Family environmental factors influencing the developing behavioral controls of food intake and childhood overweight. Childhood and Adolescent Obesity. 2001;48(4):893–907. Birch LL, Davison KK. Family environmental factors influencing the developing behavioral controls of food intake and childhood overweight. Childhood and Adolescent Obesity. 2001;48(4):893–907.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox MK, Devaney B, Reidy K, Razafindrakoto C, Ziegler P. Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(1):s77–83.PubMedCrossRef Fox MK, Devaney B, Reidy K, Razafindrakoto C, Ziegler P. Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(1):s77–83.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Chambers L. Complementary feeding: vegetables first, frequently and in variety. Nutr Bull. 2016;41:142–6.CrossRef Chambers L. Complementary feeding: vegetables first, frequently and in variety. Nutr Bull. 2016;41:142–6.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA. Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2001;73(6):1080–5.PubMedCrossRef Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA. Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2001;73(6):1080–5.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Chambers L, Hetherington M, Cooke L, Coulthard H, Fewtrell MS, Lowdon J, et al. Reaching consensus on a 'vegetables first' approach to complementary feeding. Nutr Bull. 2016;41:270–6.CrossRef Chambers L, Hetherington M, Cooke L, Coulthard H, Fewtrell MS, Lowdon J, et al. Reaching consensus on a 'vegetables first' approach to complementary feeding. Nutr Bull. 2016;41:270–6.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Prefer. 2007;18(8):1023–32.CrossRef Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Prefer. 2007;18(8):1023–32.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Barends C, De Vries JHM, Mojet J, De Graaf C. Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables or fruits on infant's vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:157–65.CrossRef Barends C, De Vries JHM, Mojet J, De Graaf C. Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables or fruits on infant's vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:157–65.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Barends C, De Vries JHM, Mojet J, De Graaf C. Effects of starting weaning exclusively with vegetables on vegetable intake at the age of 12 and 23 months. Appetite. 2014;81:193–9.PubMedCrossRef Barends C, De Vries JHM, Mojet J, De Graaf C. Effects of starting weaning exclusively with vegetables on vegetable intake at the age of 12 and 23 months. Appetite. 2014;81:193–9.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S. Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor-flavor learning and more effectively than flavor-nutrient learning. J Nutr. 2013;143(7):1194–200.PubMedCrossRef Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S. Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor-flavor learning and more effectively than flavor-nutrient learning. J Nutr. 2013;143(7):1194–200.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Exposure to vegetable variety in infants weaned at different ages. Appetite. 2014;78:1–6.CrossRef Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Exposure to vegetable variety in infants weaned at different ages. Appetite. 2014;78:1–6.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington MM, Schwartz C, Madrelle J. A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complimentary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite. 2015;84:280–90.PubMedCrossRef Hetherington MM, Schwartz C, Madrelle J. A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complimentary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite. 2015;84:280–90.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K. Laing DG. Infants' consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods Appetite. 1998;30(3):283–95.PubMed Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K. Laing DG. Infants' consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods Appetite. 1998;30(3):283–95.PubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Daniels LM, Reiter AR. Learning to like vegetables during breastfeeding: a randomized clinical trial of lactating mothers and infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106:67–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mennella JA, Daniels LM, Reiter AR. Learning to like vegetables during breastfeeding: a randomized clinical trial of lactating mothers and infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106:67–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Nehring I, Kostka T, Von Kries R, Rehruess EA. Impacts of in utero and early infant taste experiences on later taste acceptance: a systematic review. J Nutr. 2015;145:1271–9.PubMedCrossRef Nehring I, Kostka T, Von Kries R, Rehruess EA. Impacts of in utero and early infant taste experiences on later taste acceptance: a systematic review. J Nutr. 2015;145:1271–9.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange C, Visalli M, Jacob S, Chabanet P, Schlich S, Nicklaus S. Maternal feeding practices during the first year and their impact on infants' acceptance of complementary food. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:89–98.CrossRef Lange C, Visalli M, Jacob S, Chabanet P, Schlich S, Nicklaus S. Maternal feeding practices during the first year and their impact on infants' acceptance of complementary food. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:89–98.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN. Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infants' acceptance of new foods for up to two months. Clin Nutr. 2008;27:849–57.PubMedCrossRef Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN. Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infants' acceptance of new foods for up to two months. Clin Nutr. 2008;27:849–57.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Fildes A, Lopes C, Moreira P, Moschonis G, Oliveira A, Mavrogianni C, et al. An exploratory trial of parental advice for increasing vegetable acceptance in infancy. Br J Nutr. 2015;114:328–36.PubMedCrossRef Fildes A, Lopes C, Moreira P, Moschonis G, Oliveira A, Mavrogianni C, et al. An exploratory trial of parental advice for increasing vegetable acceptance in infancy. Br J Nutr. 2015;114:328–36.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier-Nöth A, Schaal B, Leathwood P, Issanchou S. The lasting influence of early food-related variety experience: A longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations. PLoS One. 2016;11(3).PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maier-Nöth A, Schaal B, Leathwood P, Issanchou S. The lasting influence of early food-related variety experience: A longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations. PLoS One. 2016;11(3).PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JC. Food neophobia and 'picky/fussy' eating in children: a review. Appetite. 2008;50:181–93.PubMedCrossRef Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JC. Food neophobia and 'picky/fussy' eating in children: a review. Appetite. 2008;50:181–93.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI. Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers' decisions about offering a new food. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:s57–64.PubMedCrossRef Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI. Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers' decisions about offering a new food. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:s57–64.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Birch LL, Doub AE. Learning to eat: birth to age 2 y. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(suppl)(723S–728S).PubMedCrossRef Birch LL, Doub AE. Learning to eat: birth to age 2 y. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(suppl)(723S–728S).PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Mura Paroche M, Caton SJ, Vereijken CMJL, Weenen H, Houston-Price C. How infants and young children learn about food: a systematic review. Front Psychol. 2017;25(8):1046.CrossRef Mura Paroche M, Caton SJ, Vereijken CMJL, Weenen H, Houston-Price C. How infants and young children learn about food: a systematic review. Front Psychol. 2017;25(8):1046.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba B, Steinberg L, Krehbiel R. "clean up your plate": effects of child feeding practices on the conditioning of meal size. Learn Motiv. 1987;18:301–17.CrossRef Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba B, Steinberg L, Krehbiel R. "clean up your plate": effects of child feeding practices on the conditioning of meal size. Learn Motiv. 1987;18:301–17.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Galloway AT, Fiorito LM, Francis LA, Birch LL. "finish your soup": counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite. 2006;46:318–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Galloway AT, Fiorito LM, Francis LA, Birch LL. "finish your soup": counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite. 2006;46:318–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Blissett J, Bennett C, Fogel A, Harris G, Higgs S. Parental modelling and prompting effects on acceptance of a novel fruit in 2–4-year-old children are dependent on children's food responsiveness. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(554–564).CrossRef Blissett J, Bennett C, Fogel A, Harris G, Higgs S. Parental modelling and prompting effects on acceptance of a novel fruit in 2–4-year-old children are dependent on children's food responsiveness. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(554–564).CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Chan L, Magarey AM, Daniels LA. Maternal feeding practices and feeding behaviors of Australian children aged 12-36 months. Matern Child Health J. 2011;15(8):1363–71.PubMedCrossRef Chan L, Magarey AM, Daniels LA. Maternal feeding practices and feeding behaviors of Australian children aged 12-36 months. Matern Child Health J. 2011;15(8):1363–71.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Reau NR, Senturia YD, Lebailly SA, Christoffel KK. Infant and toddler feeding patterns and problems. Normative data and a new direction. Journal of Developmental & Behavioural Pediatrics. 1996;17:149–53.CrossRef Reau NR, Senturia YD, Lebailly SA, Christoffel KK. Infant and toddler feeding patterns and problems. Normative data and a new direction. Journal of Developmental & Behavioural Pediatrics. 1996;17:149–53.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell GL, Farrow C, Haycraft E, Meyer C. Parental influences on children's eating behaviour and characteristics of successful parent-focussed interventions. Appetite. 2013;60:85–94.PubMedCrossRef Mitchell GL, Farrow C, Haycraft E, Meyer C. Parental influences on children's eating behaviour and characteristics of successful parent-focussed interventions. Appetite. 2013;60:85–94.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Reiter AR, Daniels LM. Vegetable and fruit acceptance during infancy: Impact of ontogeny, genetics, and early experiences. Adv Nutr. 2016;7:211s–9s.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mennella JA, Reiter AR, Daniels LM. Vegetable and fruit acceptance during infancy: Impact of ontogeny, genetics, and early experiences. Adv Nutr. 2016;7:211s–9s.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Pérez-Escamilla R, Segura-Perez S, Lott M. Feeding guidelines for infants and young toddlers: a responsive parenting approach. Healthy Eating Research: Durham, NC; 2017.CrossRef Pérez-Escamilla R, Segura-Perez S, Lott M. Feeding guidelines for infants and young toddlers: a responsive parenting approach. Healthy Eating Research: Durham, NC; 2017.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat DiSantis KI, Hodges EA, Johnson SL, Fisher JO. The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review. Int J Obes. 2011;35:480–92.CrossRef DiSantis KI, Hodges EA, Johnson SL, Fisher JO. The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review. Int J Obes. 2011;35:480–92.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S. The role of food experiences during early childhood in food pleasure learning. Appetite. 2016;104:3–9.PubMedCrossRef Nicklaus S. The role of food experiences during early childhood in food pleasure learning. Appetite. 2016;104:3–9.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz C, Scholtens PAMJ, Lalanne A, Weenen H, Nicklaus S. Development of healthy eating habits early in life. Review of recent evidence and selected guidelines. Appetite. 2011;57:796–807.PubMedCrossRef Schwartz C, Scholtens PAMJ, Lalanne A, Weenen H, Nicklaus S. Development of healthy eating habits early in life. Review of recent evidence and selected guidelines. Appetite. 2011;57:796–807.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Savage JS, Birch LL, Marini M, Anzman-Frasca S, Paul IM. Effect of the INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention on rapid infant weight gain and overweight status at age 1 year: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(8):742–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Savage JS, Birch LL, Marini M, Anzman-Frasca S, Paul IM. Effect of the INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention on rapid infant weight gain and overweight status at age 1 year: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(8):742–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Paul IM, Savage JS, Anzman SL, Beiler JS, Marini ME, Stokes JL, et al. Preventing obesity during infancy: a pilot study. Obesity. 2011;19(2):353–61.PubMedCrossRef Paul IM, Savage JS, Anzman SL, Beiler JS, Marini ME, Stokes JL, et al. Preventing obesity during infancy: a pilot study. Obesity. 2011;19(2):353–61.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Mesman J, Stolk MN, Van Zeijl J, Alink LRA, Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, et al. Extending the video-feedback intervention to sensitive discipline. The early prevention of antisocial behavior. In: Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, editors. Promoting Positive Parenting An Attachment-Based Intervention. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2008. p. 171–192. Mesman J, Stolk MN, Van Zeijl J, Alink LRA, Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, et al. Extending the video-feedback intervention to sensitive discipline. The early prevention of antisocial behavior. In: Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, editors. Promoting Positive Parenting An Attachment-Based Intervention. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2008. p. 171–192.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Feldman R, Klein PS. Toddlers' self-regulated compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: implications for theories of socialization. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(4):680–92.PubMedCrossRef Feldman R, Klein PS. Toddlers' self-regulated compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: implications for theories of socialization. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(4):680–92.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Daniels LA, Magarey AM, Battistutta D, Nicholson JM, Farrell A, Davidson G, et al. The NOURISH randomised controlled trial: positive feeding practices and food preferences in early childhood - a primary prevention program for childhood obesity. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:387–97.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Daniels LA, Magarey AM, Battistutta D, Nicholson JM, Farrell A, Davidson G, et al. The NOURISH randomised controlled trial: positive feeding practices and food preferences in early childhood - a primary prevention program for childhood obesity. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:387–97.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Harvey-Berino J, Rourke J. Obesity prevention in preschool native-American children: a pilot study using home visiting. Obes Res. 2003;11(5):606–11.PubMedCrossRef Harvey-Berino J, Rourke J. Obesity prevention in preschool native-American children: a pilot study using home visiting. Obes Res. 2003;11(5):606–11.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Horodynski MAO, Hoerr S, Coleman G. Nutrition education aimed at toddlers. A pilot program for rural, low-income families. Family Community and Health. 2004;27(2):103–13.CrossRef Horodynski MAO, Hoerr S, Coleman G. Nutrition education aimed at toddlers. A pilot program for rural, low-income families. Family Community and Health. 2004;27(2):103–13.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Aboud FE, Moore AC, Akhter S. Effectiveness of a community-based responsive feeding programme in rural Bangladesh: a cluster randomized field trial. Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4:275–86.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Aboud FE, Moore AC, Akhter S. Effectiveness of a community-based responsive feeding programme in rural Bangladesh: a cluster randomized field trial. Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4:275–86.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Willis TA, Roberts KPJ, Berry TM, Bryant M, Rudolf MCJ. The impact of HENRY on parenting and family lifestyle: a national service evaluation of a preschool obesity prevention programme. Public Health. 2016;136:101–8.PubMedCrossRef Willis TA, Roberts KPJ, Berry TM, Bryant M, Rudolf MCJ. The impact of HENRY on parenting and family lifestyle: a national service evaluation of a preschool obesity prevention programme. Public Health. 2016;136:101–8.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Fangupo LJ, Heath ALM, Williams CM, Somerville AD, Lawrence JA, Gray AR, et al. Impact of an early-life intervention on the nutrition behaviors of 2-y-old children: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;102:704–12.PubMedCrossRef Fangupo LJ, Heath ALM, Williams CM, Somerville AD, Lawrence JA, Gray AR, et al. Impact of an early-life intervention on the nutrition behaviors of 2-y-old children: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;102:704–12.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawes DJ, Dadds MR. Assessing parenting practices through parent-report and direct observation during parent-training. J Child Fam Stud. 2006;15:554–67.CrossRef Hawes DJ, Dadds MR. Assessing parenting practices through parent-report and direct observation during parent-training. J Child Fam Stud. 2006;15:554–67.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Sacco LM, Bentley ME, Carby-Shields K, Borja JB, Goldman BD. Assessment of infant feeding styles among low-income African-American mothers. Comparing reported and observed behavior. Appetite. 2007;49:131–40.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sacco LM, Bentley ME, Carby-Shields K, Borja JB, Goldman BD. Assessment of infant feeding styles among low-income African-American mothers. Comparing reported and observed behavior. Appetite. 2007;49:131–40.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis M, Worobey J. Mothers and toddlers lunch together. The relation between observed and reported behavior. Appetite. 2011;56(3):732–6.PubMedCrossRef Lewis M, Worobey J. Mothers and toddlers lunch together. The relation between observed and reported behavior. Appetite. 2011;56(3):732–6.PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Camfferman R. Happy healthy homes. The role of parenting in early childhood overweight (doctoral dissertation). Mostert & Van Onderen: Leiden; 2017. Camfferman R. Happy healthy homes. The role of parenting in early childhood overweight (doctoral dissertation). Mostert & Van Onderen: Leiden; 2017.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Price MN, Hyde JS. Perceived and observed maternal relationship quality predict sexual debut by age 15. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2011;40:1595–606.PubMedCrossRef Price MN, Hyde JS. Perceived and observed maternal relationship quality predict sexual debut by age 15. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2011;40:1595–606.PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Saavedra JM, Deming D, Dattilo A, Reidy K. Lessons from the feeding infants and toddlers study in North America: what children eat, and implications for obesity prevention. Ann Nutr Metab. 2013;62:27–36.PubMedCrossRef Saavedra JM, Deming D, Dattilo A, Reidy K. Lessons from the feeding infants and toddlers study in North America: what children eat, and implications for obesity prevention. Ann Nutr Metab. 2013;62:27–36.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Mathoulin-Pelissier S, Bellera C, Gregoire F, Yang-Ting L, Malfilatre A. Deployment of a centralized randomization service by internet in the trans European network Alea for clinical trials services (TenAlea) project. Bull Cancer. 2008;95:s73–s4. Mathoulin-Pelissier S, Bellera C, Gregoire F, Yang-Ting L, Malfilatre A. Deployment of a centralized randomization service by internet in the trans European network Alea for clinical trials services (TenAlea) project. Bull Cancer. 2008;95:s73–s4.
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Ten Cate-Hoek AJ, Amin EE, Bouman AC, Meijer K, Tick LW, Middeldorp S, et al. Individualised versus standard duration of elastic compression therapy for prevention of post-thrombotic sydnrome (IDEAL DVT): a multicentre, randomised, single-blind, allocation-concealed, non-inferiority trial. The Lancet Haematology. 2018;5:e25–33.PubMedCrossRef Ten Cate-Hoek AJ, Amin EE, Bouman AC, Meijer K, Tick LW, Middeldorp S, et al. Individualised versus standard duration of elastic compression therapy for prevention of post-thrombotic sydnrome (IDEAL DVT): a multicentre, randomised, single-blind, allocation-concealed, non-inferiority trial. The Lancet Haematology. 2018;5:e25–33.PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Veek SMC, Derkx HHF, Benninga MA, Boer F, De Haan E. Cognitive behavior therapy for pediatric functional abdominal pain: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):e1163–e72.PubMedCrossRef Van der Veek SMC, Derkx HHF, Benninga MA, Boer F, De Haan E. Cognitive behavior therapy for pediatric functional abdominal pain: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):e1163–e72.PubMedCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Promoting positive parenting. An attachment-based intervention. Bornstein MH, editor. New York: Psychology Press; 2008. Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Promoting positive parenting. An attachment-based intervention. Bornstein MH, editor. New York: Psychology Press; 2008.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O'Hare E, Neuman G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2000;20(5):561–92.PubMedCrossRef Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O'Hare E, Neuman G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2000;20(5):561–92.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Boak R, Virgo-Milton M, Hoare A, de Silva A, Gibbs L, Gold L, et al. Choosing foods for infants: a qualitative study of the factors that influence mothers. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42(3):359–69.PubMedCrossRef Boak R, Virgo-Milton M, Hoare A, de Silva A, Gibbs L, Gold L, et al. Choosing foods for infants: a qualitative study of the factors that influence mothers. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42(3):359–69.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat McPhie S, Skouteris H, Daniels L, Jansen E. Maternal correlates of maternal child feeding practices: a systematic review. Maternal & Child Nutrition. 2014;10:18–43.CrossRef McPhie S, Skouteris H, Daniels L, Jansen E. Maternal correlates of maternal child feeding practices: a systematic review. Maternal & Child Nutrition. 2014;10:18–43.CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S. Complementary feeding strategies to facilitate acceptance of fruits and vegetables: A narrative review of the literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2016;13(1160).PubMedCentralCrossRef Nicklaus S. Complementary feeding strategies to facilitate acceptance of fruits and vegetables: A narrative review of the literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2016;13(1160).PubMedCentralCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG. Food neophobia and 'picky/fussy' eating in children: a review. Appetite. 2008;50(2–3):181–93.PubMedCrossRef Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG. Food neophobia and 'picky/fussy' eating in children: a review. Appetite. 2008;50(2–3):181–93.PubMedCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kok G, Schaalma H, Ruiter RA, Van Empelen P, Brug J. Intervention mapping: protocol for applying healthy psychology theory to prevention programmes. J Health Psychol. 2004;9(1):85–98.PubMedCrossRef Kok G, Schaalma H, Ruiter RA, Van Empelen P, Brug J. Intervention mapping: protocol for applying healthy psychology theory to prevention programmes. J Health Psychol. 2004;9(1):85–98.PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartholomew Eldregde LKB, Markham CM, Ruiter RA, Kok G, Parcel GS. Planning helath promotion programs: an intervention mapping approach. San Francisco: John Wiley & Sons; 2016. Bartholomew Eldregde LKB, Markham CM, Ruiter RA, Kok G, Parcel GS. Planning helath promotion programs: an intervention mapping approach. San Francisco: John Wiley & Sons; 2016.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Kok G, Bartholomew LK, Parcel GS, Gottlieb NH, Fernandez ME. Finding theory and evidence-based alternatives to fear appeals: intervention mapping. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):98–107.PubMedCrossRef Kok G, Bartholomew LK, Parcel GS, Gottlieb NH, Fernandez ME. Finding theory and evidence-based alternatives to fear appeals: intervention mapping. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):98–107.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Holley CE, Farrow C, Haycraft E. A systematic review of methods for increasing vegetable consumption in early childhood. Current nutrition reports. 2017;6(2):157–70.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Holley CE, Farrow C, Haycraft E. A systematic review of methods for increasing vegetable consumption in early childhood. Current nutrition reports. 2017;6(2):157–70.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Rollnick S, Miller WR, Butler CC. Motivational interviewing in health care: helping patients change behavior. New York: Guilford Publications; 2008. Rollnick S, Miller WR, Butler CC. Motivational interviewing in health care: helping patients change behavior. New York: Guilford Publications; 2008.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Prochaska JO, DiClemente C, Norcross J. In search of how people change: application to addictive behaviors. Am Psychol. 1992;47:1102–14.PubMedCrossRef Prochaska JO, DiClemente C, Norcross J. In search of how people change: application to addictive behaviors. Am Psychol. 1992;47:1102–14.PubMedCrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Pairing attachment theory and social learning theory in video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;15:189–94.PubMedCrossRef Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Pairing attachment theory and social learning theory in video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;15:189–94.PubMedCrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Conway JM, Ingwersen LA, Vinyard BT, Moshfegh AJ. Effectiveness of the US Department of Agriculture 5-step multiple-pass method in assessing food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;77:1171–8.PubMedCrossRef Conway JM, Ingwersen LA, Vinyard BT, Moshfegh AJ. Effectiveness of the US Department of Agriculture 5-step multiple-pass method in assessing food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;77:1171–8.PubMedCrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Meijboom S, Van Houts-Streppel MT, Perenboom C, Siebelink E, Van de Wiel AM, Geelen A, et al. Evaluation of dietary intake assessed by the Dutch self-adminstered web-based dietary 24-h recall tool (Compl-eattm) against interviewer-administered telephone-based 24-h recalls. Journal of Nutritional Science. 2017;19(6):e49.CrossRef Meijboom S, Van Houts-Streppel MT, Perenboom C, Siebelink E, Van de Wiel AM, Geelen A, et al. Evaluation of dietary intake assessed by the Dutch self-adminstered web-based dietary 24-h recall tool (Compl-eattm) against interviewer-administered telephone-based 24-h recalls. Journal of Nutritional Science. 2017;19(6):e49.CrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Llewellyn CH, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Johnson L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Development and factor structure of the baby eating behaviour questionnaire in the Gemini birth cohort. Appetite. 2011;57(2):388–96.PubMedCrossRef Llewellyn CH, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Johnson L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Development and factor structure of the baby eating behaviour questionnaire in the Gemini birth cohort. Appetite. 2011;57(2):388–96.PubMedCrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Herle M, Fildes A, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Rijsdijk F, Llewellyn CH. Parental reports of infant and child eating behaviors are not affected by their beliefs about their twins' zygosity. Behavioral Genetics. 2016;46:763–71.CrossRef Herle M, Fildes A, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Rijsdijk F, Llewellyn CH. Parental reports of infant and child eating behaviors are not affected by their beliefs about their twins' zygosity. Behavioral Genetics. 2016;46:763–71.CrossRef
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Boggio V, Nicklaus S. Impact of adiposity, age, sex and maternal feeding practices on eating in the absence of hunger and caloric compensation in preschool children. Int J Obes. 2015;39(6):925–30.CrossRef Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Boggio V, Nicklaus S. Impact of adiposity, age, sex and maternal feeding practices on eating in the absence of hunger and caloric compensation in preschool children. Int J Obes. 2015;39(6):925–30.CrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Lansigan RK, Emond JA, Gilbert-Diamond D. Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: a systematic review of existing studies. Appetite. 2015;85:36–47.PubMedCrossRef Lansigan RK, Emond JA, Gilbert-Diamond D. Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: a systematic review of existing studies. Appetite. 2015;85:36–47.PubMedCrossRef
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher JO, Birch LL. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 years of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76(1):226–31.PubMedCrossRef Fisher JO, Birch LL. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 years of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76(1):226–31.PubMedCrossRef
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher JO, Birch LL. Restricting access to foods and children's eating. Appetite. 1999;32(3):405–19.PubMedCrossRef Fisher JO, Birch LL. Restricting access to foods and children's eating. Appetite. 1999;32(3):405–19.PubMedCrossRef
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(7).PubMedCrossRef Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(7).PubMedCrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Sleddens EFC, Kremers SPJ, Thijs C. The Children’s eating behaviour questionnaire: factorial validity and association with body mass index in Dutch children aged 6-7. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5.CrossRef Sleddens EFC, Kremers SPJ, Thijs C. The Children’s eating behaviour questionnaire: factorial validity and association with body mass index in Dutch children aged 6-7. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5.CrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Infant-mother attachment and social development: socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In: Richards MPM, editor. The integration of a child into a social world. London: Cambridge University Press; 1974. p. 9–135. Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Infant-mother attachment and social development: socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In: Richards MPM, editor. The integration of a child into a social world. London: Cambridge University Press; 1974. p. 9–135.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Hodges EA, Johnson SO, Hopkinson JM, Butte NF, Fisher JO. Development of the responsiveness to child feeding cues scale. Appetite. 2013;65:210–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hodges EA, Johnson SO, Hopkinson JM, Butte NF, Fisher JO. Development of the responsiveness to child feeding cues scale. Appetite. 2013;65:210–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson AL, Mendez MA, Borja JB, Adair LS, Zimmer CR, Bentley ME. Development and validation of the infant feeding style questionnaire. Appetite. 2009;53:210–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thompson AL, Mendez MA, Borja JB, Adair LS, Zimmer CR, Bentley ME. Development and validation of the infant feeding style questionnaire. Appetite. 2009;53:210–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(8):960–72.PubMedCrossRef Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(8):960–72.PubMedCrossRef
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Gubbels JS, Sleddens EFC, Raaijmakers LCH, Gies JM, Kremers SPJ. The child-care food and activity practices questionnaire (CFAPQ): development and first validation steps. Public Health Nutr. 2015;19(11):1964–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gubbels JS, Sleddens EFC, Raaijmakers LCH, Gies JM, Kremers SPJ. The child-care food and activity practices questionnaire (CFAPQ): development and first validation steps. Public Health Nutr. 2015;19(11):1964–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Jansen E, Mallan KM, Nicholson JM, Daniels LA. The feeding practices and structure questionnaire: construction and initial validation in a sample of Australian first-time mothers and their 2-year olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(72):1–13. Jansen E, Mallan KM, Nicholson JM, Daniels LA. The feeding practices and structure questionnaire: construction and initial validation in a sample of Australian first-time mothers and their 2-year olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(72):1–13.
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurements. 1977;1:385–401.CrossRef Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurements. 1977;1:385–401.CrossRef
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Siebelink E, Geelen A, De Vries JH. Self-reported energy intake by FFQ compared with actual energy intake to maintain body weight in 516 adults. Br J Nutr. 2011;106(2):274–81.PubMedCrossRef Siebelink E, Geelen A, De Vries JH. Self-reported energy intake by FFQ compared with actual energy intake to maintain body weight in 516 adults. Br J Nutr. 2011;106(2):274–81.PubMedCrossRef
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan EA. Exploring infant feeding practices: maternal self-efficacy as a predictor of controlling feeding (doctoral dissertation). Ann Arbor: ProQuest LLC; 2013. Dolan EA. Exploring infant feeding practices: maternal self-efficacy as a predictor of controlling feeding (doctoral dissertation). Ann Arbor: ProQuest LLC; 2013.
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson SE, Must A, Curtin C, Bandini LG. Meals in our household: reliability and initial validation of a questionnaire to assess child mealtime behaviors and family mealtime environments. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(2):276–84.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Anderson SE, Must A, Curtin C, Bandini LG. Meals in our household: reliability and initial validation of a questionnaire to assess child mealtime behaviors and family mealtime environments. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(2):276–84.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Gottschalk LA, Gleser GC. The measurement of psychological states through the content analysis of verbal behavior. Berkely: University of California Press; 1969. Gottschalk LA, Gleser GC. The measurement of psychological states through the content analysis of verbal behavior. Berkely: University of California Press; 1969.
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E. Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. J Pers Assess. 2014;96:445–58.PubMedCrossRef Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E. Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. J Pers Assess. 2014;96:445–58.PubMedCrossRef
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29(3):386–401.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29(3):386–401.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Sleddens EFC, O'Connor TM, Watson KB, Hughes SO, Power TG, Thijs C, et al. Development of the comprehensive general parenting questionnaire for caregivers of 5-13 year olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(15):1–14. Sleddens EFC, O'Connor TM, Watson KB, Hughes SO, Power TG, Thijs C, et al. Development of the comprehensive general parenting questionnaire for caregivers of 5-13 year olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(15):1–14.
110.
Zurück zum Zitat Michaelsen KF. Feeding and nutrition of infants and young children: guidelines for the WHO European region, with emphasis on the former soviet countries: WHO regional office Europe; 2000. Michaelsen KF. Feeding and nutrition of infants and young children: guidelines for the WHO European region, with emphasis on the former soviet countries: WHO regional office Europe; 2000.
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Fewtrell M, Bronsky J, Campoy C, Domellof M, Embleton N, Mis NF, et al. Complementary feeding: a position paper by the European society for paediatric gastroenterology, hepatology, and nutrition (ESPGHAN) committee on nutrition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2014;64:119–32.CrossRef Fewtrell M, Bronsky J, Campoy C, Domellof M, Embleton N, Mis NF, et al. Complementary feeding: a position paper by the European society for paediatric gastroenterology, hepatology, and nutrition (ESPGHAN) committee on nutrition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2014;64:119–32.CrossRef
112.
Zurück zum Zitat EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, N., & Allergies. Scientific Opinion on the appropriate age for introduction of complementary feeding of infants. EFSA Journal. 2009;7:1423.CrossRef EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, N., & Allergies. Scientific Opinion on the appropriate age for introduction of complementary feeding of infants. EFSA Journal. 2009;7:1423.CrossRef
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Abrams EM, Greenhawt M, Herscher DM, Chand ES. Early solid food introduction: role in food allergy prevention and implications for breastfeeding. J Pediatr. 2017;184:13–8.PubMedCrossRef Abrams EM, Greenhawt M, Herscher DM, Chand ES. Early solid food introduction: role in food allergy prevention and implications for breastfeeding. J Pediatr. 2017;184:13–8.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Baby’s first bites: a randomized controlled trial to assess the effects of vegetable-exposure and sensitive feeding on vegetable acceptance, eating behavior and weight gain in infants and toddlers
verfasst von
S. M. C. van der Veek
C. de Graaf
J. H. M. de Vries
G. Jager
C. M. J. L. Vereijken
H. Weenen
N. van Winden
M. S. van Vliet
J. M. Schultink
V. W. T. de Wild
S. Janssen
J. Mesman
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1627-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Pediatrics 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Ähnliche Überlebensraten nach Reanimation während des Transports bzw. vor Ort

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada scheint es bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Bei Amblyopie früher abkleben als bisher empfohlen?

22.05.2024 Fehlsichtigkeit Nachrichten

Bei Amblyopie ist das frühzeitige Abkleben des kontralateralen Auges in den meisten Fällen wohl effektiver als der Therapiestandard mit zunächst mehrmonatigem Brilletragen.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.